Misplaced Pages

Sociobiology: Difference between revisions

Article snapshot taken from Wikipedia with creative commons attribution-sharealike license. Give it a read and then ask your questions in the chat. We can research this topic together.
Browse history interactively← Previous editContent deleted Content addedVisualWikitext
Revision as of 12:23, 19 July 2011 editMiradre (talk | contribs)9,214 edits Criticism: npov description← Previous edit Latest revision as of 18:41, 24 December 2024 edit undoDiscospinster (talk | contribs)Administrators465,293 edits remove author spammingTag: Manual revert 
(498 intermediate revisions by more than 100 users not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
{{Short description|Subdiscipline of biology regarding social behavior}}
{{Template:Evolutionary biology}}
{{for | the book by E. O. Wilson |Sociobiology: The New Synthesis}}
{{Evolutionary biology}}


'''Sociobiology''' is a field of ] study which is based on the assumption that ] has resulted from ] and attempts to explain and examine social behavior within that context. Often considered a branch of ] and ], it also draws from ], ], ], ], ], ], and other disciplines. Within the study of human ], sociobiology is very closely allied to the fields of ] and ]. '''Sociobiology''' is a field of ] that aims to explain ] in terms of ]. It draws from disciplines including ], ], ], ], ], ], and ]. Within the study of human ], sociobiology is closely allied to ], ], ],<ref>{{Citation|author=Wilson, Edward O.|author-link=Edward O. Wilson|title=The Social Insects|date=2000-03-24|work=Sociobiology|pages=397–437|publisher=Harvard University Press|doi=10.2307/j.ctvjnrttd.22|isbn=978-0-674-74416-5}}</ref> and ].<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Nielsen|first=François|date=1994|title=Sociobiology and Sociology|url=https://www.annualreviews.org/doi/10.1146/annurev.so.20.080194.001411 |journal=Annual Review of Sociology |language=en |volume=20 |issue=1|pages=267–303 |doi=10.1146/annurev.so.20.080194.001411 |issn=0360-0572}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal|last=de Sousa|first=Ronald|date=1990-01-01|title=The sociology of sociobiology|url=https://doi.org/10.1080/02698599008573367|journal=International Studies in the Philosophy of Science|volume=4|issue=3|pages=271–283|doi=10.1080/02698599008573367|issn=0269-8595}}</ref>


Sociobiology investigates social behaviors, such as mating patterns, territorial fights, pack hunting, and the hive society of social insects. It argues that just as selection pressure led to animals evolving useful ways of interacting with the natural environment, it led to the genetic evolution of advantageous social behavior. Sociobiology investigates social behaviors such as ], ], ], and the hive society of ]s. It argues that just as selection pressure led to animals evolving useful ways of interacting with the ], so also it led to the genetic evolution of advantageous social behavior.<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Freedman|first=Daniel G.|date=January 1985|title=Sociobiology and the human dimension|journal=Ethology and Sociobiology|volume=6|issue=2|pages=121–122|doi=10.1016/0162-3095(85)90006-8|issn=0162-3095}}</ref>


While the term "sociobiology" can be traced to the 1940s, the concept didn't gain major recognition until 1975 with the publication of ]'s book, '']''. The new field quickly became the subject of heated controversy. Criticism, most notably made by ] and ], centered on sociobiology's contention that genes play an ultimate role in human behavior and that traits such as aggressiveness can be explained by biology rather than a person's social environment. Sociobiologists generally responded to the criticism by pointing to the complex relationship between ]. In response to some of the potentially fractious implications sociobiology had on human biodiversity, anthropologist ] and psychologist ] founded the field ]. While the term "sociobiology" originated at least as early as the 1940s; the concept did not gain major recognition until the publication of ]'s book '']'' in 1975. The new field quickly became the subject of ]. Critics, led by ] and ], argued that genes played a role in human behavior, but that traits such as ] could be explained by social environment rather than by biology. Sociobiologists responded by pointing to the complex relationship between ]. Among sociobiologists, the controversy between laying weight to different levels of selection was settled between D.S. Wilson and E.O. Wilson in 2007.<ref name=":0"/>


==Definition== ==Definition==
E.O. Wilson defines sociobiology as: “The extension of population biology and evolutionary theory to social organisation”<ref>Wilson, E.O. (1978) ''On Human Nature'' Page x, Cambridge, Ma: Harvard</ref>


E. O. Wilson defined sociobiology as "the extension of population biology and evolutionary theory to social organization".<ref>{{cite book |author=Wilson, E. O. |author-link=E. O. Wilson |year=1978 |title=On Human Nature |url=https://archive.org/details/onhumannature00wils |url-access=registration |page=x |publisher=Harvard |isbn=978-0674016385}}</ref>
Sociobiology is based on the premise that some behaviors (both social and individual) are at least partly inherited and can be affected by ]. It begins with the idea that behaviors have evolved over time, similar to the way that physical traits are thought to have evolved. It predicts therefore that animals will act in ways that have proven to be evolutionarily successful over time, which can among other things result in the formation of complex social processes conducive to evolutionary fitness.


Sociobiology is based on the premise that some behaviors (social and individual) are at least partly inherited and can be affected by ].<ref>{{cite book |last1=Mohammed |first1=Sulma I. |last2=Alfarouk |first2=Khalid O. |last3=Elhassan |first3=Ahmed M. |last4=Hamad |first4=Kamal |last5=Ibrahim |first5=Muntaser E. |title=The Genetics of African Populations in Health and Disease |chapter=Sociobiological Transition and Cancer |date=2019 |pages=217–232 |doi=10.1017/9781139680295.010 |isbn=9781139680295 |s2cid=214321882 |chapter-url=https://www.cambridge.org/core/books/genetics-of-african-populations-in-health-and-disease/sociobiological-transition-and-cancer/E4FE8995823F33510961E6FDF1293F38 |language=en}}</ref> It begins with the idea that behaviors have evolved over time, similar to the way that physical traits are thought to have evolved. It predicts that animals will act in ways that have proven to be evolutionarily successful over time. This can, among other things, result in the formation of complex social processes conducive to evolutionary fitness.
The discipline seeks to explain behavior as a product of natural selection. Behavior is therefore seen as an effort to preserve one's genes in the population. Inherent in sociobiological reasoning is the idea that certain genes or gene combinations that influence particular behavioral traits can be ] from generation to generation.


The discipline seeks to explain behavior as a product of natural selection. Behavior is therefore seen as an effort to preserve one's genes in the population. Inherent in sociobiological reasoning is the idea that certain genes or gene combinations that influence particular behavioral traits can be ] from generation to generation.<ref name=":0">{{cite journal |title=Rethinking The Theoretical Foundation of Sociobiology | author1=Wilson, David Sloan |author2=Wilson, Edward O. |author1-link=David Sloan Wilson|author2-link=E. O. Wilson | journal=The Quarterly Review of Biology | year=2007 | volume=82 | issue=4 | pages=327–348 | doi=10.1086/522809 | pmid=18217526| s2cid=37774648 }}</ref>
==Introductory examples==
For example, newly dominant male lions often will kill cubs in the pride that were not sired by them. This behaviour is adaptive in evolutionary terms because killing the cubs eliminates competition for their own offspring and causes the nursing females to come into heat faster, thus allowing more of his genes to enter into the population. Sociobiologists would view this instinctual cub-killing behavior as being inherited through the genes of successfully reproducing male lions, whereas non-killing behaviour may have "died out" as those lions were less successful in reproducing.


For example, newly dominant male lions often kill cubs in the pride that they did not sire. This ] because killing the cubs eliminates ] for their own offspring and causes the nursing females to come into heat faster, thus allowing more of his genes to enter into the population. Sociobiologists would view this instinctual cub-killing behavior as being inherited through the genes of successfully reproducing male lions, whereas non-killing behavior may have died out as those lions were less successful in reproducing.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Packer |first1=Craig |last2=Pusey |first2=Anne E. |title=Adaptations of Female Lions to Infanticide by Incoming Males |journal=Am. Nat. |date=1983 |volume=121 |issue=5 |pages=716–728 |url=https://www.cbs.umn.edu/sites/cbs.umn.edu/files/public/downloads/Adaptations_of_female_lions_to_infanticide.pdf |doi=10.1086/284097 |bibcode=1983ANat..121..716P |s2cid=84927815 |archive-date=2015-12-29 |access-date=2017-04-03 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20151229213243/http://cbs.umn.edu/sites/cbs.umn.edu/files/public/downloads/Adaptations_of_female_lions_to_infanticide.pdf |url-status=dead }}</ref>
Genetic mouse mutants have now been harnessed to illustrate the power that genes exert on behaviour. For example, the transcription factor FEV (aka Pet1) has been shown, through its role in maintaining the serotonergic system in the brain, to be required for normal aggressive and anxiety-like behavior.<ref>Hendricks TJ, Fyodorov DV, Wegman LJ, Lelutiu NB, Pehek EA, Yamamoto B, Silver J, Weeber EJ, Sweatt JD, Deneris ES. Pet-1 ETS gene plays a critical role in 5-HT neuron development and is required for normal anxiety-like and aggressive behaviour. Neuron. 2003 Jan 23;37(2):233-47</ref> Thus, when FEV is genetically deleted from the mouse genome, male mice will instantly attack other males, whereas their wild-type counterparts take significantly longer to initiate violent behaviour. In addition, FEV has been shown to be required for correct maternal behaviour in mice, such that their offspring do not survive unless cross-fostered to other wild-type female mice.<ref>Lerch-Haner JK, Frierson D, Crawford LK, Beck SG, Deneris ES. Serotonergic transcriptional programming determines maternal behavior and offspring survival. Nat Neurosci. 2008 Sep;11(9):1001-3.</ref>

A genetic basis for instinctive behavioural traits among non-human species, such as in the above example, is commonly accepted among many biologists; however, attempting to use a genetic basis to explain complex behaviours in human societies has remained extremely controversial.


==History== ==History==
], a central figure in the history of sociobiology.]] ], a central figure in the history of sociobiology, from the publication in 1975 of his book '']'']]
According to the ], ] coined the word "sociobiology" at a 1946 conference on genetics and social behaviour, and became widely used after it was popularized by Edward O. Wilson in his 1975 book, ''Sociobiology: The New Synthesis''. However, the influence of evolution on behavior has been of interest to biologists and philosophers since soon after the discovery of evolution itself. ]'s '']'', written in the early 1890s, is a popular example. Antecedents of modern sociobiological thinking can be traced to the 1960s and the work of such biologists as ], ] and ].


The philosopher of biology ] suggested that the political philosopher ] was the first proto-sociobiologist, arguing that in his 1651 book '']'' Hobbes had explained the origins of morals in human society from an amoral sociobiological perspective.<ref>{{cite book |author=Dennett, Daniel |author-link=Daniel Dennett |title=Darwin's Dangerous Idea |url=https://archive.org/details/darwinsdangerous0000denn |url-access=registration |publisher=Simon and Schuster |year=1995 |pages= |isbn=978-0140167344}}</ref>
Nonetheless, it was Wilson's book that pioneered and popularized the attempt to explain the evolutionary mechanics behind social behaviors such as ], ], and nurturence, primarily in ants (Wilson's own research specialty) but also in other animals.{{Citation needed|date=August 2009}} The final chapter of the book is devoted to sociobiological explanations of human behavior, and Wilson later wrote a ] winning book, '']'', that addressed human behavior specifically.


The geneticist of animal behavior ] coined the word ''sociobiology'' at a 1948 conference on genetics and social behavior, which called for a conjoint development of field and laboratory studies in animal behavior research.<ref>{{cite web |title=The Life of J.P. Scott |url=https://www.bgsu.edu/arts-and-sciences/neuroscience/john-paul-scott/the-life-of-jp-scott.html |publisher=Bowling Green State University |access-date=14 December 2016}}</ref> With John Paul Scott's organizational efforts, a "Section of Animal Behavior and Sociobiology" of the Ecological Society of America was created in 1956, which became a Division of Animal Behavior of the American Society of Zoology in 1958. In 1956, ] came in contact with this emerging sociobiology through his PhD student Stuart A. Altmann, who had been in close relation with the participants to the 1948 conference. Altmann developed his own brand of sociobiology to study the social behavior of rhesus macaques, using statistics, and was hired as a "sociobiologist" at the Yerkes Regional Primate Research Center in 1965.
==Sociobiological theory==
Wilson's sociobiology is different from ]'s or Altmann's, insofar as he drew on mathematical models of social behavior centered on the maximization of the genetic fitness by ], ], ], and ]. The three sociobiologies by Scott, Altmann and Wilson have in common to place naturalist studies at the core of the research on animal social behavior and by drawing alliances with emerging research methodologies, at a time when "biology in the field" was threatened to be made old-fashioned by "modern" practices of science (laboratory studies, mathematical biology, molecular biology).<ref name=Dobzhansky1966>{{cite journal |last1=Dobzhansky |first1=Theodosius |title=Are Naturalists Old-Fashioned? |journal=The American Naturalist |date=September 1966 |volume=100 |issue=915 |pages=541–550 |doi=10.1086/282448|bibcode=1966ANat..100..541D |s2cid=129104506 }}</ref>
{{Sociology}}
Sociobiologists believe that ], as well as nonhuman animal behavior, can be partly explained as the outcome of natural selection. They contend that in order fully to understand behavior, it must be analyzed in terms of evolutionary considerations.


Once a specialist term, "sociobiology" became widely known in 1975 when Wilson published his book ''Sociobiology: The New Synthesis'', which sparked an intense controversy. Since then "sociobiology" has largely been equated with Wilson's vision. The book pioneered and popularized the attempt to explain the evolutionary mechanics behind social behaviors such as ], ], and nurturance, primarily in ants (Wilson's own research specialty) and other ], but also in other animals. However, the influence of evolution on behavior has been of interest to biologists and philosophers since soon after the discovery of evolution itself. ]'s '']'', written in the early 1890s, is a popular example. The final chapter of the book is devoted to sociobiological explanations of human behavior, and Wilson later wrote a ] winning book, '']'', that addressed human behavior specifically.<ref>{{cite magazine |author=Walsh, Bryan | url=https://entertainment.time.com/2011/08/30/all-time-100-best-nonfiction-books/slide/on-human-nature-by-edward-o-wilson/ | title=All-Time 100 Nonfiction Books | magazine=Time |date=17 August 2011}}</ref>
] is fundamental to evolutionary theory. Variants of hereditary traits which increase an organism's ability to survive and reproduce will be more greatly represented in subsequent generations, i.e., they will be "selected for". Thus, inherited behavioral mechanisms that allowed an ] a greater chance of surviving and/or reproducing in the past are more likely to survive in present organisms. That inherited adaptive behaviors are present in nonhuman ] has been multiply demonstrated by biologists, and it has become a foundation of evolutionary biology. However, there is continued resistance by some researchers over the application of evolutionary models to humans, particularly from within the social sciences, where culture has long been assumed to be the predominant driver of behavior.

Edward H. Hagen writes in ''The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology'' that sociobiology is, despite the public controversy regarding the applications to humans, "one of the scientific triumphs of the twentieth century." "Sociobiology is now part of the core research and curriculum of virtually all biology departments, and it is a foundation of the work of almost all field biologists.
" Sociobiological research on nonhuman organisms has increased dramatically and continuously in the world's top scientific journals such as '']'' and '']''. The more general term ] is commonly substituted for the term sociobiology in order to avoid the public controversy.<ref name="Evolutionary Psychology 2005">The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology, edited by David M. Buss, John Wiley & Sons, 2005. Chapter 5 by Edward H. Hagen</ref>

==Theory==

Sociobiologists maintain that ], as well as nonhuman animal behavior, can be partly explained as the outcome of natural selection. They contend that in order to fully understand behavior, it must be analyzed in terms of evolutionary considerations.

] is fundamental to evolutionary theory. Variants of hereditary traits which increase an organism's ability to survive and reproduce will be more greatly represented in subsequent generations, i.e., they will be "selected for". Thus, inherited behavioral mechanisms that allowed an ] a greater chance of surviving and/or reproducing in the past are more likely to survive in present organisms. That inherited adaptive behaviors are present in nonhuman ] has been multiply demonstrated by biologists, and it has become a foundation of ]. However, there is continued resistance by some researchers over the application of evolutionary models to humans, particularly from within the social sciences, where culture has long been assumed to be the predominant driver of behavior.

], whose work influenced sociobiology]]


Sociobiology is based upon two fundamental premises: Sociobiology is based upon two fundamental premises:
* Certain behavioral traits are inherited, * Certain behavioral traits are inherited,
* Inherited behavioral traits have been honed by natural selection. Therefore, these traits were probably "adaptive" in the species` evolutionarily evolved environment. * Inherited behavioral traits have been honed by natural selection. Therefore, these traits were probably "adaptive" in the environment in which the species evolved.


Sociobiology uses ]'s ] and explanations of animal behavior. Two categories are at the species level; two, at the individual level. The species-level categories (often called “ultimate explanations”) are Sociobiology uses ]'s ] and explanations of animal behavior. Two categories are at the species level; two, at the individual level. The species-level categories (often called "ultimate explanations") are
* the function (i.e., ]) that a behavior serves and * the function (i.e., ]) that a behavior serves and
* the evolutionary process (i.e., phylogeny) that resulted in this functionality. * the evolutionary process (i.e., ]) that resulted in this functionality.
The individual-level categories (often called “proximate explanations”) are The individual-level categories (often called "proximate explanations") are
* the development of the individual (i.e., ontogeny) and * the development of the individual (i.e., ]) and
* the proximate mechanism (e.g., brain anatomy and hormones). * the proximate mechanism (e.g., ] and ]s).


Sociobiologists are interested in how behavior can be explained logically as a result of selective pressures in the history of a species. Thus, they are often interested in ]ive, or ] behavior, and in explaining the similarities, rather than the differences, between cultures. For example, mothers within many species of ] – including humans – are very protective of their ]. Sociobiologists reason that this protective behavior likely evolved over time because it helped those individuals which had the characteristic to survive and reproduce. Over time, individuals who exhibited such protective behaviours would have had more surviving offspring than did those who did not display such behaviours, such that this parental protection would increase in frequency in the population. In this way, the social behavior is believed to have evolved in a fashion similar to other types of nonbehavioral ]s, such as (for example) fur or the sense of smell. Sociobiologists are interested in how behavior can be explained logically as a result of selective pressures in the history of a species. Thus, they are often interested in ]ive, or ] behavior, and in explaining the similarities, rather than the differences, between cultures. For example, mothers within many species of ] – including humans – are very protective of their ]. Sociobiologists reason that this protective behavior likely evolved over time because it helped the offspring of the individuals which had the characteristic to survive. This parental protection would increase in frequency in the population. The social behavior is believed to have evolved in a fashion similar to other types of nonbehavioral ]s, such as a coat of fur, or the sense of smell.


Individual genetic advantage often fails to explain certain social behaviors as a result of gene-centred selection, and evolution may also act upon ] (citation needed). The mechanisms responsible for ] employ ] and population statistics borrowed from ]. ] argued that altruistic individuals must reproduce their own altruistic genetic traits for altruism to survive. When altruists lavish their resources on non-altruists at the expense of their own kind, the altruists tend to die out and the others tend to grow. In other words, altruism is more likely to survive if altruists practice the ] that "charity begins at home." Individual genetic advantage fails to explain certain social behaviors as a result of gene-centred selection. E.O. Wilson argued that evolution may also act upon ].<ref>Wilson, 1975. Chapter 5. "Group Selection and Altruism"</ref> The mechanisms responsible for ] employ ] and population statistics borrowed from ]. Altruism is defined as "a concern for the welfare of others". If altruism is genetically determined, then altruistic individuals must reproduce their own altruistic genetic traits for altruism to survive, but when altruists lavish their resources on non-altruists at the expense of their own kind, the altruists tend to die out and the others tend to increase. An extreme example is a soldier losing his life trying to help a fellow soldier. This example raises the question of how altruistic genes can be passed on if this soldier dies without having any children.<ref>{{cite journal |last=Tessman |first=Irwin |title=Human altruism as a courtship display |journal=Forum |year=1995 |volume=74 |issue=1 |pages=157–158|doi=10.2307/3545685 |jstor=3545685 |bibcode=1995Oikos..74..157T }}</ref>


Within sociobiology, a social behavior is first explained as a sociobiological ] by finding an ] that matches the observed behavior. Stability of a strategy can be difficult to prove, but usually, a well-formed strategy will predict gene frequencies. The hypothesis can be supported by establishing a correlation between the gene frequencies predicted by the strategy, and those expressed in a population. However, such an approach can be methodologically problematic, as a statistical correlation could be due to ] if the measurement of gene frequency indirectly uses the same measurements that describe the strategy. Within sociobiology, a social behavior is first explained as a sociobiological ] by finding an ] that matches the observed behavior. Stability of a strategy can be difficult to prove, but usually, it will predict gene frequencies. The hypothesis can be supported by establishing a correlation between the gene frequencies predicted by the strategy, and those expressed in a population.


Altruism between ]s and littermates has been explained in such a way. Altruistic behavior in some animals has been correlated to the degree of ] shared between altruistic individuals. A quantitative description of ] by male harem-mating animals when the ] is displaced as well as ] female infanticide and fetal resorption are active areas of study. In general, females with more bearing opportunities may value offspring less, and may also arrange bearing opportunities to maximize the ] and protection from mates. Altruism between ]s and littermates has been explained in such a way. Altruistic behavior, behavior that increases the reproductive fitness of others at the apparent expense of the altruist, in some animals has been correlated to the degree of ] shared between altruistic individuals. A quantitative description of ] by male harem-mating animals when the ] is displaced as well as ] female infanticide and ] are active areas of study. In general, females with more bearing opportunities may value offspring less, and may also arrange bearing opportunities to maximize the ] and protection from mates.


An important concept in sociobiology is that temperamental traits within a gene pool and between gene pools exist in an ] balance. Just as an expansion of a ] population might encourage the expansion of a ] population, an expansion of altruistic traits within a gene pool may also encourage the expansion of individuals with dependent traits. An important concept in sociobiology is that temperament traits exist in an ] balance. Just as an expansion of a ] population might encourage the expansion of a ] population, an expansion of altruistic traits within a gene pool may also encourage increasing numbers of individuals with dependent traits.


Studies of human behavior genetics have generally found behavioral traits such as creativity, extroversion, aggressiveness, and ] have high ]. The researchers who carry out those studies are careful to point out that heritability does not constrain the influence that environmental or cultural factors may have on those traits.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Johnson |first1=Wendy |last2=Turkheimer |first2=E. |last3=Gottesman |first3=Irving |last4=Bouchard |first4=Thomas |year=2009 |title=Beyond Heritability: Twin Studies in Behavioral Research |journal=Current Directions in Psychological Science |volume=18 |issue=4 |pages=217–220 |url=http://people.virginia.edu/~ent3c/papers2/Articles%20for%20Online%20CV/Johnson%20%282009%29.pdf |access-date=29 June 2010 |doi=10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01639.x |pmc=2899491 |pmid=20625474 |quote=Moreover, even highly heritable traits can be strongly manipulated by the environment, so heritability has little if anything to do with controllability. For example, height is on the order of 90% heritable, yet North and South Koreans, who come from the same genetic background, presently differ in average height by a full 6 inches (Pak, 2004; Schwekendiek, 2008). |url-status=dead |archive-url= https://web.archive.org/web/20100911043817/http://people.virginia.edu/~ent3c/papers2/Articles%20for%20Online%20CV/Johnson%20(2009).pdf |archive-date= Sep 11, 2010 }}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal |last=Turkheimer |first=Eric |date=April 2008 |title=A Better Way to Use Twins for Developmental Research |journal=LIFE Newsletter |volume=2 |issue=1 |pages=2–5 |url=http://people.virginia.edu/~ent3c/papers2/Articles%20for%20Online%20CV/Turkheimer%20%282008%29.pdf |access-date=29 October 2010 |quote=But back to the question: What does heritability mean? Almost everyone who has ever thought about heritability has reached a commonsense intuition about it: One way or another, heritability has to be some kind of index of how genetic a trait is. That intuition explains why so many thousands of heritability coefficients have been calculated over the years. . . . Unfortunately, that fundamental intuition is wrong. Heritability isn't an index of how genetic a trait is. A great deal of time has been wasted in the effort of measuring the heritability of traits in the false expectation that somehow the genetic nature of psychological phenomena would be revealed. |url-status=dead |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20111125033850/http://people.virginia.edu/~ent3c/papers2/Articles%20for%20Online%20CV/Turkheimer%20(2008).pdf |archive-date= Nov 25, 2011 }}</ref>
Sociobiology is sometimes associated with arguments over the "genetic" basis of intelligence. While sociobiology is predicated on the observation that genes do affect behavior, it is perfectly consistent to be a sociobiologist while arguing that measured IQ variations between individuals reflect mainly cultural or economic rather than genetic factors. However, many critics point out that the usefulness of sociobiology as an explanatory tool breaks down once a trait is so variable as to no longer be exposed to selective pressures. In order to explain aspects of human intelligence as the outcome of selective pressures, it must be demonstrated that those aspects are inherited, or genetic, but this does not necessarily imply differences among individuals: a common genetic inheritance could be shared by ''all'' humans, just as the genes responsible for number of limbs are shared by all individuals. An even more sensitive subject is ].


Various theorists have argued that in some environments ] might be adaptive.<ref name="mealy1995">{{cite journal |last1=Mealey |first1=Linda |title=The Sociobiology of Sociopathy: An Integrated Evolutionary Model |journal=Behavioral and Brain Sciences |date=1995 |volume=18 |issue=3 |pages=523–541 |doi=10.1017/S0140525X00039595 |s2cid=53956461 |url=http://www.bbsonline.org/Preprints/OldArchive/bbs.mealey.html |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20021026131543/http://www.bbsonline.org/Preprints/OldArchive/bbs.mealey.html |access-date=27 October 2020 |archive-date=2002-10-26 }}</ref> The ], by sociologist/criminologist ], posits that female sexual selection has led to increased competitive behavior among men, sometimes resulting in criminality. In another theory, ] argues that a history of intergroup conflict for resources between men have led to differences in violence and aggression between men and women.<ref>{{Citation |last1=Hernán |first1=Roberto |title=Gender Differences in Cooperation and Competition |date=2015 |work=Experimental Economics: Volume 1: Economic Decisions |pages=154–168 |editor-last=Branas-Garza |editor-first=Pablo |place=London |publisher=Palgrave Macmillan UK |language=en |doi=10.1057/9781137538192_10 |isbn=978-1-137-53819-2 |last2=Kujal |first2=Praveen |editor2-last=Cabrales |editor2-first=Antonio}}</ref> The novelist ] also has noted applications of sociobiological theory to cultural practices such as slavery and autocracy.<ref>], '']''. Harmondsworth: Penguin, 1981, pp. 444–445.</ref>
Researchers performing ] studies have argued that differences between people on behavioral traits such as ], ] and aggressiveness are between 45% to 75% due to genetic differences, and ] is said by some to be about 80% genetic after one matures (discussed at ]).


==Support for premise==
] is actively under study, but extremely controversial. There are arguments that in some environments criminal behavior might be adaptive.<ref></ref>
Genetic mouse mutants illustrate the power that genes exert on behavior. For example, the ] FEV (aka Pet1), through its role in maintaining the ] in the brain, is required for normal ] and ]-like behavior.<ref>Hendricks TJ, Fyodorov DV, Wegman LJ, Lelutiu NB, Pehek EA, Yamamoto B, Silver J, Weeber EJ, Sweatt JD, Deneris ES. Pet-1 ETS gene plays a critical role in 5-HT neuron development and is required for normal anxiety-like and aggressive behaviour]. Neuron. 2003 Jan 23;37(2):233–47</ref> Thus, when FEV is genetically deleted from the mouse genome, male mice will instantly attack other males, whereas their wild-type counterparts take significantly longer to initiate violent behavior. In addition, FEV has been shown to be required for correct maternal behavior in mice, such that offspring of mothers without the FEV factor do not survive unless cross-fostered to other wild-type female mice.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Lerch-Haner | first1 = JK | last2 = Frierson | first2 = D | last3 = Crawford | first3 = LK | last4 = Beck | first4 = SG | last5 = Deneris | first5 = ES | date = Sep 2008 | title = Serotonergic transcriptional programming determines maternal behavior and offspring survival | journal = Nat Neurosci | volume = 11 | issue = 9| pages = 1001–1003 | doi=10.1038/nn.2176| pmid = 19160496 | pmc = 2679641 }}</ref>


A genetic basis for instinctive behavioral traits among non-human species, such as in the above example, is commonly accepted among many biologists; however, attempting to use a genetic basis to explain complex behaviors in human societies has remained extremely controversial.<ref>{{cite news |last1=Fisher |first1=Helen |title='Wilson,' They Said, 'Your All Wet!' |url=https://www.nytimes.com/books/98/12/06/specials/wilson-naturalist.html |newspaper=New York Times |access-date=21 July 2015 |date=16 October 1994}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Gould |first1=Stephen Jay |title=Sociobiology: the art of storytelling |journal=New Scientist |date=16 November 1978 |volume=80 |issue=1129 |pages=530–533|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=tRj7EyRFVqYC&pg=PA530}}</ref>
==Criticism==
{{See also|Evolutionary psychology controversy}}
], eugenics became increasingly unpopular within academic science. Many organizations and journals that had their origins in the eugenics movement began to distance themselves from the philosophy, as when ''Eugenics Quarterly'' became ''Social Biology'' in 1969.]]
Many critics draw an intellectual link between sociobiology and ], the belief that most human differences can be traced to specific genes rather than differences in culture or social environments. Critics also draw parallels between biological determinism as an underlying philosophy to the ] and ] of the early 20th century, and ]. ] argues that critics have been overly swayed by politics and a "fear" of biological determinism.<ref name=Pinker2002>Pinker, Steven (2002). ''The Blank Slate: The Modern Denial of Human Nature''. New York: Viking.</ref> However, all these critics have claimed that sociobiology fails on scientific grounds, independent of their political critiques. In particular, ], ] & ] drew a detailed distinction between the politics and history of an idea and its scientific validity,<ref name=lrk84>{{cite book | author=Richard Lewontin, Leon Kamin, Steven Rose | title=Not in Our Genes: Biology, Ideology, and Human Nature | publisher=Pantheon Books | year=1984 | isbn=0-394-50817-3}}</ref> as has ].<ref>Gould, S.J. (1996) "The Mismeasure of Man", Introduction to the Revised Edition</ref>


==Reception==
Wilson and his supporters counter the intellectual link by denying that Wilson had a political agenda, still less a right-wing one. They pointed out that Wilson had personally adopted a number of ] political stances and had attracted progressive sympathy for his outspoken ]. They argued that as scientists they had a duty to uncover the truth whether that was ] or not. They argued that sociobiology does not necessarily lead to any particular political ] as many critics implied. Many subsequent sociobiologists, including ], ], ] and ], have used sociobiology to argue quite separate points. ] came to the defense of sociobiology's methodology, noting that it was the same methodology he used in his work on linguistics. However, he roundly criticized the sociobiologists' actual conclusions about humans as lacking substance. He also noted that the anarchist ] had made similar arguments in his book '']'', although focusing more on altruism than aggression, suggesting that anarchist societies were feasible because of an innate human tendency to cooperate.<ref>Chomsky, Noam (1995). ''Z Magazine'' 8 (Feb.): 20-31.</ref>


] argues that critics have been overly swayed by politics and a fear of ],{{efn|Biological determinism was a philosophy underlying the ] and ] of the early 20th century, and ].<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Allen |first1=Garland E. |title=The Roots of Biological Determinism: review of The Mismeasure of Man by Stephen Jay Gould |journal=Journal of the History of Biology |date=1984 |volume=17 |issue=1 |pages=141–145 |jstor=4330882 |doi=10.1007/bf00397505|pmid=11611452 |s2cid=29672121 }}</ref>}} accusing among others ] and ] of being "radical scientists", whose stance on human nature is influenced by politics rather than science,<ref>{{cite book |author=Pinker, Steven | author-link=Steven Pinker |title=The Blank Slate: The Modern Denial of Human Nature |publisher=Penguin Books |location=New York |year=2002 |page=149 |isbn=978-0-14-200334-3 |quote=A surprising number of intellectuals, particularly on the left, do deny that there is such a thing as inborn talent, especially intelligence. Stephen Jay Gould's 191 bestseller ''The Mismeasure of Man'' was written to debunk 'the abstraction of intelligence as a single entity ... and the use of these numbers to rank people in a single series of worthiness'| title-link=The Blank Slate }}</ref> while Lewontin, ] and ], who drew a distinction between the politics and history of an idea and its scientific validity,<ref name=lrk84>{{cite book |author1=Richard Lewontin |author2=Leon Kamin |author3=Steven Rose | title=Not in Our Genes: Biology, Ideology, and Human Nature |url=https://archive.org/details/notinourgenes00rich |url-access=registration | publisher=Pantheon Books | year=1984 | isbn=978-0-394-50817-7}}</ref> argue that sociobiology fails on scientific grounds. Gould grouped sociobiology with ], criticizing both in his book '']''.<ref>{{cite book |author=Gould, Stephen Jay |author-link=Stephen Jay Gould |year=1996 |title=The Mismeasure of Man |url=https://archive.org/details/mismeasureofman00goul_1 |url-access=registration |page=Introduction to the Revised Edition}}</ref> When ] scheduled sessions on sociobiology at the 1976 ] convention, other scholars attempted to cancel them with what Chagnon later described as "Impassioned accusations of racism, fascism and Nazism"; ]'s support caused the sessions to occur as scheduled.<ref name="eakin20130213">{{Cite news |last=Eakin |first=Emily |date=2013-02-17 |title=Who Are the Real Savages? |url=https://www.nytimes.com/2013/02/17/magazine/napoleon-chagnon-americas-most-controversial-anthropologist.html |access-date=2024-07-03 |work=The New York Times Magazine |pages=32 |language=en-US |issn=0362-4331}}</ref>
Wilson's claims that he had never meant to imply what ''ought'' to be, only what ''is'' the case are supported by his writings, which are descriptive, not prescriptive. However, some critics have argued that the language of sociobiology sometimes slips from "is" to "ought",<ref name=lrk84/> leading sociobiologists to make arguments against social reform on the basis that socially progressive societies are at odds with our innermost nature. For example, some groups have supported positions of ].<ref>Salter, Frank (2007). In R. Dunbar and L. Barrett ''Oxford handbook of evolutionary psychology''. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 541-551.</ref> Views such as this, however, are often criticized as examples of the ], when reasoning jumps from descriptions about what ''is'' to prescriptions about what ''ought'' to be. (A common example is the justification of ] if scientific evidence showed warfare was part of human nature.) It has also been argued that opposition to stances considered anti-social, such as ], are based on ] assumptions, not ] assumptions, meaning that it is not vulnerable to being disproved by bioscientific advances.<ref name=Pinker2002/>{{rp|145}} The history of this debate, and others related to it, are covered in detail by Cronin (1992), Segerstråle (2000) and Alcock (2001).

] has expressed views on sociobiology on several occasions. During a 1976 meeting of the ], as reported by ], Chomsky argued for the importance of a sociobiologically informed notion of human nature.{{sfn|Segerstråle|2000|p=205}} Chomsky argued that human beings are biological organisms and ought to be studied as such, with his criticism of the "]" doctrine in the social sciences (which would inspire a great deal of Steven Pinker's and others' work in evolutionary psychology), in his 1975 '']''.<ref>Chomsky, Noam (1975), ''Reflections on Language'':10. New York: Pantheon Books.</ref> Chomsky further hinted at the possible reconciliation of his anarchist political views and sociobiology in a discussion of ]'s '']'', which focused more on altruism than aggression, suggesting that anarchist societies were feasible because of an innate human tendency to cooperate.<ref>] (1995). ''Z Magazine'' 8 (Feb.): 20–31.</ref>

Wilson has claimed that he had never meant to imply what ''ought'' to be, only what ''is'' the case. However, some critics have argued that the language of sociobiology readily slips from "is" to "ought",<ref name=lrk84/> an instance of the ]. Pinker has argued that opposition to stances considered anti-social, such as ethnic nepotism, is based on ] assumptions, meaning that such opposition is not ] by scientific advances.<ref name=Pinker2002>] (2002). ''The Blank Slate: The Modern Denial of Human Nature''. New York: Viking. p. 145</ref> The history of this debate, and others related to it, are covered in detail by {{harvtxt|Cronin|1993}}, {{harvtxt|Segerstråle|2000}}, and {{harvtxt|Alcock|2001}}.


==See also== ==See also==
{{cols|colwidth=15em}}
{{col-begin}}
{{col-2}}
; Concepts:
* ] * ]
* ] * ]
* ]
* ] * ]
* ]
* ] * ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ] * ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ] * ]
* ]
* ] * ]
* ] * ]
* ] * ]
* ]
* ] * ]
* ] * ]
* ] * ]
<!--please don't add people here, describe their contribution and link them in article instead-->
{{col-2}}
{{colend}}
; Well-known sociobiologists:
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
{{col-end}}
; Books:
* '']'' by ], 1975
* '']'' by ]
* '']'' by ]
* '']'' by ], ] & ]


==References== ==References==
'''Informational notes'''
===Notes===
{{notelist}}


'''Citations'''
{{reflist}}
{{reflist|30em}}
===Bibliography===


'''Bibliography'''
{{No footnotes|date=May 2007}}
* {{cite book |last=Alcock |first=John |title=The triumph of sociobiology |year=2001 |location=Oxford, UK |publisher=Oxford University Press |isbn=978-0-19-514383-6 |url-access=registration |url=https://archive.org/details/triumphofsociobi00alco }}
{{refbegin}}
* {{cite book |editor=Barkow, Jerome |year=2006 |title=Missing the Revolution: Darwinism for Social Scientists |publisher=Oxford University Press |isbn=978-0-19-513002-7}}
* Alcock, John (2001). ''The Triumph of Sociobiology''. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Directly rebuts several of the above criticisms and misconceptions listed above.
* {{cite book |last=Cronin |first=Helena |author-link=Helena Cronin |title=The ant and the peacock: Altruism and sexual selection from Darwin to today |year=1993 |publisher=Press Syndicate of the University of Cambridge |isbn=978-0-521-45765-1 }}
* Barkow, Jerome (Ed.). (2006) ''Missing the Revolution: Darwinism for Social Scientists''. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
* {{cite book | last=Etcoff | first=Nancy | author-link=Nancy Etcoff | title=Survival of the Prettiest: The Science of Beauty | publisher=Anchor Books | year=1999 | isbn=978-0-385-47942-4 | url-access=registration | url=https://archive.org/details/survivalofpretti00nanc }}
* Cronin, H. (1992). ''The Ant and the Peacock: Altruism and Sexual Selection from Darwin to Today''. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
* {{cite book | author=Nancy Etcoff | authorlink=Nancy Etcoff | title=Survival of the Prettiest: The Science of Beauty | publisher=Anchor Books | year=1999 | isbn=0-385-47942-5}} * {{cite book |last1=Kaplan |first1=Gisela |author-link1=Gisela Kaplan |last2=Rogers |first2=Lesley J. | title=Gene Worship: Moving Beyond the Nature/Nurture Debate over Genes, Brain, and Gender | publisher=Other Press | year=2003 | isbn=978-1-59051-034-6}}
* {{cite book | author=Richard M. Lerner | author-link=Richard M. Lerner| title=Final Solutions: Biology, Prejudice, and Genocide | publisher=Pennsylvania State University Press| year=1992| isbn=978-0-271-00793-9}}
* Haugan, Gørill (2006) ''Nursing home patients’ spirituality. Interaction of the spiritual, physical, emotional and social dimensions'' (Faculty of Nursing, Sør-Trøndelag University College Norwegian University of Science and Technology)
* {{cite book | author=Richard M. Lerner | title=Final Solutions: Biology, Prejudice, and Genocide | publisher=Pennsylvania State University Press| year=1992| isbn=0-271-00793-1}}
* Richards, Janet Radcliffe (2000). ''Human Nature After Darwin: A Philosophical Introduction''. London: Routledge. * Richards, Janet Radcliffe (2000). ''Human Nature After Darwin: A Philosophical Introduction''. London: Routledge.
* Segerstråle, Ullica (2000). ''Defenders of the Truth: The Battle for Science in the Sociobiology Debate and Beyond''. