Misplaced Pages

Meme: Difference between revisions

Article snapshot taken from Wikipedia with creative commons attribution-sharealike license. Give it a read and then ask your questions in the chat. We can research this topic together.
Browse history interactively← Previous editContent deleted Content addedVisualWikitext
Revision as of 23:40, 6 July 2005 view sourceCesarB (talk | contribs)Administrators14,429 editsm Reverted edits by 211.30.190.44 to last version by CesarB← Previous edit Latest revision as of 08:03, 6 January 2025 view source OAbot (talk | contribs)Bots441,761 editsm Open access bot: hdl updated in citation with #oabot. 
Line 1: Line 1:
{{Short description|Cultural idea which spreads through imitation}}
The term and concept of '''meme''' (] {{IPA|}} in ]; from the ] word ''μνήμη'' for ']') first appeared in the ] book by ], '']''. Though Dawkins defined the meme as "a unit of cultural transmission, or a unit of imitation," memeticists vary in their definitions of ''meme''. The lack of a consistent, rigorous and precise definition of a meme remains one of the principal criticisms leveled at '']'', the study of memes.
{{About||the usage of the term on the Internet|Internet meme|other uses||}}
{{pp-vandalism|small=yes}}
{{use dmy dates|cs1-dates=l|date=August 2020}}
{{anthropology|concepts}}
A '''meme''' ({{IPAc-en|m|iː|m|audio=en-us-meme.ogg}}; {{respell|MEEM}})<ref>{{cite web |title=meme |url=https://en.oxforddictionaries.com/definition/meme |url-status=dead |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20190523192242/https://en.oxforddictionaries.com/definition/meme |archive-date=23 May 2019 |access-date=30 December 2017 |website=Oxford Dictionaries}}</ref><ref>{{cite web |date=2023 |title=Meme |url=https://dictionary.cambridge.org/dictionary/english/meme |url-status=live |access-date=8 October 2023 |website=Cambridge Dictionary |archive-date=18 March 2021 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20210318030401/https://dictionary.cambridge.org/dictionary/english/meme}}</ref><ref>{{cite web |title=meme ''noun'' |url=https://www.oxfordlearnersdictionaries.com/definition/english/meme?q=meme |url-status=dead |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20190520120515/https://www.oxfordlearnersdictionaries.com/definition/english/meme?q=meme |date=2019 |archive-date=20 May 2019 |access-date=30 December 2017 |website=Oxford Learner's Dictionaries}}</ref> is an idea, behavior, or style that spreads by means of imitation from person to person within a culture and often carries symbolic meaning representing a particular phenomenon or theme.<ref> {{Webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20180921183926/http://www.merriam-webster.com/dictionary/meme |date=21 September 2018}}. ''Merriam-Webster Dictionary''.</ref> A meme acts as a unit for carrying ] ideas, symbols, or practices, that can be transmitted from one mind to another through writing, speech, gestures, rituals, or other imitable phenomena with a mimicked theme. Supporters of the concept regard memes as cultural analogues to ]s in that they ], mutate, and respond to ].<ref>{{harvnb|Graham|2002}}</ref> In popular language, a meme may refer to an ], typically an image, that is remixed, copied, and circulated in a shared cultural experience online.<ref name=":0">{{Cite book |last=Shifman |first=Limor |url=https://www.worldcat.org/oclc/860711989 |title=Memes in Digital Culture |date=2014 |isbn=9781461947332 |publisher=] |location=Cambridge, Massachusetts |oclc=860711989 |access-date=20 June 2022 |archive-date=22 June 2022 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20220622003628/https://www.worldcat.org/title/memes-in-digital-culture/oclc/860711989 |url-status=live}}</ref><ref>{{cite book |last=Miltner |first=Kate M. |contribution=Internet Memes |date=2018 |url=https://sk.sagepub.com/reference/the-sage-handbook-of-social-media/i3302.xml |title=The Sage Handbook of Social Media |pages=412–428 |publisher=Sage Publications |doi=10.4135/9781473984066.n23 |isbn=9781412962292 |access-date=20 June 2022 |archive-date=20 June 2022 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20220620055956/https://sk.sagepub.com/reference/the-sage-handbook-of-social-media/i3302.xml |url-status=live}}</ref>


Proponents theorize that memes are a ] that may evolve by natural selection in a manner analogous to that of ].<ref name="The Selfish Gene 30th Anniversary Edition section on survival">{{cite book |last1=Dawkins |first1=Richard |url=https://archive.org/details/selfishgene00dawk_669 |title=The Selfish Gene 30th Anniversary Edition |publisher=] |date=2006 |isbn=9780191537554 |edition=3rd |page= |url-access=limited}}</ref> Memes do this through processes analogous to those of ], ], ], and ], each of which influences a meme's reproductive success. Memes spread through the behavior that they generate in their hosts. Memes that ] less prolifically may become ], while others may survive, spread, and (for better or for worse) mutate. Memes that replicate most effectively enjoy more success, and some may replicate effectively even when they prove to be detrimental to the welfare of their hosts.<ref name="Kelly">{{harvnb|Kelly|1994|p=360}} "But if we consider culture as its own self-organizing system—a system with its own agenda and pressure to survive—then the history of humanity gets even more interesting. As Richard Dawkins has shown, systems of self-replicating ideas or memes can quickly accumulate their own agenda and behaviours. I assign no higher motive to a cultural entity than the primitive drive to reproduce itself and modify its environment to aid its spread. One way the self organizing system can do this is by consuming human biological resources."</ref>
Different definitions of the meme generally agree, very roughly, that a meme consists of some sort of a self-propagating unit of ] having a resemblance to the ] (the unit of ]). Dawkins introduced the term after writing that evolution depended not on the particular chemical basis of genetics, but only on the existence of a self-replicating unit of transmission&mdash;in the case of evolution, the gene. For Dawkins, the meme exemplifies another self-replicating unit, and most importantly, one which he thought would prove useful in explaining human behavior and cultural evolution.


A field of study called '']''<ref>{{harvnb |Heylighen|Chielens|2009}}</ref> arose in the 1990s to explore the concepts and transmission of memes in terms of an ]. Criticism from a variety of fronts has challenged the notion that academic study can examine memes ]ly. However, developments in ] may make empirical study possible.<ref name="mcnamara">{{harvnb|McNamara|2011}}</ref> Some commentators in the ] question the idea that one can meaningfully categorize culture in terms of discrete units, and are especially critical of the biological nature of the theory's underpinnings.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Gill |first=Jameson |date=2011 |title=Memes and narrative analysis: A potential direction for the development of neo-Darwinian orientated research in organisations. |url=http://shura.shu.ac.uk/4241/1/Memes_and_Narrative%5B1%5D.pdf |journal=EURAM 11: Proceedings of the European Academy of Management |publisher=] |pages=0–30 |issn=2466-7498 |access-date=5 April 2022 |s2cid=54894144 |archive-date=10 October 2021 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20211010093736/http://shura.shu.ac.uk/4241/1/Memes_and_Narrative%5B1%5D.pdf |url-status=live}}</ref> Others have argued that this use of the term is the result of a misunderstanding of the original proposal.<ref name="misunderstanding">{{cite journal |last1=Burman |first1=J. T.|date=2012 |title=The misunderstanding of memes: Biography of an unscientific object, 1976–1999 |journal=] |volume=20 |issue=1 |pages=75–104 |doi=10.1162/POSC_a_00057 |s2cid=57569644 |doi-access=free | issn = 1063-6145 }}</ref>
] exemplifies what some might consider a visual meme. Anyone who has seen a smiley can copy, reproduce, or modify it and then show it to others.]]


The word ''meme'' itself is a ] coined by ], originating from his 1976 book '']''.<ref name="cream">{{harvnb|Dawkins|1989|p=}} "We need a name for the new replicator, a noun that conveys the idea of a unit of cultural transmission, or a unit of ''imitation''. 'Mimeme' comes from a suitable Greek root, but I want a monosyllable that sounds a bit like 'gene'. I hope my classicist friends will forgive me if I abbreviate mimeme to ''meme''. If it is any consolation, it could alternatively be thought of as being related to 'memory', or to the French word ''même''. It should be pronounced to rhyme with 'cream'."</ref> Dawkins's own position is somewhat ambiguous. He welcomed ]'s suggestion that "memes should be considered as living structures, not just metaphorically",<ref name="cream" /> and proposed to regard memes as "physically residing in the brain".<ref name="TEP">{{cite book |last=Dawkins |first=Richard |author-link=Richard Dawkins |title=The Extended Phenotype |publisher=] |date=1982 |isbn=9780192860880 |page=109}}</ref> Although Dawkins said his original intentions had been simpler, he approved Humphrey's opinion and he endorsed ]'s ] to give a scientific theory of memes, complete with predictions and empirical support.<ref>Dawkins's foreword to {{harvnb|Blackmore|1999}}, p. xvi–xvii</ref>
In casual use, the term ''meme'' often refers to any piece of information passed from one mind to another. This usage more closely resembles the analogy of "] as a ]" than Dawkins' analogy of memes as replicating units. This definition has come into popular use on the ] to refer to ] such as ], "]", ] and ].


==Etymology==
<!--
I've commented out the following paragraph because it doesn't seem to fit here. Is there a better place? It also seems to assume a particular definition of a meme, which seems perhaps like a bad idea. --wpegden
Some memes, such as many on the Internet, tend to proliferate for periods of time then quietly die off: many start as obscure ] within net ]s, which gradually lose their original meaning or become otherwise detached. Some people consider ] humor as a good source of memes. Generally, the better the communication medium, the faster memes can come into and out of ]. -->


The term ''meme'' is a shortening (modeled on ''gene'') of ''mimeme'', which comes from ] {{lang|grc-Latn|mīmēma}} ({{lang|grc|μίμημα}}; {{IPA-el|míːmɛːma|pron}}), meaning 'imitated thing', itself from {{lang|grc-Latn|mimeisthai}} ({{lang|grc|μιμεῖσθαι}}, 'to imitate'), from {{lang|grc-Latn|mimos}} ({{Lang|grc|μῖμος}}, 'mime').<ref>{{cite book |chapter=meme |title=The American Heritage Dictionary of the English Language |edition=4th |date=2000}}</ref><ref>{{OEtymD|meme}}</ref><ref>{{LSJ|mi/mhma|μίμημα}}, {{LSJ|mime/omai|μιμεῖσθαι}},{{LSJ|mi{{=}}mos|μῖμος|ref}}.</ref>
==Basic introduction==
<!-- This section aims to give a sharp and solid basic understanding of the meme concept to the new reader, thus contributors and editors should express themselves in SHARPEST manner using the MINIMUM POSSIBLE words. -->


The word was coined by British evolutionary biologist ] in '']'' (1976) as a concept for discussion of ]ary principles in explaining the spread of ideas and cultural phenomena.<ref name="cream" /><ref>{{harvnb|Millikan|2004|p=}}. "Richard Dawkins invented the term 'memes' to stand for items that are reproduced by imitation rather than reproduced genetically."</ref> Examples of memes given in Dawkins' book include ], ]s, fashion, and the technology of building arches.<ref name="selfish">{{harvnb|Dawkins|1989|p=352}}</ref>
Though ] do not generally agree on a specific definition, one can roughly define 'meme' as any piece of information transferable from one mind to another. Examples might include thoughts, ideas, theories, practices, habits, songs, dances and moods.


== Origins ==
Memes supposedly have, as their fundamental property, ] via ] in a way very similar to ]'s ideas concerning ] evolution, on the premise that replication, ], survival and competition influence them. For example, while one idea may become extinct, others will survive, spread and mutate&mdash;for better or worse&mdash;through modification. Note an important fact, however: not only the memes most beneficial to their hosts will necessarily survive; rather, memes supposedly spread best by functioning as the most effective replicators, which allows for the possibility that successful memes might prove detrimental to their hosts.


] coined the word ''meme'' in his 1976 book '']''.]]
<!-- I'm commenting out this next block because, apart from being blatantly one sided on its view of memetics (nearly all of these claims would be contested by many scientists), this stuff is surely better suited for the article on memetics.


=== Early formulations ===
The conception and study of memes, known as ], has led to new insights in:
Although Richard Dawkins invented the term ''meme'' and developed meme theory, he has not claimed that the idea was entirely novel,<ref>{{cite web|last=Shalizi|first=Cosma Rohilla|title=Memes |url=http://www.cscs.umich.edu/~crshalizi/formerly-hyper-weird/memetics.html |access-date=8 October 2021 |website=Center for the Study of Complex Systems |publisher=]|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20120611125712/http://www.cscs.umich.edu/~crshalizi/formerly-hyper-weird/memetics.html |archive-date=11 June 2012 |url-status=dead}}</ref> and there have been other expressions for similar ideas in the past.<ref name="mneme">{{Cite journal |last=Laurent |first=John |url=http://cfpm.org/jom-emit/1999/vol3/laurent_j.html |title=A Note on the Origin of 'Memes'/'Mnemes' |journal=Journal of Memetics |date=1999 |volume=3 |pages=14–19 |issue=1 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20210413222038/http://cfpm.org/jom-emit/1999/vol3/laurent_j.html|archive-date=13 April 2021}}</ref>
#general ] operation
#]


For instance, the possibility that ideas were subject to the same pressures of evolution as were biological attributes was discussed in the time of Charles Darwin. ] (1880) claimed that "The struggle for existence holds as much in the intellectual as in the physical world. A theory is a species of thinking, and its right to exist is coextensive with its power of resisting extinction by its rivals."<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Huxley |first=T. H. |date=1880 |title=The coming of age of 'The origin of species' |journal=Science |volume=1 |issue=2 |pages=15–20 |doi=10.1126/science.os-1.2.15 |pmid=17751948|s2cid=4061790}}</ref> ] (1922) observed the similarity between intellectual systems and living organisms, noting that a certain degree of ], rather than being a hindrance, is a necessity for continued survival.<ref>{{cite book |last=Chesterton |first=G. K. |author-link=G. K. Chesterton |date= 1990| orig-date=1922 |chapter=III. The Case for Complexity |editor-last1=Marlin |editor-first1=R. P. |editor-last2=Rabatin |editor-first2=G. J. |editor-last3=Swan |editor-first3=J. L. |editor-last4=Sobran |editor-first4=J. |editor-last5=Azar |editor-first5=P. |editor-last6=Mysak |editor-first6=J. |editor-last7=Paine |editor-first7=R. |editor-last8=Marlin |editor-first8=B. D. |title=The Collected Works of G. K. Chesterton|volume=III|url= |location=San Francisco|publisher=Ignatius Press|pages= 37–40|isbn=0-89870-310-7}}</ref>
Memetics and the introduction of the meme as a concept build on several previous fundamental scientific discoveries:
*Evolutionary theory (by Darwin)
*Recognition of ] as an information sequence
*Recognition of the fact that the primary objects of evolution are information sequences in ]


In 1904, ] published ''Die Mneme'' (which appeared in English in 1924 as ''The Mneme''). The term ''mneme'' was also used in ]'s ''The Life of the White Ant'' (1926), with some parallels to Dawkins's concept.<ref name="mneme" /> ] had, in 1954, coined the related terms ], generalizing the linguistic units of ], ], ], ], and ] (as set out by ]), distinguishing insider and outside views of communicative behavior.<ref>{{cite book |first=Kenneth |last=Pike |title=Language in Relation to a Unified Theory of the Structure of Human Behavior |orig-year=1954 |edition=Revised |date=1967}}</ref>
Memeticists may regard meme evolution as a new level of biological evolution, whereby new ideas evolve in seconds rather than over generations (as in biological evolution); this may explain the rapid progress of '']''.


=== Dawkins ===
Memetics may lead to a new level of understanding of meta-science and common ] practices, such as scientific approach, ] and ].
The word ''meme'' originated with ]' 1976 book '']''.


Dawkins cites as inspiration the work of geneticist ], anthropologist F. T. Cloak,<ref>Cloak, F. T. 1966. "Cultural microevolution". ''Research Previews'' 13(2): 7–10. (Also presented at the November, 1966 annual meeting of the American Anthropological Association.)</ref><ref>Cloak, F. T. 1975. "Is a cultural ethology possible?" ] 3: 161–182. {{doi|10.1007/BF01531639}}.</ref> and ethologist J. M. Cullen.<ref>" {{Webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20211114235259/https://www.litcharts.com/lit/the-selfish-gene/chapter-11-memes-the-new-replicators |date=14 November 2021}}". ''LitCharts''.</ref> Dawkins wrote that evolution depended not on the particular chemical basis of genetics, but only on the existence of a self-replicating unit of transmission—in the case of biological evolution, the gene. For Dawkins, the meme exemplified another self-replicating unit with potential significance in explaining human behavior and cultural evolution.
Memetics provides another level of understanding of ]s, such as ]s, ]s and ].


]" was a ] that became popular in the 1940s, and existed under various names in different countries, illustrating how a meme can be modified through replication. This is seen as one of the first widespread memes in the world.<ref>{{cite web |last=Gardner |first=Martin |url=https://www.latimes.com/archives/la-xpm-2000-mar-05-bk-5402-story.html |title=Kilroy Was Here |work=] |date=5 March 2000 |access-date=8 October 2021 |archive-date=11 October 2021 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20211011161432/https://www.latimes.com/archives/la-xpm-2000-mar-05-bk-5402-story.html |url-status=live}}</ref>]]
Memetics can serve as a bridge between ] and biology (''See also'' ]s, ]).
Dawkins used the term to refer to any cultural entity that an observer might consider a ]. He hypothesized that one could view many cultural entities as replicators, and pointed to melodies, fashions and learned skills as examples. Memes generally replicate through exposure to humans, who have evolved as efficient copiers of information and behavior. Because humans do not always copy memes perfectly, and because they may refine, combine or otherwise modify them with other memes to create new memes, they can change over time. Dawkins likened the process by which memes survive and change through the ] to the natural selection of genes in biological ].<ref name="selfish"/>
-->
Dawkins noted that in a society with culture a person need not have biological descendants to remain influential in the actions of individuals thousands of years after their death:<blockquote>But if you contribute to the world's culture, if you have a good idea...it may live on, intact, long after your genes have dissolved in the common pool. ] may or may not have a gene or two alive in the world today, as ] has remarked, but who cares? The meme-complexes of Socrates, ], ] and ] are still going strong.<ref name="The Selfish Gene 30th Anniversary Edition section on survival" /></blockquote>


In that context, Dawkins defined the ''meme'' as a unit of ], or a unit of imitation and replication, but later definitions would vary. The lack of a consistent, rigorous, and precise understanding of what typically makes up one unit of cultural transmission remains a problem in debates about ].<ref name="machine">{{harvnb|Blackmore|1999}}</ref> In contrast, the concept of genetics gained concrete evidence with the ] of the ] of ]. Meme transmission requires a physical medium, such as photons, sound waves, touch, taste, or smell because memes can be transmitted only through the senses.
==History of the meme concept==
The concept of ideas that spread according to genetic rules predates the coining by Richard Dawkins in ''The Selfish Gene''; for example ] asserted that "anguage is a virus."


=== After Dawkins: Role of physical media ===
] in ''The Journal of Memetics'' has suggested that the term 'meme' itself may have derived from the work of the little-known ] biologist ]. In ] Semon published ''Die Mneme'' (published in English as ''The Mneme'' in ]). His book discussed the cultural transmission of experiences with insights parallel to those of Dawkins. Laurent found the use of the term ''mneme'' in ''The Soul of the White Ant'' (1927) by ] and highlights its parallels to Dawkins's concept:
Initially, Dawkins did not seriously give context to the material of memetics. He considered a meme to be an idea, and thus a mental concept. However, from Dawkins' initial conception, it is how a medium might function in relation to the meme which has garnered the most attention. For example, ] suggested that while memes might exist as Dawkins conceives of them, he finds it important to suggest that instead of determining them as idea "replicators" (i.e. mind-determinant influences) one might notice that the medium itself has an influence in the meme's evolutionary outcomes.<ref>{{Cite book |last=Hull |first=David L. |title=Darwinizing Culture: The Status of Memetics as a Science |publisher=Oxford University Press |date=2001 |isbn=9780192632449 |editor-last=Aunger |editor-first=Robert |edition=1st |pages=43–67 |chapter=Taking memetics seriously: Memetics will be what we make it}}</ref> Thus, he refers to the medium as an "interactor" to avoid this determinism. Alternatively, ] suggests that the medium and the idea are not distinct in that memes only exist because of their medium.<ref>{{cite book |last=Dennett |first=Daniel C. |title=From Bacteria to Bach and Back: The Evolution of Minds |date=2017 |url=http://worldcat.org/oclc/957746925 |publisher=HighBridge Audio |isbn=9781681684390 |oclc=957746925 |access-date=11 January 2023}}</ref> Dennett argued this in order to remain consistent with his denial of ] and the notion of materially deterministic evolution which was consistent with Dawkins' account. A particularly more divergent theory is that of ], a communication and media scholar of "]tics". She argues that any memetic argument which claims the distinction between the meme and the meme-vehicle (i.e. the meme's medium) are empirically observable is mistaken from the offset.<ref>{{cite book |last=Shifman |first=Limor |title=Memes in Digital Culture |date=2014 |publisher=MIT Press |isbn=9781469063256 |oclc=929971523}}</ref> Shifman claims to be following a similar theoretical direction as ]; however, her attention to the media surrounding Internet culture has enabled Internet memetic research to depart in empirical interests from previous memetic goals.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Lankshear |first1=Colin |last2=Knobel |first2=Michele |date=2019 |title=Memes, Macros, Meaning, and Menace: Some Trends in Internet Memes |url=https://cgscholar.com/bookstore/works/memes-macros-meaning-and-menace |journal=The Journal of Communication and Media Studies |volume=4 |issue=4 |pages=43–57 |doi=10.18848/2470-9247/CGP/v04i04/43-57 |s2cid=214369629 |issn=2470-9247 |access-date=11 January 2023 |archive-date=4 January 2023 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20230104172842/https://cgscholar.com/bookstore/works/memes-macros-meaning-and-menace |url-status=live}}</ref> Regardless of Internet Memetic's divergence in theoretical interests, it plays a significant role in theorizing and empirically investigating the connection between cultural ideologies, behaviors, and their mediation processes.
:Maeterlinck, in discussing theories which attempt to explain 'memory' in termites as well as the other 'social' insects (ants, bees etc.), uses the phrase "engrammata upon the individual mneme" (Maeterlinck, 1927, p.198). Webster's Collegiate dictionary defines an engram as "a memory trace; specif.: a protoplasmic change in neural tissue hypothesized to account for persistence of memory". Note that Maeterlinck explains that he obtained his phrase from the "German philosopher" Richard Semon.


== Memetic lifecycle: transmission, retention ==
Laurent suggests that the etymological roots of the term 'meme' may come from ''mimneskesthai'', the Greek term for 'memory,' rather than from Dawkins's root of ''mimeisthai'', "to imitate."
{{See also|Diffusion of innovations}}<!-- Similar process, although memes are not necessarily "innovations". Sections "Process" and "Rate of Adoption". -->
] album '']'' (1969), on which the band members cross the road in front of the ] in a row, has become popular with fans and ] visitors.]]
] '']'' reenact the Beatles cover in 2010, extending the original Beatles meme by their film costumes.]]
]s imitate the above meme during the manga convention Paris Manga 2012 at a zebra crossing in Paris, thus further separating the meme from the root situation of 1969 tied to the Abbey Road zebra crossing.]]
Memes, analogously to genes, vary in their aptitude to replicate; successful memes remain and spread, whereas unfit ones stall and are forgotten. Thus, memes that prove more effective at replicating and surviving are selected in the meme pool.{{citation needed|date=February 2023}}


Memes first need retention. The longer a meme stays in its hosts, the higher its chances of propagation are. When a host uses a meme, the meme's life is extended.<ref>{{cite web |last=Heylighen |first=Francis |title=Meme replication: The memetic life-cycle |url=http://pespmc1.vub.ac.be/MEMEREP.html |website=Principia Cybernetica |access-date=26 July 2013 |archive-date=4 October 2018 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20181004223220/http://pespmc1.vub.ac.be/MEMEREP.html |url-status=live}}</ref> The reuse of the neural space hosting a certain meme's copy to host different memes is the greatest threat to that meme's copy.<ref>{{cite web |last=R. Evers |first=John |title=A justification of societal altruism according to the memetic application of Hamilton's rule |url=http://pespmc1.vub.ac.be/Conf/MemePap/Evers.html |access-date=26 July 2013 |archive-date=6 October 2018 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20181006162715/http://pespmc1.vub.ac.be/Conf/MemePap/Evers.html |url-status=live}}</ref> A meme that increases the longevity of its hosts will generally survive longer. On the contrary, a meme that shortens the longevity of its hosts will tend to disappear faster. However, as hosts are mortal, retention is not sufficient to perpetuate a meme in the long term; memes also need transmission.
] pioneered the "]" theory (formalised in ]) which explains how and why people adopt new ideas. Rogers reflected some of the influence of ], who set out "laws of imitation" in his book of ] that explained how people decided whether to imitate behavior. ] of the ] has come up with what he called ]. These criteria opened the way to a specialized field of applied memetics to find out if these selection criteria could stand the test of quantitative analyses. In ] Klaas Chielens carried out these tests in a Masters thesis project on the testability of the selection criteria.


Life-forms can transmit information both vertically (from parent to child, via replication of genes) and horizontally (through viruses and other means).
==Memetics==
Memes can replicate vertically or horizontally within a single biological generation. They may also lie dormant for long periods of time.
{{seemain|Memetics}}


Memes reproduce by copying from a nervous system to another one, either by communication or ]. Imitation often involves the copying of an ] behavior of another individual. Communication may be direct or indirect, where memes transmit from one individual to another through a copy recorded in an inanimate source, such as a book or a ]. Adam McNamara has suggested that memes can be thereby classified as either internal or external memes (i-memes or e-memes).<ref name="mcnamara" />
Memetics, the study of memes, remains a controversial field among many scientists and skeptics. Memetics originated when Richard Dawkins reduced the process of biological genetic evolution to its most fundamental unit: the replicator (or gene). Dawkins, in a search for parallels and other things that he might classify as replicators, suggested that the information and ideas in brains&mdash;], for example&mdash;could function as replicators as well. ] may represent another form of replicator with which evolution may eventually build grand things, whether socially as in the ], or through the use of ] .


Some commentators have likened the transmission of memes to the spread of ].<ref>{{harvnb|Blackmore|1998}}; "The term 'contagion' is often associated with memetics. We may say that certain memes are contagious, or more contagious than others."</ref> Social contagions such as ], ], ], and ] exemplify memes seen as the contagious imitation of ideas. Observers distinguish the contagious imitation of memes from instinctively contagious phenomena such as yawning and laughing, which they consider innate (rather than socially learned) behaviors.<ref name="defmeme">{{harvnb|Blackmore|1998}}
Memetics takes concepts from the theory of evolution (especially ]) and applies them to human culture. Memetics also uses ]s to try to explain many very controversial subjects such as religion and political systems, though principal criticisms of memetics include the claim that memetics ignores established advances in the fields (such as ], ], ], etc.) most relevant to the claims and methodologies of memetics.
</ref>


] described seven general patterns of meme transmission, or "thought contagion":<ref name="lynch">{{harvnb|Lynch|1996}}</ref>
The term ''memetic association'' refers to the idea that memes ]. For example, a meme for bluejeans includes memes for trouser flies, riveted clothing, blue dye, cotton clothing, belt loops and double-sewn seams. In this way, groups of memes can operate symbiotically (to use a biological analogy) in the sense that they act for their mutual benefit.


# Quantity of parenthood: an idea that influences the number of children one has. Children respond particularly receptively to the ideas of their parents, and thus ideas that directly or indirectly encourage a higher birth rate will replicate themselves at a higher rate than those that discourage higher birth rates.
The phrase ''memetic drift'' refers to the process of an idea or meme changing as it replicates between one person to another. Memetic drift increases when meme transmission occurs in an awkward way. Very few memes show strong ''memetic inertia'' (the characteristic of a meme to manifest in the same way and to have the same impact regardless of who receives or transmits the meme). Memetic inertia increases when the meme transfers along with mnemonic devices, such as a rhyme, to preserve the memory of the meme prior to its transmission. See ] for one example of memetic drift.
# Efficiency of parenthood: an idea that increases the proportion of children who will adopt ideas of their parents. Cultural ] exemplifies one practice in which one can expect a higher rate of meme-replication—because the meme for separation creates a barrier from exposure to competing ideas.
# Proselytic: ideas generally passed to others beyond one's own children. Ideas that encourage the ] of a meme, as seen in many religious or political movements, can replicate memes horizontally through a given generation, spreading more rapidly than parent-to-child meme-transmissions do.
# Preservational: ideas that influence those that hold them to continue to hold them for a long time. Ideas that encourage longevity in their hosts, or leave their hosts particularly resistant to abandoning or replacing these ideas, enhance the preservability of memes and afford protection from the competition or proselytism of other memes.
# Adversative: ideas that influence those that hold them to attack or sabotage competing ideas and/or those that hold them. Adversative replication can give an advantage in meme transmission when the meme itself encourages aggression against other memes.
# Cognitive: ideas perceived as cogent by most in the population who encounter them. Cognitively transmitted memes depend heavily on a cluster of other ideas and cognitive traits already widely held in the population, and thus usually spread more passively than other forms of meme transmission. Memes spread in cognitive transmission do not count as self-replicating.
# Motivational: ideas that people adopt because they perceive some self-interest in adopting them. Strictly speaking, motivationally transmitted memes do not self-propagate, but this mode of transmission often occurs in association with memes self-replicated in the efficiency parental, proselytic and preservational modes.


== Memes as discrete units ==
Memeticists generate much memetic terminology by prepending 'mem(e)-' to an existing, usually biological, term or by putting 'mem(e)' in place of 'gen(e)' in various terms. Examples include: ], ], ], ].


Dawkins initially defined ''meme'' as a noun that "conveys the idea of a unit of cultural transmission, or a unit of ''imitation''".<ref name="selfish"/> John S. Wilkins retained the notion of meme as a kernel of cultural imitation while emphasizing the meme's evolutionary aspect, defining the meme as "the least unit of sociocultural information relative to a selection process that has favorable or unfavorable selection bias that exceeds its endogenous tendency to change".<ref name="wilkins">
:''See also'' ]
{{cite journal |last=Wilkins |first=John S. |title=What's in a Meme? Reflections from the perspective of the history and philosophy of evolutionary biology |journal=Journal of Memetics |date=1998 |volume=2 |url=http://jom-emit.cfpm.org/ |access-date=13 December 2008 |url-status=dead |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20091201161123/http://jom-emit.cfpm.org/ |archive-date=1 December 2009}}
</ref> The meme as a unit provides a convenient means of discussing "a piece of thought copied from person to person", regardless of whether that thought contains others inside it, or forms part of a larger meme. A meme could consist of a single word, or a meme could consist of the entire speech in which that word first occurred. This forms an analogy to the idea of a gene as a single unit of self-replicating information found on the self-replicating ].


While the identification of memes as "units" conveys their nature to replicate as discrete, indivisible entities, it does not imply that thoughts somehow become ] or that "]ic" ideas exist that cannot be dissected into smaller pieces. A meme has no given size. ] writes that melodies from ]'s symphonies are commonly used to illustrate the difficulty involved in delimiting memes as discrete units. She notes that while the first four notes of ] ({{Audio|Beet5mov1bars1to5.ogg|listen}}) form a meme widely replicated as an independent unit, one can regard the entire symphony as a single meme as well.<ref name="machine"/>
==Memetic evolution==


The inability to pin an idea or cultural feature to quantifiable key units is widely acknowledged as a problem for memetics. It has been argued however that the traces of memetic processing can be quantified utilizing neuroimaging techniques which measure changes in the "connectivity profiles between brain regions".<ref name="mcnamara"/> Blackmore meets such criticism by stating that memes compare with genes in this respect: that while a ] has no particular size, nor can we ascribe every ] feature directly to a particular gene, it has value because it encapsulates that key unit of inherited expression subject to evolutionary pressures. To illustrate, she notes evolution selects for the gene for features such as eye color; it does not select for the individual nucleotide in a strand of ]. Memes play a comparable role in understanding the evolution of imitated behaviors.<ref name="machine"/>
Memetic evolution, like genetic evolution, cannot happen without mutation. Mutation produces the essential variations, whereupon those variations that prove "better" at replication will become more common and therefore have a greater chance at replication again. However, unlike genetic evolution, memetic evolution has no separate underlying ]. If, for example, a mouse loses its tail or a bodybuilder lifts weights, the genetic information in their genotype, stored on their DNA, will remain unchanged, and when that denetic information replicates it will not pass on these acquired superficial characteristics.


''Genes, Mind, and Culture: The Coevolutionary Process'' (1981) by ] and ] proposes the theory that genes and culture co-evolve, and that the fundamental biological units of culture must correspond to neuronal networks that function as nodes of semantic ]. Lumsden and Wilson coined their own word, '']'', which did not catch on. Coauthor Wilson later acknowledged the term ''meme'' as the best label for the fundamental unit of cultural inheritance in his 1998 book '']'', which elaborates upon the fundamental role of memes in unifying the ] and ].<ref>{{harvnb|Wilson|1998}}</ref>
In memetics the ] serves as the genotype and therefore changes in the former will accumulate and get passed on as they replicate. Memetics therefore behaves in a ] manner, highlighting the irony of a great deal of effort and debate devoted to proving that genetic evolution does not function in a Lamarckian manner.


At present, the existence of discrete cultural units which satisfy memetic theory has been challenged in a variety of ways. What is critical from this perspective is that in denying memetics unitary status is to deny a particularly fundamental part of Dawkins' original argument. In particular, denying memes are a unit, or are explainable in some clear unitary structure denies the cultural analogy that inspired Dawkins to define them. If memes are not describable as unitary, memes are not accountable within a neo-Darwinian model of evolutionary culture.
] most likely evolved from just a handful of primitive syllables, the original language phenotypes, into the modern wide array of dialects because of mutation. Further mutations of language include writing, Braille, sign language, etc. Even the oft-cited "]" meme produced variations such as "all your vote are belong to us". Other lines in the originating videogame's dialogue such as "Someone set up us the bomb" also replicated on the Internet, but with less success. Researchers may employ ]s as an imperfect tool in measuring the popularity of various memetic phrases.


Within cultural anthropology, materialist approaches are skeptical of such units. In particular, ] argues that memes are not unitary in the sense that there are no two instances of exactly the same cultural idea, all that can be argued is that there is material mimicry of an idea. Thus every instance of a "meme" would not be a true evolutionary unit of replication.<ref>{{Cite book |first=Dan |last=Sperber |url=http://worldcat.org/oclc/247213620 |title=Explaining culture : a naturalistic approach |date=1998 |publisher=Blackwell Publ |isbn=0631200452 |oclc=247213620 |access-date=23 January 2023 |archive-date=17 March 2023 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20230317201257/https://worldcat.org/title/247213620 |url-status=live}}</ref>
===Do cultures evolve?===
Dawkins observed that ]s can evolve in much the same way that ]s of ]s evolve. Various ideas pass from one ] to the next; such ideas may either enhance or detract from the survival of the people who obtain those ideas. This process affects which of those ideas will survive for passing on to future generations. For example, a certain culture may have unique designs and methods of tool-building that another culture may not have; therefore, the culture with the more effective methods may prosper more than the other culture, ''ceteris paribus''. This leads to a higher proportion of the overall population adopting the more effective methods as time passes. Each tool design thus acts somewhat similarly to a biological gene in that some populations have it and others do not, and the meme's function directly affects the presence of the design in future generations.


Dan Deacon,<ref>{{cite book |last=Deacon |first=Terrence W. |chapter=Memes as Signs in the Dynamic Logic of Semiosis: Beyond Molecular Science and Computation Theory |title=Conceptual Structures at Work |date=2004 |chapter-url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-540-27769-9_2 |series="Lecture Notes in Computer Science" series, no. 3127 |volume=3127 |pages=17–30 |place=Berlin / Heidelberg |publisher=Springer |doi=10.1007/978-3-540-27769-9_2 |isbn=9783540223924 |access-date=17 March 2023 |archive-date=17 March 2023 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20230317201237/https://link.springer.com/chapter/10.1007/978-3-540-27769-9_2 |url-status=live}}</ref> ]<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Kull |first=Kalevi |date=2000 |title=Copy versus translate, meme versus sign: Development of Biological Textuality |url=http://www.zbi.ee/~kalevi/copytr.htm |journal=European Journal for Semiotic Studies |volume=12 |issue=1 |pages=101–120 |access-date=23 January 2023 |archive-date=23 January 2023 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20230123063112/http://www.zbi.ee/~kalevi/copytr.htm |url-status=live}}</ref> separately argued memes are degenerate ] in that they offer only a partial explanation of the triadic in ] semiotic theory: a sign (a reference to an object), an object (the thing being referred to), and an interpretant (the interpreting actor of a sign). They argue the meme unit is a sign which only is defined by its replication ability. Accordingly, in the broadest sense, the objects of copying are memes, whereas the objects of translation and interpretation are signs. Later, Sara Cannizzaro more fully develops out this semiotic relation in order to reframe memes as being a kind of semiotic activity, however she too denies that memes are units, referring to them as "sign systems" instead.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Cannizzaro |first=Sara |date=31 December 2016 |title=Internet memes as internet signs: A semiotic view of digital culture |url=http://ojs.utlib.ee/index.php/sss/article/view/SSS.2016.44.4.05 |journal=Sign Systems Studies |volume=44 |issue=4 |pages=562–586 |doi=10.12697/SSS.2016.44.4.05 |s2cid=53374867 |issn=1736-7409 |access-date=23 January 2023 |archive-date=1 February 2023 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20230201012712/https://ojs.utlib.ee/index.php/sss/article/view/SSS.2016.44.4.05 |url-status=live |doi-access=free}}</ref>
===Propagation of memes===
Memes have as an important characteristic their propagation through ], a concept introduced by the French sociologist ]. Imitation means to copy the ] behaviour of another individual. Typically imitators copy behaviour from observing other humans, but they may also copy from an inanimate source, such as from a book or from a musical score.


In Limor Shifman's account of Internet memetics, she also denies memetics as being unitary.<ref name=":0" /> She argues memes are not unitary, however many assume they are because many previous memetic researchers confounded memes with the cultural interest in "virals": singular informational objects which spread with a particular rate and veracity such as a video or a picture.<ref>{{Cite book |last1=Nahon |first1=Karine |url=https://www.worldcat.org/oclc/849213692 |title=Going viral |last2=Hemsley |first2=Jeff |publisher=Polity Press |date=2013 |isbn=9780745671284 |location=Cambridge, England |oclc=849213692 |access-date=23 January 2023 |archive-date=17 March 2023 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20230317201244/https://www.worldcat.org/title/849213692 |url-status=live}}</ref> As such, Shifman argues that Dawkins' original notion of meme is closer to what communication and information studies consider digitally viral replication.
When imitation first evolved in humans or in their ancestors, it proved itself a valuable skill for learning, which increased an individual's ability to reproduce genetically. Some have speculated that sexual selection of the best imitators further drove a genetic increase in the ability of brains to imitate well.


==Evolutionary influences on memes==
Memes propagate by imitation, direct or indirect, of one individual by another, and thus depend on brains sufficiently powerful to assess the key aspects of the imitated behavior (what to copy and why) as well as its potential benefits. Researchers have observed memetic copying in just a few species on ], including ]s, ]s and ]s which learn how to sing by imitating their parents. One could argue however that there exist examples of less complex memes in other species&mdash;for example, scientists have artificially induced imitative behavior in ]s and in ]s. Zoopharmacognosy (the use of drugs by animals) may conceivably examplify an animal meme. Observers have noticed that some species ingest non-foods, such as toxic plants or charcoal, to ward off parasitic infestation or poisoning, respectively (for an accessible description of several examples, see
Dawkins noted the three conditions that must exist for evolution to occur:<ref name="conscious">{{harvnb|Dennett|1991}}</ref>
{{Web reference 4|author=Biser, J. A.|title=Really Wild Remedies&mdash;Medicinal Plant Use by Animals||URL=http://nationalzoo.si.edu/Publications/ZooGoer/1998/1/reallywildremedies.cfm|refyear=1998|accessdate=January 13|accessyear=2005}}).
# variation, or the introduction of new change to existing elements;
# heredity or replication, or the capacity to create copies of elements;
# differential "fitness", or the opportunity for one element to be more or less suited to the environment than another.
Dawkins emphasizes that the process of evolution naturally occurs whenever these conditions co-exist, and that evolution does not apply only to organic elements such as genes. He regards memes as also having the properties necessary for evolution, and thus sees meme evolution as not simply analogous to genetic evolution, but as a real phenomenon subject to the laws of ]. Dawkins noted that as various ideas pass from one ] to the next, they may either enhance or detract from the survival of the people who obtain those ideas, or influence the survival of the ideas themselves. For example, a certain culture may develop unique designs and methods of ]-making that give it a competitive advantage over another culture. Each tool-design thus acts somewhat similarly to a biological ] in that some populations have it and others do not, and the meme's function directly affects the presence of the design in future generations. In keeping with the thesis that in evolution one can regard organisms simply as suitable "hosts" for reproducing genes, Dawkins argues that one can view people as "hosts" for replicating memes. Consequently, a successful meme may or may not need to provide any benefit to its host.<ref name="conscious"/>


Unlike genetic evolution, memetic evolution can show both ] and ] traits. Cultural memes will have the characteristic of Lamarckian inheritance when a host aspires to replicate the given meme through inference rather than by exactly copying it. Take for example the case of the transmission of a simple skill such as hammering a nail, a skill that a learner imitates from watching a demonstration without necessarily imitating every discrete movement modeled by the teacher in the demonstration, stroke for stroke.<ref>{{harvnb|Dawkins|2004}}</ref> ] distinguishes the difference between the two modes of inheritance in the evolution of memes, characterizing the Darwinian mode as "copying the instructions" and the Lamarckian as "copying the product".<ref name="machine"/>
Both genes and memes can survive much longer than the individual organisms that carry them. A successful gene, such as a gene for powerful teeth in a population of ]s, can remain unchanged in the ] for hundreds of thousands, or even millions of years. A successful meme can propagate itself from one individual to another long after the original form of the meme perished with its carrier.


Clusters of memes, or '']es'' (also known as ''meme complexes'' or as ''memecomplexes''), such as cultural or political doctrines and systems, may also play a part in the acceptance of new memes. Memeplexes comprise groups of memes that replicate together and coadapt.<ref name="machine"/> Memes that fit within a successful memeplex may gain acceptance by "piggybacking" on the success of the memeplex.
Interestingly, memetics suggests that memes have the potential for a much more lasting effect than genes. Most organisms pass their genes on to their offspring sexually, but with every generation the genetic contribution of a given ancestor halves - so that a person only has 1/4 of his/her grandfather's genes, for example. Susan Blackmore has poignantly evaluated the legacy of ]. Since the 5th century BC Socrates' genes have become thoroughly diluted, if any even still exist in the collective gene pool; however, his memes still have a profound effect on modern thought and on contemporary philosophical discourse.
As an example, John D. Gottsch discusses the transmission, mutation and selection of religious memeplexes and the theistic memes contained.<ref>{{cite journal |url= http://cfpm.org/jom-emit/2001/vol5/gottsch_jd.html |last=Gottsch |first=John D. |title=Mutation, Selection, And Vertical Transmission Of Theistic Memes In Religious Canons |journal=Journal of Memetics |volume=5 |issue=1 |date=2001 |access-date=8 October 2021 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20210412200631/http://cfpm.org/jom-emit/2001/vol5/gottsch_jd.html |archive-date=12 April 2021}}
</ref> Theistic memes discussed include the "prohibition of aberrant sexual practices such as incest, adultery, homosexuality, bestiality, castration, and religious prostitution", which may have increased vertical transmission of the parent religious memeplex. Similar memes are thereby included in the majority of religious memeplexes, and harden over time; they become an "inviolable canon" or set of ]s, eventually finding their way into secular ]. This could also be referred to as the propagation of a ].


==Memetics==
==Biological analogies==
{{Main|Memetics}}Memetics is the name of the field of science that studies memes and their evolution and culture spread.{{sfn|Heylighen|Chielens|2009}} While the term "meme" appeared in various forms in German and Austrian texts near the turn of the 20th century, Dawkin's unrelated use of the term in The Selfish Gene marked its emergence into mainstream study. Based on the Dawkin's framing of a meme as a cultural analogue to a gene, meme theory originated as an attempt to apply biological ] principles to cultural ] and ].<ref>{{Cite journal |title=Memes in a Digital World: Reconciling with a Conceptual Troublemaker |url=https://academic.oup.com/jcmc/article/18/3/362/4067545 |access-date=1 May 2023 |journal=Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication|date=2013 |doi=10.1111/jcc4.12013 |last1=Shifman |first1=Limor |volume=18 |issue=3 |pages=362–377 |hdl=11059/14843 |hdl-access=free }}</ref> Thus, memetics attempts to apply conventional scientific methods (such as those used in ] and ]) to explain existing patterns and transmission of ] ideas.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Petrova |first=Yulia |date=2021 |title=Meme language, its impact on digital culture and collective thinking |journal=E3S Web of Conferences |volume=273 |pages=11026 |doi=10.1051/e3sconf/202127311026 |bibcode=2021E3SWC.27311026P |s2cid=237986424 |issn=2267-1242|doi-access=free}}</ref>
In much the same way that the ] concept offers a fruitful way of understanding and reasoning about aspects of biological evolution, the meme concept allegedly can conceivably assist in the better understanding of some otherwise puzzling aspects of human culture (and learned behaviors of other animals as well). However, if one cannot test for "better" empirically, the question will remain whether or not the meme concept counts as a ] scientific theory. Memetics thus remains a science in its infancy, a ] (although critics sometimes label it a ]).


Principal criticisms of memetics include the claim that memetics ignores established advances in other fields of cultural study, such as ], ], ], and ]. Questions remain whether or not the meme concept counts as a ] scientific theory. This view regards memetics as a theory in its infancy: a ] to proponents, or a ] to some detractors.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Benítez-Bribiesca |first=Luis |date=January 2001 |title=MEMETICS: A DANGEROUS IDEA |url=https://www.proquest.com/docview/210137265 |journal=Interciencia |volume=26 |issue=1 |id={{ProQuest|210137265}} |via=Research Library}}</ref>
===Thoughts as discrete units===


=== Criticism of meme theory ===
Although memeticists speak of memes as discrete units, this need not imply that thoughts somehow become ]d or that "]ic" ideas exist which one cannot break down into smaller pieces. The meme as a unit simply provides a convenient way of discussing "a piece of thought copied from person to person", regardless of whether that thought contains others inside it, or forms part of a larger meme. A meme could consist of a single newly-coined word, or a meme could consist of the entire speech in which that word was first uttered. The "word itself" meme will most likely survive many more generations (after transmission alone or in other sentences) than the "speech in its entirety" meme will survive (due to errors of memory, abridged versions, etc.)
One frequent criticism of meme theory looks at the perceived gap in the gene/meme analogy. For example, Luis Benitez-Bribiesca points to the lack of a "code script" for memes (analogous to the DNA of genes), and to the excessive instability of the meme mutation mechanism (that of an idea going from one brain to another), which would lead to a low replication accuracy and a high mutation rate, rendering the evolutionary process chaotic.<ref>{{cite journal |last=Benitez Bribiesca |first=Luis |title=Memetics: A Dangerous Idea |date=January 2001 |url=http://redalyc.org/pdf/339/33905206.pdf |journal=Interciencia: Revista de Ciencia y Technologia de América |volume=26 |issue=1 |pages=29–31 |access-date=11 February 2010 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20180920145421/http://www.redalyc.org/pdf/339/33905206.pdf |url-status=live |issn=0378-1844 |quote=If the mutation rate is high and takes place over short periods, as memetics predict, instead of selection, adaptation and survival a chaotic disintegration occurs due to the accumulation of errors. |archive-date=20 September 2018}}</ref> In his book '']'', Daniel C. Dennett points to the existence of self-regulating correction mechanisms (vaguely resembling those of gene transcription) enabled by the redundancy and other properties of most meme expression languages which stabilize information transfer.<ref name="Dennett19952">{{cite book |last=Dennett |first=Daniel C. |title=Darwin's Dangerous Idea: Evolution and the Meanings of Life |date=1995 |location=New York |publisher=Simon and Schuster}}</ref> Dennett notes that spiritual narratives, including music and dance forms, can survive in full detail across any number of generations even in cultures with oral tradition only. In contrast, when applying only meme theory, memes for which stable copying methods are available will inevitably get selected for survival more often than those which can only have unstable mutations (such as the noted music and dance forms), which, according to meme theory, should have resulted in those forms of cultural expression going extinct.


A second common criticism of meme theory views it as a ] and inadequate<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Fracchia |first1=Joseph |title=The price of metaphor |date=February 2005 |journal=History and Theory |volume=44 |issue=1 |pages=14–29 |doi=10.1111/j.1468-2303.2005.00305.x |issn=0018-2656 |jstor=3590779 |quote=The selectionist paradigm requires the reduction of society and culture to inheritance systems that consist of randomly varying, individual units, some of which are selected, and some not; and with society and culture thus reduced to inheritance systems, history can be reduced to 'evolution.' ... We conclude that while historical phenomena can always be modeled selectionistically, selectionist explanations do no work, nor do they contribute anything new except a misleading vocabulary that anesthetizes history. |first2=Richard |last2=Lewontin |author2-link=Richard Lewontin}}</ref> version of more accepted anthropological theories. Kim Sterelny and Paul Griffiths noted the cumulative evolution of genes depends on biological selection-pressures neither too great nor too small in relation to mutation-rates, while pointing out there is no reason to think that the same balance will exist in the selection pressures on memes.<ref>{{harvnb|Sterelny|Griffiths|1999}}; p. 333</ref> ] theorists such as ]<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Deacon |first1=Terrence |author-link=Terrence Deacon |title=The trouble with memes (and what to do about it) |journal=The Semiotic Review of Books |volume=10 |page=3}}</ref> and ]<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Kull |first1=Kalevi |author-link=Kalevi Kull |date=2000 |title=Copy versus translate, meme versus sign: development of biological textuality |journal=European Journal for Semiotic Studies |volume=12 |issue=1 |pages=101–120}}</ref> regard the concept of a meme as a primitivized or degenerate concept of a ], containing only a sign's basic ability to be copied, but lacks other core elements of the sign concept such as translation and interpretation. Evolutionary biologist ] similarly disapproved of Dawkins's gene-based view of meme, asserting it to be an "unnecessary synonym" for a ], reasoning that concepts are not restricted to an individual or a generation, may persist for long periods of time, and may evolve.
This forms an analogy to the idea of a gene as a self-replicating set of code. The gene in this definition does not consist of a set number of ]s, but simply a collection of nucleotides (possibly in many different locations on the DNA) that replicate together and code for some set of behaviors or body parts.


==Applications==
===Evolution of memes===
Opinions differ as to how best to apply the concept of memes within a "proper" disciplinary framework. One view sees memes as providing a useful philosophical perspective with which to examine ]. Proponents of this view (such as ] and ]) argue that considering cultural developments from a meme's-eye view—''as if'' memes themselves respond to pressure to maximise their own replication and survival—can lead to useful insights and yield valuable predictions into how culture develops over time. Others such as Bruce Edmonds and Robert Aunger have focused on the need to provide an empirical grounding for memetics to become a useful and respected ].<ref>
{{cite journal |url=http://cfpm.org/jom-emit/2002/vol6/edmonds_b_letter.html |last=Edmonds |first=Bruce |date=September 2002 |volume=6 |issue=2 |title=Three Challenges for the Survival of Memetics |journal=Journal of Memetics |access-date=8 October 2021 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20210908040830/http://cfpm.org/jom-emit/2002/vol6/edmonds_b_letter.html |archive-date=8 September 2021}}</ref><ref>{{harvnb|Aunger|2000}}</ref>


A third approach, described by Joseph Poulshock, as "radical memetics" seeks to place memes at the centre of a ] ] and of ].<ref>{{harvnb|Poulshock|2002}}</ref>
Evolution requires not only ] and natural selection but also mutation, and memes also exhibit this property. Ideas may undergo changes in transmission which accumulate over time. Generations of hosts pass on these changes in the "]" (the information in brains or retention systems). In other words, unlike genetic evolution, memetic evolution can show both Darwinian and ] traits. For example, ]s and ]s often become embellished in the retelling to make them more memorable or more appropriate and therefore more impressed hearers have a greater likelihood of retelling them, complete with embellishments. More modern examples appear in the various ]s and ]es&mdash;such as the ] warning&mdash;that circulate on the Internet.


Prominent researchers in ] and ], including ], ], ], ] and others, argue the possibility of incompatibility between ] and memetics.{{citation needed|date=December 2010}} In their view, minds structure certain communicable aspects of the ideas produced, and these communicable aspects generally trigger or elicit ideas in other minds through inference (to relatively rich structures generated from often low-fidelity input) and not high-fidelity replication or imitation. Atran discusses communication involving religious beliefs as a case in point. In one set of experiments he asked religious people to write down on a piece of paper the meanings of the ]. Despite the subjects' own expectations of consensus, interpretations of the commandments showed wide ranges of variation, with little evidence of consensus. In another experiment, subjects with autism and subjects without autism interpreted ideological and religious sayings (for example, "Let a thousand flowers bloom" or "To everything there is a season"). People with autism showed a significant tendency to closely paraphrase and repeat content from the original statement (for example: "Don't cut flowers before they bloom"). Controls tended to infer a wider range of cultural meanings with little replicated content (for example: "Go with the flow" or "Everyone should have equal opportunity"). Only the subjects with autism—who lack the degree of inferential capacity normally associated with aspects of ]—came close to functioning as "meme machines".<ref>{{harvnb|Atran|2002}}</ref>
A behavior, idea or usage distinguishes itself as a meme when some property of itself influences the likelihood of adoption. For example, tool designs affect the efficacy of a tool independently of the habits of the different people using them. Legends and myths often teach a moral lesson or explain a mystery, so they are more likely to be retold to serve different speakers' purposes than other similar stories without those elements.


In his book ''The Robot's Rebellion'', ] uses the memes and memeplex concepts to describe a program of cognitive reform that he refers to as a "rebellion". Specifically, Stanovich argues that the use of memes as a descriptor for cultural units is beneficial because it serves to emphasize transmission and acquisition properties that parallel the study of ]. These properties make salient the sometimes parasitic nature of acquired memes, and as a result individuals should be motivated to reflectively acquire memes using what he calls a "]" process.<ref>{{cite book |last=Stanovich |first=Keith E. |date=2004 |title=The Robot's Rebellion: Finding Meaning in the Age of Darwin |publisher=University of Chicago Press |isbn=9780226770895}}</ref>
===Evolutionary forces affecting memes===


===Memetic explanations of racism===
A gene or a meme's success is determined only by the number of copies (and where the copies reside) that are extant. There exists a strong (but not complete) correlation between genes that do well and genes that have a positive effect on the organism which contains those genes. And if we restrict attention to memes normally interpreted as statements of fact, then a correlation emerges between those memes that do well and those that reflect reality. However, some genes and memes do survive which owe their success to other factors. Similarly, a correlation exists between successful memes of a technological/economic nature and those that help the economy.
In ''Cultural Software: A Theory of Ideology'', ] argued that memetic processes can explain many of the most familiar features of ] thought. His theory of "cultural software" maintained that memes form ]s, social networks, metaphoric and ] models, and a variety of different mental structures. Balkin maintains that the same structures used to generate ideas about free speech or free markets also serve to generate racistic beliefs. To Balkin, whether memes become harmful or maladaptive depends on the environmental context in which they exist rather than in any special source or manner to their origination. Balkin describes racist beliefs as "fantasy" memes that <!-- may? or must? -->become harmful or unjust "ideologies" when diverse peoples come together, as through trade or competition.<ref>{{harvnb|Balkin|1998}}</ref>


==Religion==
A gene's success in a body may stem from its attempt to bypass the normal sexual lottery by making itself present in more than 50% of ]s in an organism. Some genes find other ways of having themselves transmitted between cells. Hence multiple factors influence the evolution of genes &mdash; not just the success of the species as a whole. Similarly the evolutionary pressures on memes include much more than just truth and economic success. Evolutionary pressures may include the following:
{{See also|Evolutionary psychology of religion}}
#''Experience'': If a meme does not correlate with an individual's ], then that individual has a reduced likelihood of remembering that meme
Richard Dawkins called for a re-analysis of religion in terms of the evolution of self-replicating ] ''apart from'' any resulting biological advantages they might bestow.
#''Happiness'': If a meme makes people feel happier then they have a greater likelihood of remembering it
{{blockquote|As an enthusiastic Darwinian, I have been dissatisfied with explanations that my fellow-enthusiasts have offered for human behaviour. They have tried to look for 'biological advantages' in various attributes of human civilization. For instance, tribal religion has been seen as a mechanism for solidifying group identity, valuable for a pack-hunting species whose individuals rely on cooperation to catch large and fast prey. Frequently the evolutionary preconception in terms of which such theories are framed is implicitly group-selectionist, but it is possible to rephrase the theories in terms of orthodox gene selection.|]|'']''}}
#''Fear'': If a meme constitutes a threat then people may become frightened into believing it. The memes "if you do not do this you will burn in hell..." and "...do this and you will go to heaven" provide common examples
#''Censorship'': If an organisation destroys any retention systems containing a particular meme or otherwise controls the usage of said meme, then that meme is put at a selective disadvantage. (Note that "Censorship is wrong" is a meme. It is interesting to speculate that this meme may have prospered by increasing the wealth of those nations that enforced it, thus increasing the influence of that meme itself.)
#''Economics'': If people or organisations with economic influence exhibit a particular meme, then the meme has a greater likelihood of benefitting from a greater audience. If a meme tends to increase the riches of an individual holding it, then that meme is likely to spread because of imitation. Such memes might include "Hard work is good" and "Put number one first."
#''Distinction'': If the meme enables hearers to recognize tellers (as leaders, intelligent people, insightful, etc.), then the meme has a greater chance of spreading. The erstwhile receivers will want to become themselves tellers of the same meme (or an evolved/mutated version). Thus élite knowledge can provide a promotion to élite status.


He argued that the role of key replicator in cultural evolution belongs not to genes, but to memes replicating thought from person to person by means of imitation. These replicators respond to selective pressures that may or may not affect biological reproduction or survival.<ref name="selfish"/>
Memes, like genes, do not purposely do or want anything &mdash; they either get replicated or not. Some meme systems have negative effects on the host or on their host society, but we generally have a symbiotic relationship with these abstract entities.


In her book '']'', ] regards religions as particularly tenacious memes. Many of the features common to the most widely practiced religions provide built-in advantages in an evolutionary context, she writes. For example, religions that preach of the value of ] over ] from everyday experience or ] inoculate societies against many of the most basic tools people commonly use to evaluate their ideas. By linking ] with religious affiliation, religious memes can proliferate more quickly because people perceive that they can reap societal as well as personal rewards. The longevity of religious memes improves with their documentation in revered ].<ref name="machine"/>
Memes don't mutate in an exclusively passive way. The brain inhabited by a meme system performs a sort of active modification of a meme. One could draw an analogy with a cell's error-correction systems, but they clearly function quite differently. In essence, people create and modify memes almost continuously. One can modify, manipulate, and create meme systems in thought, for instance through internal dialogue. As soon as one opens one's mouth and says something (or does something) that one has not copied (but that others can copy), one has unleashed a novel meme. Thus, one could conclude that we all perform the role of a memetic engineer to some degree (even if not consciously).


] attributed the robustness of religious memes in human culture to the fact that such memes incorporate multiple modes of meme transmission. Religious memes pass down the generations from parent to child and across a single generation through the meme-exchange of ]. Most people will hold the religion taught them by their parents throughout their life. Many religions feature adversarial elements, punishing ], for instance, or demonizing ]. In ''Thought Contagion'' Lynch identifies the memes of transmission in ] as especially powerful in scope. Believers view the conversion of non-believers both as a religious duty and as an act of altruism. The promise of ] to believers and threat of ] to non-believers provide a strong incentive for members to retain their belief. Lynch asserts that belief in the ] in Christianity amplifies each of its other replication advantages through the indebtedness believers have to their ] for sacrifice on the cross. The image of the crucifixion recurs in religious ]s, and the proliferation of symbols of the ] in homes and churches potently reinforces the wide array of Christian memes.<ref name="lynch"/>
This seems especially evident in modern society, more notably in the scientific and philosophical realms and in the ]. It has become standard practice for scientists and philosophers alike to assemble memetic systems and to question their philosophical and emperical integrity. On perceiving a flaw, one may seek philosophical (thought experiments/logic/analysis) or ] (experimental/observational/ mathematical) resolution. This happens in large part due to the influence of some of the more "modern" philosophers of the past. Over the last few hundred (or thousand) years, a "philosophy" or ] has evolved and developed which benefits the societies in which many embrace it. That philosophy includes the ethical, moral, and scientific obligation to take nothing for granted and always to question any new information one perceives. People following this tradition have transformed the memetic base of modern science and philosophy. These people include (just to name a scant few) ], ], ], ], ], ], ], ] and ]. Science accepts nothing as true unless empirical evidence and observation suggests such “truth” strongly and consistently. This entire procedure adheres to a meme system that has evolved to the point of rejecting almost any absolute truth. This meme system now includes such novel analytical paradigms as the ] and ]'s ] model (among many other meme-based systems) to help distinguish useful (or truthful) meme systems from "bad" ones.


Although religious memes have proliferated in human cultures, the modern scientific community has been relatively resistant to religious belief. Robertson (2007)<ref name=Robertson2007>{{cite journal |last=Robertson |first=Lloyd Hawkeye |date=2007 |title=Reflections on the use of spirituality to privilege religion in scientific discourse: Incorporating considerations of self |journal=Journal of Religion and Health |volume=46 |issue=3 |pages=449–461 |doi=10.1007/s10943-006-9105-y |s2cid=39449795 |url=https://zenodo.org/record/890987 |access-date=1 July 2019 |archive-date=8 March 2021 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20210308114723/https://zenodo.org/record/890987 |url-status=live}}</ref> reasoned that if evolution is accelerated in conditions of propagative difficulty,<ref name=Dennett1995>{{cite book |last=Dennett |first=Daniel C. |date=1995 |title=Darwin's Dangerous Idea: Evolution and the Meanings of Life |publisher=Simon and Schuster |location=New York}}</ref>{{page number needed|date=October 2023}} then we would expect to encounter variations of religious memes, established in general populations, addressed to scientific communities. Using a memetic approach, Robertson deconstructed two attempts to privilege religiously held spirituality in scientific discourse. Advantages of a memetic approach as compared to more traditional "modernization" and "supply side" theses in understanding the evolution and propagation of religion were explored.
Essentially, people modify and fabricate memes consciously, even intentionally (though some argue that the intention comes from the memes). This would help to explain how rapidly, extensively and usefully memetic evolution has functioned in and for culture. People apply many ever-evolving meme-based systems of analysis and error correction to all information coming in and out. Just as genetic material has developed gene-based error-correction models, memetic systems have found it advantageous to associate with meme-based error-correction models. The entire process could appear as meme-based systemic complexes taking advantage (like a virus) of an extensive computational system (the human brain in this case), programming it to produce and modify memes, and thus to modify and expand the memotypic soup which largely dictates our thoughts and actions (and of course to create very useful, but still likely erroneous memeplexes).


==Architectural memes==
In early social development, people influence and are influenced by memes just like developed individuals. People later may become aware of this influence and begin to consciously filter what influence the meme systems have on them as well as what influence they have on meme systems (likely in response to memeplex programming). In later, possibly somewhat esoteric stages, people become more acutely aware of the meme systems flooding their existence, and many people have begun to reach a more complete comprehension of these memes through a novel memeplex which evolves to explain its own kind, though one does not necessarily need to know of meme theory to realize that the situation exists. Eventually, many see the potential to fabricate or modify meme systems consciously to specific ends, based on conscious plans and logical foresight (all aided by interacting memeplexes which arguably constitute thought), such that the ] becomes a cluttered canvas of interconnected variables which everyone influences. Enlightened, the memetecist (or the meme artist) manifests in society to expand ] at an ever-accelerating rate.
In '']'', ] speaks of memes as "freely propagating clusters of information" which can be beneficial or harmful. He contrasts memes to ] and true knowledge, characterizing memes as "greatly simplified versions of patterns" and as "unreasoned matching to some visual or mnemonic prototype".{{sfn|Salingaros|2008|pp=243, 260}} Taking reference to Dawkins, Salingaros emphasizes that they can be transmitted due to their own communicative properties, that "the simpler they are, the faster they can proliferate", and that the most successful memes "come with a great psychological appeal".{{sfn|Salingaros|2008|pp=243–245}}


Architectural memes, according to Salingaros, can have destructive power: "Images portrayed in architectural magazines representing buildings that could not possibly accommodate everyday uses become fixed in our memory, so we reproduce them unconsciously."{{sfn|Salingaros|2008|p=249}} He lists various architectural memes that circulated since the 1920s and which, in his view, have led to contemporary architecture becoming quite decoupled from human needs. They lack connection and meaning, thereby preventing "the creation of true connections necessary to our understanding of the world". He sees them as no different from ]s in software design—as solutions that are false but are re-utilized nonetheless.{{sfn|Salingaros|2008|p=259}}
Many of the world's most successful religions (and arguably all religions) have become subject to sentient memetic modification over time. ], ], ] and ] (and their descendents) &mdash; just to name a few in that family&mdash;all arose (presumably) through variation, modification and memetic recombination from a common one or few ancestors. (] appears to have functioned as an important and widely-share memetic ancestor, contributing to Judaism, Christianity, Islam and their many derivative religions.) Those ancestors presumably resulted from extensive memetic engineering themselves, possibly more impressive than the modification of their descendants (as early religious meme systems had less to work with).


==Internet culture==
{{Main|Internet meme}}
{{see also|List of Internet phenomena}}
An "Internet meme" is a concept that spreads rapidly from person to person via the ].<ref name="usatoday">{{cite news |url=https://www.usatoday.com/tech/news/2003-07-28-ebay-weirdness_x.htm |newspaper=USA Today |first=Karen |last=Schubert |title=Bazaar goes bizarre |access-date=5 July 2007 |date=2003-07-31 |archive-date=2 July 2012 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20120702142144/http://www.usatoday.com/tech/news/2003-07-28-ebay-weirdness_x.htm |url-status=live}}</ref> Memes can spread from person to person via ]s, ]s, direct ], or news sources. Sending ]s as a form of affection is known as ].<ref name="Edelman">{{cite news |last1=Edelman |first1=Amelia |title=Always sending memes to your loved ones? It's called 'pebbling.' Here's why experts say the trend has its pros and cons. |url=https://www.yahoo.com/lifestyle/always-sending-memes-to-your-loved-ones-its-called-pebbling-heres-why-experts-say-the-trend-has-its-pros-and-cons-100021620.html |access-date=12 July 2024 |work=Yahoo Life |date=26 June 2024}}</ref>


In 2013, Dawkins characterized an Internet meme as one deliberately altered by human creativity, distinguished from his original idea involving mutation "by random change and a form of Darwinian selection".<ref name=Wired20130620>{{cite magazine|url=https://www.wired.co.uk/news/archive/2013-06/20/richard-dawkins-memes |title=Richard Dawkins on the internet's hijacking of the word 'meme' |last=Solon |first=Olivia |date=June 20, 2013 |magazine=Wired UK |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20130709152558/http://www.wired.co.uk/news/archive/2013-06/20/richard-dawkins-memes |archive-date=July 9, 2013 |url-status=dead}}</ref>
] holds that the evolutionary pressures of economy and ecology explain many aspects of human culture. For example, the ]s, sometimes enshrined in religions like the concepts of ]s or ] and ], would have prospered because they allowed the believing population to (say) live more hygienically and thus survive longer than non-believers in their environments. A migration or a change of the economic infrastructure could render the taboo neutral or even adverse.


Internet memes are an example of Dawkins' meme theory at work in the sense of how they so rapidly mirror current cultural events and become a part of how the time period is defined. Limor Shifman uses the example of the 'Gangnam Style' Music video by South Korean pop-star, ] that went viral in 2012. Shifman cites examples of how the meme mutated itself into the cultural sphere, mixing with other things going on at the time such as the ], which led to the creation of ] Style, a parody of the original Gangnam style, intended to be a jab at the 2012 Republican presidential candidate, Mitt Romney.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Pettis |first=Ben T. |date=19 August 2021 |title=Know your meme and the homogenization of Web history |journal=Internet Histories |volume=1-17 |issue=3 |pages=263–279 |doi=10.1080/24701475.2021.1968657 |s2cid=238660211 |url=http://mediarxiv.org/urgy7/ |access-date=28 February 2023 |archive-date=17 March 2023 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20230317201249/https://mediarxiv.org/urgy7/ |url-status=live}}</ref><ref>{{Cite book |last=Denisova |first=Anastasia |title=Internet Memes and Society: Social, Cultural and Political Contexts |publisher=Routledge |date=2019 |isbn=9780429469404 |location=New York |pages=13–26}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal |last=Shifman |first=Limor |date=26 March 2013 |title=Memes in a Digital World: Reconciling with a Conceptual Troublemaker |journal=Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication |volume=18 |issue=3 |pages=362–377 |doi=10.1111/jcc4.12013 |doi-access=free|hdl=11059/14843 |hdl-access=free }}</ref>
===Memetic virus exchange?===


===Meme stocks===
One controversial application of this "selfish meme" parallel results in the idea that certain collections of memes can act as "memetic viruses": collections of ideas that behave as independent life forms which continue to get passed on &mdash; even at the expense of their hosts &mdash; simply because of their success at getting passed on. Some observers have suggested that ] ]s and ]s behave this way; so by including the act of passing on their beliefs as a moral virtue, other beliefs of the religion also get passed along even if they do not provide particular benefits to the believer.
{{Main|Meme stock}}
Meme stocks, a particular subset of Internet memes in general, are listed companies lauded for the social media buzz they create, rather than their operating performance.<ref>{{cite web |url=https://www.fool.com/investing/stock-market/types-of-stocks/meme-stocks/ |title=What Are Meme Stocks? |date=23 September 2021 |website=] |first=Nicholas |last=Rossolillo |access-date=8 October 2021 |archive-date=13 November 2021 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20211113155252/https://www.fool.com/investing/stock-market/types-of-stocks/meme-stocks/ |url-status=live}}</ref> Meme stocks find themselves surging in popularity after gaining the interest of individuals or groups through the internet.<ref>{{Cite web |last=Rossolillo |first=Nicholas |title=Top Meme Stocks of 2023 |url=https://www.fool.com/investing/stock-market/types-of-stocks/meme-stocks/ |access-date=23 April 2023 |website=The Motley Fool}}</ref> ], a ] where participants discuss ], and the financial services company ], became notable in 2021 for their involvement on the popularization and enhancement of meme stocks.<ref>{{Cite news |last1=Phillips |first1=Matt |last2=Marcos |first2=Coral Murphy |date=4 August 2021 |title=Robinhood's shares jump as much as 65 percent, like the meme stocks it enabled. |work=The New York Times |url=https://www.nytimes.com/2021/08/04/business/robinhood-stock-price.html |access-date=15 June 2023 |issn=0362-4331}}</ref><ref>{{Cite news |last1=Popper |first1=Nathaniel |last2=Browning |first2=Kellen |date=29 January 2021 |title=The 'Roaring Kitty' Rally: How a Reddit User and His Friends Roiled the Markets |work=The New York Times |url=https://www.nytimes.com/2021/01/29/technology/roaring-kitty-reddit-gamestop-markets.html |access-date=15 June 2023 |issn=0362-4331}}</ref> One of the most commonly recognized instances of a meme stock is ], whose stocks saw a sudden increase after a ]-led idea to invest in 2021.<ref>{{Cite web |last=Brown |first=Abram |title=Robinhood Stock's Surprise Supporters: Investors From The Reddit Group Who Hated The Company |url=https://www.forbes.com/sites/abrambrown/2021/08/05/robinhood-stock-reddit-meme-stock-wall-street-bets/ |access-date=23 April 2023 |website=Forbes}}</ref>


==Politics==
Others maintain that the wide prevalence of human adoption of religious ideas provides evidence to suggest that such ideas offer some ecological, sexual, ethical or moral value; otherwise memetic evolution would long ago have selected against such ideas. For example, most religions urge peace and cooperation among their followers ("Thou shalt not kill") which may possibly tend to promote the biological survival of the social groups that carry these memes. <!-- Certainly religious promoters claim such value for following their rules or principles&mdash;but how does that relate to what they feel is divine? -->
In the ] the presidential campaigns have utilized memes on the Internet in the last three cycles. ] has been charged by political contestants with memes being a concern of the complaints.<ref>Helmus, Todd C. Artificial Intelligence, Deepfakes, and Disinformation: A Primer. RAND Corporation, 2022. JSTOR, http://www.jstor.org/stable/resrep42027. Retrieved 17 Feb. 2024.</ref><ref>Anderson, Karrin Vasb, and Kristina Horn Sheeler. “Texts (and Tweets) from Hillary: Meta-Meming and Postfeminist Political Culture.” ''Presidential Studies Quarterly'', vol. 44, no. 2, 2014, pp. 224–43. JSTOR, http://www.jstor.org/stable/43286740. Accessed 17 Feb. 2024.</ref>
==See also==
<!-- Please keep this list alphabeticised. Thanks! -->
{{div col|colwidth=18em}}
* ]
* '']''
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
{{div col end}}


==Notes==
A tendency exists in memetics to disparage religious memes. However, some speculate that traditional religions act as mental immune systems to suppress new and potentially harmful memes. Interestingly, we can compare this scenario with the action of a virus (here a religion &mdash; a "bundle" of religious memes) proving ineffective and maladaptive if it kills its host(s). For example, popular ] forbids both murder and suicide (an idea from ]'s '']''), and its precise definitions of ] ensure that "properly" educated Christians cannot accept new religions which advocate such actions.
{{Reflist}}

One could make a case (as Susan Blackmore ) that the study of ] ] in itself comprises a process of meme "pruning," i.e., a means to remove experiential clichés that reduce the value of life. This has not exempted ] itself from serving as a source of highly mobile memes, such as "the sound of one hand clapping" or "]".

It may surprise many memetics advocates to learn of meme-like concepts described long ago, and prevalent in ] teaching. ]s rank as separate beings, elementals, that make up human thought (compare ]'s ]s).

(Note that the framing of this whole discussion may mislead. If humans, as seen rigorously from Dawkins' perspective, comprise a collection of the extended effects of our various genes and memes, then the question of what counts as "valuable to the individual" cannot readily become separated from what benefits their genes and/or memes. Since one cannot easily determine the "ecological, sexual, ethical or moral value" of a meme coutside of the context of the memes of the determiner, memeticists can easily misuse the idea of "meme" or of a "meme virus" to reject others' positions in a pseudo-scientific way. For example, insofar as you agree with me&mdash;i.e., we carry the same memes&mdash;I call your ideas "ideas"; insofar as I find your ideas wrong&mdash;i.e., you fail to transmit your memes to me&mdash;I can call them "memes" and brand you as "infected with a meme virus".)

Dawkins notes that one can distinguish a biological virus from its host's normal genetic material by the fact that it can propagate alone, without the entire genetic corpus of the host being propagated&mdash;or half of it, in the case of ] ]; thus, a virus can "sabotage" the host's other genes. This applies to memes in the sense that a meme that requires the success of its hosts has a greater likelihood of favouring the interests of these hosts than a does meme capable of succeeding even if each host quickly dies. For example, the commonplace meme encouraging people to wash their hands after they use the bathroom or before handling food, and to remind others to do the same, is not at all harmful. In contrast, a cultish meme telling people to quit their jobs, abandon their families, and run around spreading the meme seems quite virulent.

Memetics offers maximum explanatory value in cases where one cannot demonstrate the truth of the contents of the meme. For example, one can readily show that washing hands helps to prevent illness, so the best explanation for the widespread popularity of this practice is that "it works," though memetics still helps explain the rate of spread, and details such as why the practice of washing hands before surgery took so long to catch on. Memetics though excels in explaining the spread of certain value judgements ("chastity is important"), preferences ("pork is icky"), superstitions ("black cats are unlucky") and other scientifically unverifiable beliefs ("Allah is the one true God"), since one cannot easily account for any of these phenomena in terms of their truth-value. Calling someone's ideas/beliefs/action a "meme" therefore does not constitute an insult, but saying that it is "just a meme" does.

===Non-natural selection===
How "naturally" does this type of selection occur? Perhaps as naturally as sexual attraction or as ethical habits. The relationship of the meme to other ideas of evolution, e.g., those that separate ecological, sexual, ethical and moral factors and reserve no special or separate role for "culture" beyond these, seems to resemble that of a "pretender to the throne"&mdash;pretending to explain these more specific ideas of evolution and culture&mdash;but without any model to test. This causes quite a few scientists and others to scoff at culture as any kind of factor in human life.

A famous observation of this type came from ], who bluntly stated: "there is no such thing as society"&mdash;evidently she saw "it" as a set of survival, seduction and moral choice factors specific to individuals, couples and families, and not as a unified "culture" or "society" in any sense.

===Reproductive isolation in meme "speciation"===
In traditional population genetics the normal ], selection, and drift do not lead to formation of a new species without some form of "]"; i.e., in order to split a single species into two species, the two subpopulations of the original species must ultimately lose their ability to interbreed, which would normally maintain their heterogeneity. However, once separated, natural selection and/or just ] acting on the normal genetic variation in the two subspecies will eventually change enough characteristics of the two subgroups that they can no longer interbreed, which by definition means that they will comprise two different species. Examples of reproductive isolation include geographical isolation, where a 'suddenly' appearing mountain range or river separates the two subgroups; temporal isolation, where one subgroup becomes entirely ] in its habits while the other becomes entirely ]; or even just 'behavioral' isolation, as seen in ] and ]: they ''could'' interbreed, biologically speaking, but normally they do not.

A similar phenomenon can occur with memes. Normally, the population of individuals having a meme in their consciousness is heterogeneous and mixes enough to keep the meme intact although it covers a wide range of variations. Should that population become split, however, without sufficient contact for the two different subgroups of variations of the meme to equilibrate, eventually each group will evolve its own version of that meme, differing sufficiently from that of the other group to appear as a distinct entity.

The ''Kellerman'' meme provides an example of this occurring on the Internet. A search of the web and/or Usenet for the word 'Kellerman' will turn up a large number of citations, describing at great length the behavior of a 'Dr. Arthur Kellerman', who, with the willing assistance of the ] and the public health lobby, purportedly fabricated studies in order to implicate firearms (and by extension their owners) as a menace to public safety, for the purposes of statist control of the population. The authors of these pages and postings describe purported machinations, "junk science," a subsequent recantation by Dr. 'Kellerman', and the use of his work by ] proponents.

In reality, no "Dr. Arthur Kellerman" exists, at least not in any connection with the above description. There is, however, a ] (with ''double'' n), who has indeed published several papers estimating the overall impact on the public health of firearm availability and various aspects of firearm storage, as part of a career in public health and emergency and trauma medicine. As in any such series of studies, Kellermann's work has strengths and weaknesses, which pundits rigorously debate both in the literature and online. However, even after eliminating matters of opinion and statements which are not fully supported, the remaining verifiable facts of Kellermann's studies and career remain virtually unrecognizable in the negative descriptions of 'Kellerman.'

The original meme of Kellermann and his work on gun-related violent injury has generated a new meme, "Dr. Kellerman is a evil lying gun-grabbing enemy of freedom," by the classic genetic phenomenon of a ]. The sub-population involved had strongly negative attitudes towards Kellermann's work as well as a lack of first-hand familiarity with his studies and career. Because of the "reproductive isolation" caused by the total non-intersection of the results of searches for "Kellerman" and "Kellermann," the 'Kellerman' meme drifted even further in the direction of negativity, unchecked by facts related to the real Kellermann. As this group encounters new individuals of similar general outlook, they introduce new recruits to the 'Kellerman' lore only, and go on to produce their own websites and postings furthering the rapid progress of this meme.

This phenomenon also demonstrates two other features of memes &mdash; the "meme-complex," a set of mutually-assisting "co-memes" which have co-evolved a symbiotic relationship, and the infection strategy.

==Forms taken by memes in the brain==

In 1981 biologists ] and ] published a theory of gene-culture coevolution in the book ''Genes, Mind, and Culture: The Coevolutionary Process''. They pointed out that the fundamental biological units of culture must correspond to neuronal networks that function as nodes of semantic ]. Wilson later adopted the term 'meme' as the best existing name for the fundamental unit of cultural inheritance and elaborated upon the fundamental role of memes in unifying the ] and ] in his book '']''.

===The "be happy" and "make others happy" memes===

Some spiritual practices such as ] clearly promote ecological and moral goals recognizable to most people, i.e., The ] emphasizes limited consumption, reduced cruelty, no delegation of violence or participation in violent systems, and a withdrawal from sexual and ethical processes that have no clear ecological or moral value to the practitioner&mdash;regardless of the value they may have to others.

The ] "Western" religions, however, focus more on devotion to a transcendent deity and to moral codes of behavior, including social and ethical codes affecting every aspect of life from public behavior to commerce to sexual expression. Such religions strongly encourage people to devote themselves to the needs of others.

The contrast between "be happy" and "make others happy," although not as stark in practice or theory as the traditional debate suggests, may satisfy constraints of different ecological or sexual norms in some non-obvious way. But it seems entirely likely that they are valuable to the believer. At least, the majority of people on Earth believe so.

===Religion===

Some (such as Dawkins himself, see ) consider ] itself a meme &mdash; or, more exactly, a memeplex or group of memes. To observers infected by a different set of memes, it appears that some ] ] movements act only to add to their own numbers. The movements in question devote what appears to their opponents as an inordinate amount of time to evangelical activity, and therefore may seem to unsympathetic observers to serve no other function. This makes it possible to characterize them as self-serving if one does not accept their premises (such as postulating a ]). On the other hand, for the meme to continue to propagate, it must provide some ] or ]al ]: ], a release from worry and ], a sense of ], happiness, moral energy, etc.

The ] in the ] has a unified message built around religious ]. By attaching conservative political views to Christian religious evangelism (meme piggybacking), they have associated a set of political ideas/memeplexes with a set of religious ideas/memeplexes that throughout history have "replicated" very effectively. That is, Christianity has won converts for centuries; now in many cases a religious conversion also becomes a political conversion.

Similarly, the ] offers a body of social and experimental techniques which, given certain preconditions&mdash;a free press for the circulation of information, a large number of people predisposed to see the world as a mechanism subject to general rules which can be discovered through repeatable experiments&mdash;acts highly virulently, spreading quickly through an educated population. By demonstrating its success at making predictions, science as a practice can make itself more attractive to converts. Ideas and attitudes which are not necessarily verifiable by experimentation, but which tend to be held by scientists or feel aesthetically pleasing in combination with scientific discoveries, can propagate themselves in societies where science has a high status by the same process of "meme piggybacking."

===Meme resistance===
] advocated this in the strongest possible terms: "The survival value of intelligence is that it allows us to extinct a bad idea, before the idea extincts us."

Resistance to science and technology has formed a common meme that can guide human cultural and cognitive evolution away from disastrous paths&mdash;for instance the US and USSR stockpiled but did not use ] in the ] period. Ignorance has been in some cultures considered a virtue&mdash;in particular, ignorance of certain temptations that the culture believes would be disastrous if pursued by many individuals.

The Internet, perhaps the ultimate meme vector, seems to be hosting both sides of this debate. Although it would seem to a naïve observer that no adult user of the Internet could oppose its use by other adults, that does in fact happen, based on any number of criteria from ethics to intent to ability to resist ] or ].

The ] project maintains a , comprising a list of different types of memes. It also refers to an essay by ], '''', which criticises very strongly "]s" who assert memes over bodies.

==Examples of memes==
Crudely-stated versions of some common memes include:
*]: cars, paper-clips, etc. Technology clearly demonstrates mutation as well, which memetic (or genetic) progress requires. Many paper-clip designs have emerged throughout history, for example, with varying degrees of longevity, fecundity and copying fidelity (i.e., memetic "success"). An often-cited example of "technology as meme" involves the building of a fire.
*]s: ] ]s set to an engaging ]
*]s: songs that you can't stop humming or thinking. "It's a Small World After All" is a common example.
*]s: or at least those jokes popularly considered funny
*]s and ]s: "You can't keep a good man down."
*]s: propagated from parent to child over many generations, sometimes with associated actions and movements.
*Children's culture: games, activities and taunts typical for different age groups.
*]s: once important memes for preserving oral history; writing has largely superceded them.
*]s: "You must send this message to five other people, or something bad will happen to you."
*]
*]: "I am a lucky person. Here are some stories of my luck. If you believe in good luck, you can become lucky like me." (and its obverse)
*Fashions: especially clothing styles such as ].
*Medical and safety advice: "Don't swim for an hour after eating" or "Steer in the direction of a skid."
*]: very memetic given their mass replication, movies tend to cause people to replicate scenes or repeat popular ]s such as "You can't handle the truth!" from '']'' or "Alllllllrighty then!" from '']'', even if they have not seen the movie themselves.
*]s: complex memes, including ] beliefs; can even spread virally (such as ]).
*Popular concepts: these include ], ], ], ], ], or ]
*Group-based biases: everything from ] and ] to ]s.
*Longstanding political memes that suppress democratic notions and activity, such as "mob rule" and "republic, not a democracy."
*]: from ] and ] to ].
*]: ] and ] such as "]."
*]s: the proliferation of collaborative editing systems following the Wiki example in their multiple incarnations. ], ], etc.
*]: this meme has a particularly interesting form of self-replication. The conviction that "semiconductor complexity doubles every 18 months" became considerably more than a predictive observation; it became a performance target for an entire industry once it was extensively believed. Manufacturers now strive to make the next generation of semiconductor technology recreate the performance growth of the previous generation, and so maintain belief in Moore's Law.
*] and the ]: ] theorized that a "self" merely comprises a collection of memetic stories which she calls the ''selfplex''.
*The concept of memes itself comprises a meme. Even the idea that the concept of memes is itself a meme has become a widely spread meme. However, the idea that the idea that the concept of memes is itself a meme is not yet particularly common as a meme. (Not to mention that, at this stage, the idea makes most people's heads hurt.)

The ''Memetic Lexicon'' lists meme attributes compiled by Glenn Grant under a "share-alike" licence. The thoughtful examples it offers help to focus the concept for readers unfamiliar with memetic thinking. The Lexicon has circulated since the early 1990s, and evolved into its version 3.5 memeplex in 2004:

==Common misconceptions==
A very common misconception about memes represents them as very special, rare kinds of thought or as some special trick of ] gurus. Generally, memes can comprise any piece of information that can possibly transfer between two minds &mdash; idea, thought, joke, song, dance, habit, even state of mood.

==See also==
<!-- Please keep this list alphabetized. Thanks! -->
*]
*]
*]
*]
*]
*]
*]
*]
*]
*]
*]
*]
*]
*]
*]
*]
*]
*]
*]
*]
*] artist group
*]
*], a video game centered on the concept of ] as memetic
*]
*]
*]
*]
*]
*]
*]
*]


==References== ==References==
{{refbegin|30em}}
*''The ]'' by ], , 1976, 2nd edition, December 1989, hardcover, 352 pages, ISBN 0192177737; April 1992, ISBN 019857519X; trade paperback, September 1990, 352 pages, ISBN 0192860925
* {{Cite book |last=Atran |first=Scott |author-link=Scott Atran |title=In Gods We Trust: The Evolutionary Landscape of Religion |publisher=Oxford University Press |date=2002 |isbn=9780195149302}}
*''The Music of Life'', ], Omega Uniform Edition, 2nd edition, 1993, trade paperback: 353 pages, ISBN 093087238X. An introduction to the ''muwakkals,'' the Eastern memes.
* {{Cite journal |last=Atran |first=Scott |author-link=Scott Atran |url=http://sites.lsa.umich.edu/wp-content/uploads/sites/330/2015/10/human_nature_01.pdf |title=The Trouble with Memes |journal=Human Nature |issue=4 |volume=12 |date=2001 |doi=10.1007/s12110-001-1003-0 |pmid=26192412 |pages=351–381 |s2cid=1530055 |access-date=8 October 2021 |archive-date=30 October 2021 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20211030165058/https://sites.lsa.umich.edu/wp-content/uploads/sites/330/2015/10/human_nature_01.pdf |url-status=live}}
*''Virus of the Mind: The New Science of the Meme'' by ], Integral Pr, September 1995, 251 Pages, ISBN 09636001
* {{Cite book |last=Aunger |first=Robert |title=Darwinizing Culture: The Status of Memetics as a Science |publisher=Oxford University Press |date=2000 |isbn=9780192632449}}
*''The Lucifer Principle: A Scientific Expedition into the Forces of History'' by ], Atlantic Monthly Press, February 1997, 480 pages, ISBN 0871136643
* {{Cite book |last=Aunger |first=Robert |title=The Electric Meme: A New Theory of How We Think |publisher=Free Press |location=New York |date=2002 |isbn=9780743201506}}
*''The Meme Machine'' by ], , 1999, hardcover ISBN 0198503652, trade paperback ISBN 0965881784, May 2000, ISBN 019286212X
* {{Cite book |last=Balkin |first=J. M. |title=Cultural Software: A Theory of Ideology |publisher=] |location=New Haven, Connecticut |date=1998 |isbn=9780300072884}}
*'''' by Agner Fog. Dordrecht: Kluwer 1999. ISBN 0-7923-5579-2.
* {{Cite book |last=Bloom |first=Howard S. |title=The Lucifer Principle: A Scientific Expedition into the Forces of History |author-link=Howard Bloom |publisher=Atlantic Monthly Press |location=Boston |date=1997 |page=480 |isbn=9780871136640}}
*''Thought Contagion: How Belief Spreads Through Society'' by Aaron Lynch, Basic Books, 1999, ISBN 0465084672
* {{cite journal |last=Blackmore |first=Susan |author-link=Susan Blackmore |title=Imitation and the definition of a meme |journal=Journal of Memetics |date=1998 |volume=2 |url=http://www.baillement.com/texte-blakemore.pdf |access-date=12 December 2008 |archive-date=26 February 2021 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20210226124815/http://www.baillement.com/texte-blakemore.pdf |url-status=live}}
**Review:
* {{Cite book |last=Blackmore |first=Susan |author-link=Susan Blackmore |title=The Meme Machine |publisher=Oxford University Press |date=1999 |page=288 |isbn=9780198503651}}
*'']'' by ], Bantam Doubleday Dell, reprint, 2000, trade paperback: 440 pages, ISBN 0553380958 (science fiction novel about a metavirus which can penetrate and take over ''any'' information system, and thus can spread as gene, meme, or biological virus)
* {{Cite book |last=Brodie |first=Richard |author-link=Richard Brodie (programmer) |title=Virus of the Mind: The New Science of the Meme |publisher=Integral Press |location=Seattle, Washington |date=1996 |page=251 |isbn=9780963600110}}
*"Eyes at the back of your head: How Richard Semon's memes gave way to Richard Dawkins's memes" by ], ''Times Literary Supplement'', October 19, 2001
* {{Cite book |edition=2nd |publisher=Oxford University Press |isbn=9780192177735 |page=368 |last=Dawkins |first=Richard |title=The Selfish Gene |date=1989 |chapter=11. Memes: The new replicators |author-link=Richard Dawkins |title-link=The Selfish Gene}}
*''The Electric Meme: A New Theory of How We Think'' by ], Free Press, 2002, hardcover ISBN 0743201507
* {{Cite book |last=Dawkins |first=Richard |author-link=Richard Dawkins |title=A Devil's Chaplain: Reflections on Hope, Lies, Science, and Love |publisher=Mariner Books |location=Boston |date=2004 |page=263 |isbn=9780618485390 |title-link=A Devil's Chaplain: Reflections on Hope, Lies, Science, and Love}}
*'''', an essay by Jaron Lanier which very strongly criticises "meme totalists" who assert memes over bodies.
* {{Cite book |publisher=] / ] |isbn=9780593072561 |pages=404–408 |last=Dawkins |first=Richard |title=Brief Candle in the Dark: My Life in Science |location=London |date=2015 |chapter=Memes |author-link=Richard Dawkins |title-link=Brief Candle in the Dark: My Life in Science}}
*
* {{Cite book |last=Dennett |first=Daniel |author-link=Daniel Dennett |title=Breaking the Spell: Religion as a Natural Phenomenon |date=2006 |publisher=Viking (Penguin) |isbn=9780670034727}}
*] holds a , comprising a list of different types of memes.
* {{Cite book |last=Dennett |first=Daniel |author-link=Daniel Dennett |title=Consciousness Explained |location=Boston |publisher=Little, Brown and Co. |date=1991 |isbn=9780316180658}}
*A list of
* {{Cite book |last=Distin |first=Kate |title=The Selfish Meme: A Critical Reassessment |publisher=] |date=2005 |page=238 |isbn=9780521606271}}
*'''' by Alan Carr. Lulu Publishing, Content .58184 Examines ] as a mind-virus.
* {{Cite book |last=Farnish |first=Keith |title=Time's Up! An Uncivilized Solution to a Global Crisis |publisher=Green Books |location=Totnes |page=256 |isbn=9781900322485 |date=2009}}
*'''' by ], a formal characterization of memes.
* {{Cite book |last=Graham |first=Gordon |title=Genes: A Philosophical Inquiry |publisher=Routledge |location=New York |date=2002 |page=196 |isbn=9780415252577}}
*'''' by ], formal interplays between memetics and cultural analysis.
* {{Cite book |last1=Heylighen |first1=Francis |author-link=Francis Heylighen |last2=Chielens |first2=K. |date=2009 |title=Encyclopedia of Complexity and Systems Science: Evolution of Culture, Memetics |editor-last=Meyers |editor-first=B. |url=http://pespmc1.vub.ac.be/Papers/Memetics-Springer.pdf |bibcode=2009ecss.book.....M |doi=10.1007/978-0-387-30440-3 |isbn=9780387758886 |access-date=22 May 2009 |archive-date=24 February 2021 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20210224212950/http://pespmc1.vub.ac.be/Papers/Memetics-Springer.pdf |url-status=live}}
*'''' by Klaas Chielens
* {{Cite journal |last=Ingold |first=Tim |author-link=Tim Ingold |date=2000 |title=The poverty of selectionism |journal=Anthropology Today |volume=16 |issue=3 |page=1 |doi=10.1111/1467-8322.00022}}
* {{cite journal |author-link=Francis Heylighen|last=Heylighen |first=Francis |date=1992 |title=Selfish Memes and the Evolution of Cooperation |journal=Journal of Ideas |volume=2 |issue=4 |pages=77–84}}
* {{cite book |last=Jan |first=Steven |date=2007 |url=https://www.ashgate.com/default.aspx?page=637&calcTitle=1&title_id=8553&edition_id=9264 |title=The Memetics of Music: A Neo-Darwinian View of Musical Structure and Culture |location=Aldershot |publisher=Ashgate |access-date=10 December 2008 |archive-date=5 January 2015 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20150105022128/https://www.ashgate.com/default.aspx?page=637&calcTitle=1&title_id=8553&edition_id=9264 |url-status=dead}}
* {{Cite book |last=Kelly |first=Kevin |title=Out of Control: The New Biology of Machines, Social Systems and the Economic World |publisher=Addison-Wesley |location=Boston |date=1994 |page= |isbn=9780201483406 |url=https://archive.org/details/outofcontrolnewb00kell/page/360}}
* {{Cite book |last=Lynch |first=Aaron |author-link=Aaron Lynch (writer) |title=Thought Contagion: How Belief Spreads Through Society |publisher=BasicBooks |location=New York |date=1996 |page=208 |isbn=9780465084678}}
* {{Cite journal |last=McNamara |first=Adam |title=Can we measure memes? |journal=Frontiers in Evolutionary Neuroscience |volume=3 |pages=1 |doi=10.3389/fnevo.2011.00001 |date=2011 |pmc=3118481 |pmid=21720531 |doi-access=free}}
* {{Cite book |last=Millikan |first=Ruth Garrett |author-link=Ruth Garrett Millikan |title=Varieties of Meaning: The 2002 Jean Nicod Lectures |publisher=] |location=Cambridge, Massachusetts |date=2004 |isbn=9780262134446}}
* {{Cite book |last1=Post |first1=Stephen Garrard |last2=Underwood |first2=Lynn G. |last3=Schloss |first3=Jeffrey P. |last4=Hurlbut |first4=Willam B. |title=Altruism & Altruistic Love: Science, Philosophy, & Religion in Dialogue |publisher=Oxford University Press |date=2002 |page=500 |isbn=9780195143584}}
* {{cite journal |last=Moritz |first=Elan |date=1995 |title=Metasystems, Memes and Cybernetic Immortality |editor1-last=Heylighen |editor1-first=F. |editor2-last=Joslyn |editor2-first=C. |editor3-last=Turchin |editor3-first=V. |journal=World Futures: The Journal of General Evolution |volume=45 |issue=Special Issue: ''The Quantum of Evolution: Toward a Theory of Metasystem Transitions'' |publisher=Gordon and Breach Science Publishers |location=New York |pages=155–171|doi=10.1080/02604027.1995.9972558 }}
* {{Cite journal |last=Poulshock |first=Joseph |title=The Problem and Potential of Memetics |journal=Journal of Psychology and Theology |publisher=Rosemead School of Psychology / Gale Group |date=2002 |volume=30 |pages=68–80|doi=10.1177/009164710203000105 |s2cid=140875579 }}
* {{cite book |last=Russell |first=Bertrand |author-link=Bertrand Russell |title=The Analysis of Mind |publisher=George Allen & Unwin |location=London |date=1921 |url=https://gutenberg.org/ebooks/2529}}
* {{cite book |last=Salingaros |first=Nikos |author-link=Nikos Salingaros |date=2008 |title=Theory of Architecture |chapter=Architectural memes in a universe of information |isbn=9783937954073 |publisher=Umbau-Verlag}}
* {{cite book |last1=Sterelny |first1=Kim |last2=Griffiths |first2=Paul E. |title=Sex and Death: An Introduction to Philosophy of Biology |publisher=] |date=1999 |page=456 |isbn=9780226773049}}
* {{cite book |last=Veszelszki |first=Ágnes |contribution=Promiscuity of Images: Memes from an English–Hungarian Contrastive Perspective |editor1-last=Benedek |editor1-first=András |editor2-last=Nyíri |editor2-first=Kristóf |title=How to Do Things with Pictures: Skill, Practice, Performance |series="Visual Learning" series, no. 3 |publisher=Peter Lang |location=Frankfurt |date=2013 |pages=115–127 |isbn=9783631629727}}
* {{Cite book |last=Wilson |first=Edward O. |author-link=E. O. Wilson |title=Consilience: The Unity of Knowledge |publisher=Knopf |location=New York |date=1998 |page= |isbn=9780679450771 |url=https://archive.org/details/consilienceunity00wils/page/352}}
{{refend}}


==External links== ==External links==
{{Spoken Misplaced Pages|Spoken_Wikipedia_-_Meme.ogg|date=2019-8-29}}
*
{{Wiktionary}}
*
{{Commons category|Memes and image macros}}
*
* , Dawkins 2006
*
* : article by ].
*
* {{cite news |url= https://www.wired.com/wired/archive/2.10/godwin.if_pr.html |title=Meme, Counter-meme |first=Mike |last=Godwin |author-link=Mike Godwin |magazine=] |access-date=15 November 2009}}
*
* , a peer-refereed journal of memetics published from 1997 until 2005.
*
* , TED Talks February 2008.
*
* , translated from: ] (ed.), ''Enzyklopädie Philosophie und Wissenschaftstheorie'', 2nd edn, vol. 5, Stuttgart/Weimar: Metzler 2013.
*
*


{{Richard Dawkins}}
]
{{World view}}
]
{{Portal bar|Society}}
]
{{Authority control}}
]


] ]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]

Latest revision as of 08:03, 6 January 2025

Cultural idea which spreads through imitation For the usage of the term on the Internet, see Internet meme. For other uses, see Meme (disambiguation).

Part of a series on
Anthropology
Types
Archaeological
Biological
Linguistic
Research framework
Key concepts
Key theories
Lists

A meme (/miːm/ ; MEEM) is an idea, behavior, or style that spreads by means of imitation from person to person within a culture and often carries symbolic meaning representing a particular phenomenon or theme. A meme acts as a unit for carrying cultural ideas, symbols, or practices, that can be transmitted from one mind to another through writing, speech, gestures, rituals, or other imitable phenomena with a mimicked theme. Supporters of the concept regard memes as cultural analogues to genes in that they self-replicate, mutate, and respond to selective pressures. In popular language, a meme may refer to an Internet meme, typically an image, that is remixed, copied, and circulated in a shared cultural experience online.

Proponents theorize that memes are a viral phenomenon that may evolve by natural selection in a manner analogous to that of biological evolution. Memes do this through processes analogous to those of variation, mutation, competition, and inheritance, each of which influences a meme's reproductive success. Memes spread through the behavior that they generate in their hosts. Memes that propagate less prolifically may become extinct, while others may survive, spread, and (for better or for worse) mutate. Memes that replicate most effectively enjoy more success, and some may replicate effectively even when they prove to be detrimental to the welfare of their hosts.

A field of study called memetics arose in the 1990s to explore the concepts and transmission of memes in terms of an evolutionary model. Criticism from a variety of fronts has challenged the notion that academic study can examine memes empirically. However, developments in neuroimaging may make empirical study possible. Some commentators in the social sciences question the idea that one can meaningfully categorize culture in terms of discrete units, and are especially critical of the biological nature of the theory's underpinnings. Others have argued that this use of the term is the result of a misunderstanding of the original proposal.

The word meme itself is a neologism coined by Richard Dawkins, originating from his 1976 book The Selfish Gene. Dawkins's own position is somewhat ambiguous. He welcomed N. K. Humphrey's suggestion that "memes should be considered as living structures, not just metaphorically", and proposed to regard memes as "physically residing in the brain". Although Dawkins said his original intentions had been simpler, he approved Humphrey's opinion and he endorsed Susan Blackmore's 1999 project to give a scientific theory of memes, complete with predictions and empirical support.

Etymology

The term meme is a shortening (modeled on gene) of mimeme, which comes from Ancient Greek mīmēma (μίμημα; pronounced [míːmɛːma]), meaning 'imitated thing', itself from mimeisthai (μιμεῖσθαι, 'to imitate'), from mimos (μῖμος, 'mime').

The word was coined by British evolutionary biologist Richard Dawkins in The Selfish Gene (1976) as a concept for discussion of evolutionary principles in explaining the spread of ideas and cultural phenomena. Examples of memes given in Dawkins' book include melodies, catchphrases, fashion, and the technology of building arches.

Origins

Richard Dawkins coined the word meme in his 1976 book The Selfish Gene.

Early formulations

Although Richard Dawkins invented the term meme and developed meme theory, he has not claimed that the idea was entirely novel, and there have been other expressions for similar ideas in the past.

For instance, the possibility that ideas were subject to the same pressures of evolution as were biological attributes was discussed in the time of Charles Darwin. T. H. Huxley (1880) claimed that "The struggle for existence holds as much in the intellectual as in the physical world. A theory is a species of thinking, and its right to exist is coextensive with its power of resisting extinction by its rivals." G. K. Chesterton (1922) observed the similarity between intellectual systems and living organisms, noting that a certain degree of complexity, rather than being a hindrance, is a necessity for continued survival.

In 1904, Richard Semon published Die Mneme (which appeared in English in 1924 as The Mneme). The term mneme was also used in Maurice Maeterlinck's The Life of the White Ant (1926), with some parallels to Dawkins's concept. Kenneth Pike had, in 1954, coined the related terms emic and etic, generalizing the linguistic units of phoneme, morpheme, grapheme, lexeme, and tagmeme (as set out by Leonard Bloomfield), distinguishing insider and outside views of communicative behavior.

Dawkins

The word meme originated with Richard Dawkins' 1976 book The Selfish Gene.

Dawkins cites as inspiration the work of geneticist L. L. Cavalli-Sforza, anthropologist F. T. Cloak, and ethologist J. M. Cullen. Dawkins wrote that evolution depended not on the particular chemical basis of genetics, but only on the existence of a self-replicating unit of transmission—in the case of biological evolution, the gene. For Dawkins, the meme exemplified another self-replicating unit with potential significance in explaining human behavior and cultural evolution.

"Kilroy was here" was a graffito that became popular in the 1940s, and existed under various names in different countries, illustrating how a meme can be modified through replication. This is seen as one of the first widespread memes in the world.

Dawkins used the term to refer to any cultural entity that an observer might consider a replicator. He hypothesized that one could view many cultural entities as replicators, and pointed to melodies, fashions and learned skills as examples. Memes generally replicate through exposure to humans, who have evolved as efficient copiers of information and behavior. Because humans do not always copy memes perfectly, and because they may refine, combine or otherwise modify them with other memes to create new memes, they can change over time. Dawkins likened the process by which memes survive and change through the evolution of culture to the natural selection of genes in biological evolution.

Dawkins noted that in a society with culture a person need not have biological descendants to remain influential in the actions of individuals thousands of years after their death:

But if you contribute to the world's culture, if you have a good idea...it may live on, intact, long after your genes have dissolved in the common pool. Socrates may or may not have a gene or two alive in the world today, as G.C. Williams has remarked, but who cares? The meme-complexes of Socrates, Leonardo, Copernicus and Marconi are still going strong.

In that context, Dawkins defined the meme as a unit of cultural transmission, or a unit of imitation and replication, but later definitions would vary. The lack of a consistent, rigorous, and precise understanding of what typically makes up one unit of cultural transmission remains a problem in debates about memetics. In contrast, the concept of genetics gained concrete evidence with the discovery of the biological functions of DNA. Meme transmission requires a physical medium, such as photons, sound waves, touch, taste, or smell because memes can be transmitted only through the senses.

After Dawkins: Role of physical media

Initially, Dawkins did not seriously give context to the material of memetics. He considered a meme to be an idea, and thus a mental concept. However, from Dawkins' initial conception, it is how a medium might function in relation to the meme which has garnered the most attention. For example, David Hull suggested that while memes might exist as Dawkins conceives of them, he finds it important to suggest that instead of determining them as idea "replicators" (i.e. mind-determinant influences) one might notice that the medium itself has an influence in the meme's evolutionary outcomes. Thus, he refers to the medium as an "interactor" to avoid this determinism. Alternatively, Daniel Dennett suggests that the medium and the idea are not distinct in that memes only exist because of their medium. Dennett argued this in order to remain consistent with his denial of qualia and the notion of materially deterministic evolution which was consistent with Dawkins' account. A particularly more divergent theory is that of Limor Shifman, a communication and media scholar of "Internet memetics". She argues that any memetic argument which claims the distinction between the meme and the meme-vehicle (i.e. the meme's medium) are empirically observable is mistaken from the offset. Shifman claims to be following a similar theoretical direction as Susan Blackmore; however, her attention to the media surrounding Internet culture has enabled Internet memetic research to depart in empirical interests from previous memetic goals. Regardless of Internet Memetic's divergence in theoretical interests, it plays a significant role in theorizing and empirically investigating the connection between cultural ideologies, behaviors, and their mediation processes.

Memetic lifecycle: transmission, retention

See also: Diffusion of innovations
Imitating the cover of the Beatles album Abbey Road (1969), on which the band members cross the road in front of the Abbey Road Studios in a row, has become popular with fans and London visitors.
The four actresses of the Japanese media franchise Milky Holmes reenact the Beatles cover in 2010, extending the original Beatles meme by their film costumes.
In 2011, four cosplayers imitate the above meme during the manga convention Paris Manga 2012 at a zebra crossing in Paris, thus further separating the meme from the root situation of 1969 tied to the Abbey Road zebra crossing.

Memes, analogously to genes, vary in their aptitude to replicate; successful memes remain and spread, whereas unfit ones stall and are forgotten. Thus, memes that prove more effective at replicating and surviving are selected in the meme pool.

Memes first need retention. The longer a meme stays in its hosts, the higher its chances of propagation are. When a host uses a meme, the meme's life is extended. The reuse of the neural space hosting a certain meme's copy to host different memes is the greatest threat to that meme's copy. A meme that increases the longevity of its hosts will generally survive longer. On the contrary, a meme that shortens the longevity of its hosts will tend to disappear faster. However, as hosts are mortal, retention is not sufficient to perpetuate a meme in the long term; memes also need transmission.

Life-forms can transmit information both vertically (from parent to child, via replication of genes) and horizontally (through viruses and other means). Memes can replicate vertically or horizontally within a single biological generation. They may also lie dormant for long periods of time.

Memes reproduce by copying from a nervous system to another one, either by communication or imitation. Imitation often involves the copying of an observed behavior of another individual. Communication may be direct or indirect, where memes transmit from one individual to another through a copy recorded in an inanimate source, such as a book or a musical score. Adam McNamara has suggested that memes can be thereby classified as either internal or external memes (i-memes or e-memes).

Some commentators have likened the transmission of memes to the spread of contagions. Social contagions such as fads, hysteria, copycat crime, and copycat suicide exemplify memes seen as the contagious imitation of ideas. Observers distinguish the contagious imitation of memes from instinctively contagious phenomena such as yawning and laughing, which they consider innate (rather than socially learned) behaviors.

Aaron Lynch described seven general patterns of meme transmission, or "thought contagion":

  1. Quantity of parenthood: an idea that influences the number of children one has. Children respond particularly receptively to the ideas of their parents, and thus ideas that directly or indirectly encourage a higher birth rate will replicate themselves at a higher rate than those that discourage higher birth rates.
  2. Efficiency of parenthood: an idea that increases the proportion of children who will adopt ideas of their parents. Cultural separatism exemplifies one practice in which one can expect a higher rate of meme-replication—because the meme for separation creates a barrier from exposure to competing ideas.
  3. Proselytic: ideas generally passed to others beyond one's own children. Ideas that encourage the proselytism of a meme, as seen in many religious or political movements, can replicate memes horizontally through a given generation, spreading more rapidly than parent-to-child meme-transmissions do.
  4. Preservational: ideas that influence those that hold them to continue to hold them for a long time. Ideas that encourage longevity in their hosts, or leave their hosts particularly resistant to abandoning or replacing these ideas, enhance the preservability of memes and afford protection from the competition or proselytism of other memes.
  5. Adversative: ideas that influence those that hold them to attack or sabotage competing ideas and/or those that hold them. Adversative replication can give an advantage in meme transmission when the meme itself encourages aggression against other memes.
  6. Cognitive: ideas perceived as cogent by most in the population who encounter them. Cognitively transmitted memes depend heavily on a cluster of other ideas and cognitive traits already widely held in the population, and thus usually spread more passively than other forms of meme transmission. Memes spread in cognitive transmission do not count as self-replicating.
  7. Motivational: ideas that people adopt because they perceive some self-interest in adopting them. Strictly speaking, motivationally transmitted memes do not self-propagate, but this mode of transmission often occurs in association with memes self-replicated in the efficiency parental, proselytic and preservational modes.

Memes as discrete units

Dawkins initially defined meme as a noun that "conveys the idea of a unit of cultural transmission, or a unit of imitation". John S. Wilkins retained the notion of meme as a kernel of cultural imitation while emphasizing the meme's evolutionary aspect, defining the meme as "the least unit of sociocultural information relative to a selection process that has favorable or unfavorable selection bias that exceeds its endogenous tendency to change". The meme as a unit provides a convenient means of discussing "a piece of thought copied from person to person", regardless of whether that thought contains others inside it, or forms part of a larger meme. A meme could consist of a single word, or a meme could consist of the entire speech in which that word first occurred. This forms an analogy to the idea of a gene as a single unit of self-replicating information found on the self-replicating chromosome.

While the identification of memes as "units" conveys their nature to replicate as discrete, indivisible entities, it does not imply that thoughts somehow become quantized or that "atomic" ideas exist that cannot be dissected into smaller pieces. A meme has no given size. Susan Blackmore writes that melodies from Beethoven's symphonies are commonly used to illustrate the difficulty involved in delimiting memes as discrete units. She notes that while the first four notes of Beethoven's Fifth Symphony (listen) form a meme widely replicated as an independent unit, one can regard the entire symphony as a single meme as well.

The inability to pin an idea or cultural feature to quantifiable key units is widely acknowledged as a problem for memetics. It has been argued however that the traces of memetic processing can be quantified utilizing neuroimaging techniques which measure changes in the "connectivity profiles between brain regions". Blackmore meets such criticism by stating that memes compare with genes in this respect: that while a gene has no particular size, nor can we ascribe every phenotypic feature directly to a particular gene, it has value because it encapsulates that key unit of inherited expression subject to evolutionary pressures. To illustrate, she notes evolution selects for the gene for features such as eye color; it does not select for the individual nucleotide in a strand of DNA. Memes play a comparable role in understanding the evolution of imitated behaviors.

Genes, Mind, and Culture: The Coevolutionary Process (1981) by Charles J. Lumsden and E. O. Wilson proposes the theory that genes and culture co-evolve, and that the fundamental biological units of culture must correspond to neuronal networks that function as nodes of semantic memory. Lumsden and Wilson coined their own word, culturgen, which did not catch on. Coauthor Wilson later acknowledged the term meme as the best label for the fundamental unit of cultural inheritance in his 1998 book Consilience: The Unity of Knowledge, which elaborates upon the fundamental role of memes in unifying the natural and social sciences.

At present, the existence of discrete cultural units which satisfy memetic theory has been challenged in a variety of ways. What is critical from this perspective is that in denying memetics unitary status is to deny a particularly fundamental part of Dawkins' original argument. In particular, denying memes are a unit, or are explainable in some clear unitary structure denies the cultural analogy that inspired Dawkins to define them. If memes are not describable as unitary, memes are not accountable within a neo-Darwinian model of evolutionary culture.

Within cultural anthropology, materialist approaches are skeptical of such units. In particular, Dan Sperber argues that memes are not unitary in the sense that there are no two instances of exactly the same cultural idea, all that can be argued is that there is material mimicry of an idea. Thus every instance of a "meme" would not be a true evolutionary unit of replication.

Dan Deacon, Kalevi Kull separately argued memes are degenerate Signs in that they offer only a partial explanation of the triadic in Charles Sanders Peirce's semiotic theory: a sign (a reference to an object), an object (the thing being referred to), and an interpretant (the interpreting actor of a sign). They argue the meme unit is a sign which only is defined by its replication ability. Accordingly, in the broadest sense, the objects of copying are memes, whereas the objects of translation and interpretation are signs. Later, Sara Cannizzaro more fully develops out this semiotic relation in order to reframe memes as being a kind of semiotic activity, however she too denies that memes are units, referring to them as "sign systems" instead.

In Limor Shifman's account of Internet memetics, she also denies memetics as being unitary. She argues memes are not unitary, however many assume they are because many previous memetic researchers confounded memes with the cultural interest in "virals": singular informational objects which spread with a particular rate and veracity such as a video or a picture. As such, Shifman argues that Dawkins' original notion of meme is closer to what communication and information studies consider digitally viral replication.

Evolutionary influences on memes

Dawkins noted the three conditions that must exist for evolution to occur:

  1. variation, or the introduction of new change to existing elements;
  2. heredity or replication, or the capacity to create copies of elements;
  3. differential "fitness", or the opportunity for one element to be more or less suited to the environment than another.

Dawkins emphasizes that the process of evolution naturally occurs whenever these conditions co-exist, and that evolution does not apply only to organic elements such as genes. He regards memes as also having the properties necessary for evolution, and thus sees meme evolution as not simply analogous to genetic evolution, but as a real phenomenon subject to the laws of natural selection. Dawkins noted that as various ideas pass from one generation to the next, they may either enhance or detract from the survival of the people who obtain those ideas, or influence the survival of the ideas themselves. For example, a certain culture may develop unique designs and methods of tool-making that give it a competitive advantage over another culture. Each tool-design thus acts somewhat similarly to a biological gene in that some populations have it and others do not, and the meme's function directly affects the presence of the design in future generations. In keeping with the thesis that in evolution one can regard organisms simply as suitable "hosts" for reproducing genes, Dawkins argues that one can view people as "hosts" for replicating memes. Consequently, a successful meme may or may not need to provide any benefit to its host.

Unlike genetic evolution, memetic evolution can show both Darwinian and Lamarckian traits. Cultural memes will have the characteristic of Lamarckian inheritance when a host aspires to replicate the given meme through inference rather than by exactly copying it. Take for example the case of the transmission of a simple skill such as hammering a nail, a skill that a learner imitates from watching a demonstration without necessarily imitating every discrete movement modeled by the teacher in the demonstration, stroke for stroke. Susan Blackmore distinguishes the difference between the two modes of inheritance in the evolution of memes, characterizing the Darwinian mode as "copying the instructions" and the Lamarckian as "copying the product".

Clusters of memes, or memeplexes (also known as meme complexes or as memecomplexes), such as cultural or political doctrines and systems, may also play a part in the acceptance of new memes. Memeplexes comprise groups of memes that replicate together and coadapt. Memes that fit within a successful memeplex may gain acceptance by "piggybacking" on the success of the memeplex. As an example, John D. Gottsch discusses the transmission, mutation and selection of religious memeplexes and the theistic memes contained. Theistic memes discussed include the "prohibition of aberrant sexual practices such as incest, adultery, homosexuality, bestiality, castration, and religious prostitution", which may have increased vertical transmission of the parent religious memeplex. Similar memes are thereby included in the majority of religious memeplexes, and harden over time; they become an "inviolable canon" or set of dogmas, eventually finding their way into secular law. This could also be referred to as the propagation of a taboo.

Memetics

Main article: Memetics

Memetics is the name of the field of science that studies memes and their evolution and culture spread. While the term "meme" appeared in various forms in German and Austrian texts near the turn of the 20th century, Dawkin's unrelated use of the term in The Selfish Gene marked its emergence into mainstream study. Based on the Dawkin's framing of a meme as a cultural analogue to a gene, meme theory originated as an attempt to apply biological evolutionary principles to cultural information transfer and cultural evolution. Thus, memetics attempts to apply conventional scientific methods (such as those used in population genetics and epidemiology) to explain existing patterns and transmission of cultural ideas.

Principal criticisms of memetics include the claim that memetics ignores established advances in other fields of cultural study, such as sociology, cultural anthropology, cognitive psychology, and social psychology. Questions remain whether or not the meme concept counts as a validly disprovable scientific theory. This view regards memetics as a theory in its infancy: a protoscience to proponents, or a pseudoscience to some detractors.

Criticism of meme theory

One frequent criticism of meme theory looks at the perceived gap in the gene/meme analogy. For example, Luis Benitez-Bribiesca points to the lack of a "code script" for memes (analogous to the DNA of genes), and to the excessive instability of the meme mutation mechanism (that of an idea going from one brain to another), which would lead to a low replication accuracy and a high mutation rate, rendering the evolutionary process chaotic. In his book Darwin's Dangerous Idea, Daniel C. Dennett points to the existence of self-regulating correction mechanisms (vaguely resembling those of gene transcription) enabled by the redundancy and other properties of most meme expression languages which stabilize information transfer. Dennett notes that spiritual narratives, including music and dance forms, can survive in full detail across any number of generations even in cultures with oral tradition only. In contrast, when applying only meme theory, memes for which stable copying methods are available will inevitably get selected for survival more often than those which can only have unstable mutations (such as the noted music and dance forms), which, according to meme theory, should have resulted in those forms of cultural expression going extinct.

A second common criticism of meme theory views it as a reductionist and inadequate version of more accepted anthropological theories. Kim Sterelny and Paul Griffiths noted the cumulative evolution of genes depends on biological selection-pressures neither too great nor too small in relation to mutation-rates, while pointing out there is no reason to think that the same balance will exist in the selection pressures on memes. Semiotic theorists such as Terrence Deacon and Kalevi Kull regard the concept of a meme as a primitivized or degenerate concept of a sign, containing only a sign's basic ability to be copied, but lacks other core elements of the sign concept such as translation and interpretation. Evolutionary biologist Ernst Mayr similarly disapproved of Dawkins's gene-based view of meme, asserting it to be an "unnecessary synonym" for a concept, reasoning that concepts are not restricted to an individual or a generation, may persist for long periods of time, and may evolve.

Applications

Opinions differ as to how best to apply the concept of memes within a "proper" disciplinary framework. One view sees memes as providing a useful philosophical perspective with which to examine cultural evolution. Proponents of this view (such as Susan Blackmore and Daniel Dennett) argue that considering cultural developments from a meme's-eye view—as if memes themselves respond to pressure to maximise their own replication and survival—can lead to useful insights and yield valuable predictions into how culture develops over time. Others such as Bruce Edmonds and Robert Aunger have focused on the need to provide an empirical grounding for memetics to become a useful and respected scientific discipline.

A third approach, described by Joseph Poulshock, as "radical memetics" seeks to place memes at the centre of a materialistic theory of mind and of personal identity.

Prominent researchers in evolutionary psychology and anthropology, including Scott Atran, Dan Sperber, Pascal Boyer, John Tooby and others, argue the possibility of incompatibility between modularity of mind and memetics. In their view, minds structure certain communicable aspects of the ideas produced, and these communicable aspects generally trigger or elicit ideas in other minds through inference (to relatively rich structures generated from often low-fidelity input) and not high-fidelity replication or imitation. Atran discusses communication involving religious beliefs as a case in point. In one set of experiments he asked religious people to write down on a piece of paper the meanings of the Ten Commandments. Despite the subjects' own expectations of consensus, interpretations of the commandments showed wide ranges of variation, with little evidence of consensus. In another experiment, subjects with autism and subjects without autism interpreted ideological and religious sayings (for example, "Let a thousand flowers bloom" or "To everything there is a season"). People with autism showed a significant tendency to closely paraphrase and repeat content from the original statement (for example: "Don't cut flowers before they bloom"). Controls tended to infer a wider range of cultural meanings with little replicated content (for example: "Go with the flow" or "Everyone should have equal opportunity"). Only the subjects with autism—who lack the degree of inferential capacity normally associated with aspects of theory of mind—came close to functioning as "meme machines".

In his book The Robot's Rebellion, Keith Stanovich uses the memes and memeplex concepts to describe a program of cognitive reform that he refers to as a "rebellion". Specifically, Stanovich argues that the use of memes as a descriptor for cultural units is beneficial because it serves to emphasize transmission and acquisition properties that parallel the study of epidemiology. These properties make salient the sometimes parasitic nature of acquired memes, and as a result individuals should be motivated to reflectively acquire memes using what he calls a "Neurathian bootstrap" process.

Memetic explanations of racism

In Cultural Software: A Theory of Ideology, Jack Balkin argued that memetic processes can explain many of the most familiar features of ideological thought. His theory of "cultural software" maintained that memes form narratives, social networks, metaphoric and metonymic models, and a variety of different mental structures. Balkin maintains that the same structures used to generate ideas about free speech or free markets also serve to generate racistic beliefs. To Balkin, whether memes become harmful or maladaptive depends on the environmental context in which they exist rather than in any special source or manner to their origination. Balkin describes racist beliefs as "fantasy" memes that become harmful or unjust "ideologies" when diverse peoples come together, as through trade or competition.

Religion

See also: Evolutionary psychology of religion

Richard Dawkins called for a re-analysis of religion in terms of the evolution of self-replicating ideas apart from any resulting biological advantages they might bestow.

As an enthusiastic Darwinian, I have been dissatisfied with explanations that my fellow-enthusiasts have offered for human behaviour. They have tried to look for 'biological advantages' in various attributes of human civilization. For instance, tribal religion has been seen as a mechanism for solidifying group identity, valuable for a pack-hunting species whose individuals rely on cooperation to catch large and fast prey. Frequently the evolutionary preconception in terms of which such theories are framed is implicitly group-selectionist, but it is possible to rephrase the theories in terms of orthodox gene selection.

— Richard Dawkins, The Selfish Gene

He argued that the role of key replicator in cultural evolution belongs not to genes, but to memes replicating thought from person to person by means of imitation. These replicators respond to selective pressures that may or may not affect biological reproduction or survival.

In her book The Meme Machine, Susan Blackmore regards religions as particularly tenacious memes. Many of the features common to the most widely practiced religions provide built-in advantages in an evolutionary context, she writes. For example, religions that preach of the value of faith over evidence from everyday experience or reason inoculate societies against many of the most basic tools people commonly use to evaluate their ideas. By linking altruism with religious affiliation, religious memes can proliferate more quickly because people perceive that they can reap societal as well as personal rewards. The longevity of religious memes improves with their documentation in revered religious texts.

Aaron Lynch attributed the robustness of religious memes in human culture to the fact that such memes incorporate multiple modes of meme transmission. Religious memes pass down the generations from parent to child and across a single generation through the meme-exchange of proselytism. Most people will hold the religion taught them by their parents throughout their life. Many religions feature adversarial elements, punishing apostasy, for instance, or demonizing infidels. In Thought Contagion Lynch identifies the memes of transmission in Christianity as especially powerful in scope. Believers view the conversion of non-believers both as a religious duty and as an act of altruism. The promise of heaven to believers and threat of hell to non-believers provide a strong incentive for members to retain their belief. Lynch asserts that belief in the Crucifixion of Jesus in Christianity amplifies each of its other replication advantages through the indebtedness believers have to their Savior for sacrifice on the cross. The image of the crucifixion recurs in religious sacraments, and the proliferation of symbols of the cross in homes and churches potently reinforces the wide array of Christian memes.

Although religious memes have proliferated in human cultures, the modern scientific community has been relatively resistant to religious belief. Robertson (2007) reasoned that if evolution is accelerated in conditions of propagative difficulty, then we would expect to encounter variations of religious memes, established in general populations, addressed to scientific communities. Using a memetic approach, Robertson deconstructed two attempts to privilege religiously held spirituality in scientific discourse. Advantages of a memetic approach as compared to more traditional "modernization" and "supply side" theses in understanding the evolution and propagation of religion were explored.

Architectural memes

In A Theory of Architecture, Nikos Salingaros speaks of memes as "freely propagating clusters of information" which can be beneficial or harmful. He contrasts memes to patterns and true knowledge, characterizing memes as "greatly simplified versions of patterns" and as "unreasoned matching to some visual or mnemonic prototype". Taking reference to Dawkins, Salingaros emphasizes that they can be transmitted due to their own communicative properties, that "the simpler they are, the faster they can proliferate", and that the most successful memes "come with a great psychological appeal".

Architectural memes, according to Salingaros, can have destructive power: "Images portrayed in architectural magazines representing buildings that could not possibly accommodate everyday uses become fixed in our memory, so we reproduce them unconsciously." He lists various architectural memes that circulated since the 1920s and which, in his view, have led to contemporary architecture becoming quite decoupled from human needs. They lack connection and meaning, thereby preventing "the creation of true connections necessary to our understanding of the world". He sees them as no different from antipatterns in software design—as solutions that are false but are re-utilized nonetheless.

Internet culture

Main article: Internet meme See also: List of Internet phenomena

An "Internet meme" is a concept that spreads rapidly from person to person via the Internet. Memes can spread from person to person via social networks, blogs, direct email, or news sources. Sending memes as a form of affection is known as pebbling.

In 2013, Dawkins characterized an Internet meme as one deliberately altered by human creativity, distinguished from his original idea involving mutation "by random change and a form of Darwinian selection".

Internet memes are an example of Dawkins' meme theory at work in the sense of how they so rapidly mirror current cultural events and become a part of how the time period is defined. Limor Shifman uses the example of the 'Gangnam Style' Music video by South Korean pop-star, Psy that went viral in 2012. Shifman cites examples of how the meme mutated itself into the cultural sphere, mixing with other things going on at the time such as the 2012 U.S. presidential election, which led to the creation of Mitt Romney Style, a parody of the original Gangnam style, intended to be a jab at the 2012 Republican presidential candidate, Mitt Romney.

Meme stocks

Main article: Meme stock

Meme stocks, a particular subset of Internet memes in general, are listed companies lauded for the social media buzz they create, rather than their operating performance. Meme stocks find themselves surging in popularity after gaining the interest of individuals or groups through the internet. r/wallstreetbets, a subreddit where participants discuss stock and option trading, and the financial services company Robinhood Markets, became notable in 2021 for their involvement on the popularization and enhancement of meme stocks. One of the most commonly recognized instances of a meme stock is GameStop, whose stocks saw a sudden increase after a Reddit-led idea to invest in 2021.

Politics

In the United States the presidential campaigns have utilized memes on the Internet in the last three cycles. Disinformation has been charged by political contestants with memes being a concern of the complaints.

See also

Notes

  1. "meme". Oxford Dictionaries. Archived from the original on 23 May 2019. Retrieved 30 December 2017.
  2. "Meme". Cambridge Dictionary. 2023. Archived from the original on 18 March 2021. Retrieved 8 October 2023.
  3. "meme noun". Oxford Learner's Dictionaries. 2019. Archived from the original on 20 May 2019. Retrieved 30 December 2017.
  4. Meme Archived 21 September 2018 at the Wayback Machine. Merriam-Webster Dictionary.
  5. Graham 2002
  6. ^ Shifman, Limor (2014). Memes in Digital Culture. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press. ISBN 9781461947332. OCLC 860711989. Archived from the original on 22 June 2022. Retrieved 20 June 2022.
  7. Miltner, Kate M. (2018). "Internet Memes". The Sage Handbook of Social Media. Sage Publications. pp. 412–428. doi:10.4135/9781473984066.n23. ISBN 9781412962292. Archived from the original on 20 June 2022. Retrieved 20 June 2022.
  8. ^ Dawkins, Richard (2006). The Selfish Gene 30th Anniversary Edition (3rd ed.). Oxford University Press. p. 199. ISBN 9780191537554.
  9. Kelly 1994, p. 360 "But if we consider culture as its own self-organizing system—a system with its own agenda and pressure to survive—then the history of humanity gets even more interesting. As Richard Dawkins has shown, systems of self-replicating ideas or memes can quickly accumulate their own agenda and behaviours. I assign no higher motive to a cultural entity than the primitive drive to reproduce itself and modify its environment to aid its spread. One way the self organizing system can do this is by consuming human biological resources."
  10. Heylighen & Chielens 2009
  11. ^ McNamara 2011
  12. Gill, Jameson (2011). "Memes and narrative analysis: A potential direction for the development of neo-Darwinian orientated research in organisations" (PDF). EURAM 11: Proceedings of the European Academy of Management. European Academy of Management: 0–30. ISSN 2466-7498. S2CID 54894144. Archived (PDF) from the original on 10 October 2021. Retrieved 5 April 2022.
  13. Burman, J. T. (2012). "The misunderstanding of memes: Biography of an unscientific object, 1976–1999". Perspectives on Science. 20 (1): 75–104. doi:10.1162/POSC_a_00057. ISSN 1063-6145. S2CID 57569644.
  14. ^ Dawkins 1989, p. 192 "We need a name for the new replicator, a noun that conveys the idea of a unit of cultural transmission, or a unit of imitation. 'Mimeme' comes from a suitable Greek root, but I want a monosyllable that sounds a bit like 'gene'. I hope my classicist friends will forgive me if I abbreviate mimeme to meme. If it is any consolation, it could alternatively be thought of as being related to 'memory', or to the French word même. It should be pronounced to rhyme with 'cream'."
  15. Dawkins, Richard (1982). The Extended Phenotype. Oxford University Press. p. 109. ISBN 9780192860880.
  16. Dawkins's foreword to Blackmore 1999, p. xvi–xvii
  17. "meme". The American Heritage Dictionary of the English Language (4th ed.). 2000.
  18. Harper, Douglas. "meme". Online Etymology Dictionary.
  19. μίμημα, μιμεῖσθαι,μῖμος. Liddell, Henry George; Scott, Robert; A Greek–English Lexicon at the Perseus Project.
  20. Millikan 2004, p. 16. "Richard Dawkins invented the term 'memes' to stand for items that are reproduced by imitation rather than reproduced genetically."
  21. ^ Dawkins 1989, p. 352
  22. Shalizi, Cosma Rohilla. "Memes". Center for the Study of Complex Systems. University of Michigan. Archived from the original on 11 June 2012. Retrieved 8 October 2021.
  23. ^ Laurent, John (1999). "A Note on the Origin of 'Memes'/'Mnemes'". Journal of Memetics. 3 (1): 14–19. Archived from the original on 13 April 2021.
  24. Huxley, T. H. (1880). "The coming of age of 'The origin of species'". Science. 1 (2): 15–20. doi:10.1126/science.os-1.2.15. PMID 17751948. S2CID 4061790.
  25. Chesterton, G. K. (1990) . "III. The Case for Complexity". In Marlin, R. P.; Rabatin, G. J.; Swan, J. L.; Sobran, J.; Azar, P.; Mysak, J.; Paine, R.; Marlin, B. D. (eds.). The Collected Works of G. K. Chesterton. Vol. III. San Francisco: Ignatius Press. pp. 37–40. ISBN 0-89870-310-7.
  26. Pike, Kenneth (1967) . Language in Relation to a Unified Theory of the Structure of Human Behavior (Revised ed.).
  27. Cloak, F. T. 1966. "Cultural microevolution". Research Previews 13(2): 7–10. (Also presented at the November, 1966 annual meeting of the American Anthropological Association.)
  28. Cloak, F. T. 1975. "Is a cultural ethology possible?" Human Ecology 3: 161–182. doi:10.1007/BF01531639.
  29. "The Selfish Gene: Chapter 11 – Summary & Analysis Archived 14 November 2021 at the Wayback Machine". LitCharts.
  30. Gardner, Martin (5 March 2000). "Kilroy Was Here". Los Angeles Times. Archived from the original on 11 October 2021. Retrieved 8 October 2021.
  31. ^ Blackmore 1999
  32. Hull, David L. (2001). "Taking memetics seriously: Memetics will be what we make it". In Aunger, Robert (ed.). Darwinizing Culture: The Status of Memetics as a Science (1st ed.). Oxford University Press. pp. 43–67. ISBN 9780192632449.
  33. Dennett, Daniel C. (2017). From Bacteria to Bach and Back: The Evolution of Minds. HighBridge Audio. ISBN 9781681684390. OCLC 957746925. Retrieved 11 January 2023.
  34. Shifman, Limor (2014). Memes in Digital Culture. MIT Press. ISBN 9781469063256. OCLC 929971523.
  35. Lankshear, Colin; Knobel, Michele (2019). "Memes, Macros, Meaning, and Menace: Some Trends in Internet Memes". The Journal of Communication and Media Studies. 4 (4): 43–57. doi:10.18848/2470-9247/CGP/v04i04/43-57. ISSN 2470-9247. S2CID 214369629. Archived from the original on 4 January 2023. Retrieved 11 January 2023.
  36. Heylighen, Francis. "Meme replication: The memetic life-cycle". Principia Cybernetica. Archived from the original on 4 October 2018. Retrieved 26 July 2013.
  37. R. Evers, John. "A justification of societal altruism according to the memetic application of Hamilton's rule". Archived from the original on 6 October 2018. Retrieved 26 July 2013.
  38. Blackmore 1998; "The term 'contagion' is often associated with memetics. We may say that certain memes are contagious, or more contagious than others."
  39. Blackmore 1998
  40. ^ Lynch 1996
  41. Wilkins, John S. (1998). "What's in a Meme? Reflections from the perspective of the history and philosophy of evolutionary biology". Journal of Memetics. 2. Archived from the original on 1 December 2009. Retrieved 13 December 2008.
  42. Wilson 1998
  43. Sperber, Dan (1998). Explaining culture : a naturalistic approach. Blackwell Publ. ISBN 0631200452. OCLC 247213620. Archived from the original on 17 March 2023. Retrieved 23 January 2023.
  44. Deacon, Terrence W. (2004). "Memes as Signs in the Dynamic Logic of Semiosis: Beyond Molecular Science and Computation Theory". Conceptual Structures at Work. "Lecture Notes in Computer Science" series, no. 3127. Vol. 3127. Berlin / Heidelberg: Springer. pp. 17–30. doi:10.1007/978-3-540-27769-9_2. ISBN 9783540223924. Archived from the original on 17 March 2023. Retrieved 17 March 2023.
  45. Kull, Kalevi (2000). "Copy versus translate, meme versus sign: Development of Biological Textuality". European Journal for Semiotic Studies. 12 (1): 101–120. Archived from the original on 23 January 2023. Retrieved 23 January 2023.
  46. Cannizzaro, Sara (31 December 2016). "Internet memes as internet signs: A semiotic view of digital culture". Sign Systems Studies. 44 (4): 562–586. doi:10.12697/SSS.2016.44.4.05. ISSN 1736-7409. S2CID 53374867. Archived from the original on 1 February 2023. Retrieved 23 January 2023.
  47. Nahon, Karine; Hemsley, Jeff (2013). Going viral. Cambridge, England: Polity Press. ISBN 9780745671284. OCLC 849213692. Archived from the original on 17 March 2023. Retrieved 23 January 2023.
  48. ^ Dennett 1991
  49. Dawkins 2004
  50. Gottsch, John D. (2001). "Mutation, Selection, And Vertical Transmission Of Theistic Memes In Religious Canons". Journal of Memetics. 5 (1). Archived from the original on 12 April 2021. Retrieved 8 October 2021.
  51. Heylighen & Chielens 2009.
  52. Shifman, Limor (2013). "Memes in a Digital World: Reconciling with a Conceptual Troublemaker". Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication. 18 (3): 362–377. doi:10.1111/jcc4.12013. hdl:11059/14843. Retrieved 1 May 2023.
  53. Petrova, Yulia (2021). "Meme language, its impact on digital culture and collective thinking". E3S Web of Conferences. 273: 11026. Bibcode:2021E3SWC.27311026P. doi:10.1051/e3sconf/202127311026. ISSN 2267-1242. S2CID 237986424.
  54. Benítez-Bribiesca, Luis (January 2001). "MEMETICS: A DANGEROUS IDEA". Interciencia. 26 (1). ProQuest 210137265 – via Research Library.
  55. Benitez Bribiesca, Luis (January 2001). "Memetics: A Dangerous Idea" (PDF). Interciencia: Revista de Ciencia y Technologia de América. 26 (1): 29–31. ISSN 0378-1844. Archived (PDF) from the original on 20 September 2018. Retrieved 11 February 2010. If the mutation rate is high and takes place over short periods, as memetics predict, instead of selection, adaptation and survival a chaotic disintegration occurs due to the accumulation of errors.
  56. Dennett, Daniel C. (1995). Darwin's Dangerous Idea: Evolution and the Meanings of Life. New York: Simon and Schuster.
  57. Fracchia, Joseph; Lewontin, Richard (February 2005). "The price of metaphor". History and Theory. 44 (1): 14–29. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2303.2005.00305.x. ISSN 0018-2656. JSTOR 3590779. The selectionist paradigm requires the reduction of society and culture to inheritance systems that consist of randomly varying, individual units, some of which are selected, and some not; and with society and culture thus reduced to inheritance systems, history can be reduced to 'evolution.' ... We conclude that while historical phenomena can always be modeled selectionistically, selectionist explanations do no work, nor do they contribute anything new except a misleading vocabulary that anesthetizes history.
  58. Sterelny & Griffiths 1999; p. 333
  59. Deacon, Terrence. "The trouble with memes (and what to do about it)". The Semiotic Review of Books. 10: 3.
  60. Kull, Kalevi (2000). "Copy versus translate, meme versus sign: development of biological textuality". European Journal for Semiotic Studies. 12 (1): 101–120.
  61. Edmonds, Bruce (September 2002). "Three Challenges for the Survival of Memetics". Journal of Memetics. 6 (2). Archived from the original on 8 September 2021. Retrieved 8 October 2021.
  62. Aunger 2000
  63. Poulshock 2002
  64. Atran 2002
  65. Stanovich, Keith E. (2004). The Robot's Rebellion: Finding Meaning in the Age of Darwin. University of Chicago Press. ISBN 9780226770895.
  66. Balkin 1998
  67. Robertson, Lloyd Hawkeye (2007). "Reflections on the use of spirituality to privilege religion in scientific discourse: Incorporating considerations of self". Journal of Religion and Health. 46 (3): 449–461. doi:10.1007/s10943-006-9105-y. S2CID 39449795. Archived from the original on 8 March 2021. Retrieved 1 July 2019.
  68. Dennett, Daniel C. (1995). Darwin's Dangerous Idea: Evolution and the Meanings of Life. New York: Simon and Schuster.
  69. Salingaros 2008, pp. 243, 260.
  70. Salingaros 2008, pp. 243–245.
  71. Salingaros 2008, p. 249.
  72. Salingaros 2008, p. 259.
  73. Schubert, Karen (31 July 2003). "Bazaar goes bizarre". USA Today. Archived from the original on 2 July 2012. Retrieved 5 July 2007.
  74. Edelman, Amelia (26 June 2024). "Always sending memes to your loved ones? It's called 'pebbling.' Here's why experts say the trend has its pros and cons". Yahoo Life. Retrieved 12 July 2024.
  75. Solon, Olivia (20 June 2013). "Richard Dawkins on the internet's hijacking of the word 'meme'". Wired UK. Archived from the original on 9 July 2013.
  76. Pettis, Ben T. (19 August 2021). "Know your meme and the homogenization of Web history". Internet Histories. 1–17 (3): 263–279. doi:10.1080/24701475.2021.1968657. S2CID 238660211. Archived from the original on 17 March 2023. Retrieved 28 February 2023.
  77. Denisova, Anastasia (2019). Internet Memes and Society: Social, Cultural and Political Contexts. New York: Routledge. pp. 13–26. ISBN 9780429469404.
  78. Shifman, Limor (26 March 2013). "Memes in a Digital World: Reconciling with a Conceptual Troublemaker". Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication. 18 (3): 362–377. doi:10.1111/jcc4.12013. hdl:11059/14843.
  79. Rossolillo, Nicholas (23 September 2021). "What Are Meme Stocks?". The Motley Fool. Archived from the original on 13 November 2021. Retrieved 8 October 2021.
  80. Rossolillo, Nicholas. "Top Meme Stocks of 2023". The Motley Fool. Retrieved 23 April 2023.
  81. Phillips, Matt; Marcos, Coral Murphy (4 August 2021). "Robinhood's shares jump as much as 65 percent, like the meme stocks it enabled". The New York Times. ISSN 0362-4331. Retrieved 15 June 2023.
  82. Popper, Nathaniel; Browning, Kellen (29 January 2021). "The 'Roaring Kitty' Rally: How a Reddit User and His Friends Roiled the Markets". The New York Times. ISSN 0362-4331. Retrieved 15 June 2023.
  83. Brown, Abram. "Robinhood Stock's Surprise Supporters: Investors From The Reddit Group Who Hated The Company". Forbes. Retrieved 23 April 2023.
  84. Helmus, Todd C. Artificial Intelligence, Deepfakes, and Disinformation: A Primer. RAND Corporation, 2022. JSTOR, http://www.jstor.org/stable/resrep42027. Retrieved 17 Feb. 2024.
  85. Anderson, Karrin Vasb, and Kristina Horn Sheeler. “Texts (and Tweets) from Hillary: Meta-Meming and Postfeminist Political Culture.” Presidential Studies Quarterly, vol. 44, no. 2, 2014, pp. 224–43. JSTOR, http://www.jstor.org/stable/43286740. Accessed 17 Feb. 2024.

References

External links

Listen to this article (26 minutes)
Spoken Misplaced Pages iconThis audio file was created from a revision of this article dated 29 August 2019 (2019-08-29), and does not reflect subsequent edits.(Audio help · More spoken articles)
Richard Dawkins
Books
Related works
Documentaries
See also
Worldview
Related terms
Aspects
Biases
Change and
maintenance
Culture
Groupthink
Knowledge
Metaphysics
Value
Examples
Attitudes
Economic and
political ideologies
Religions
Schools of
philosophy
Portal: Categories: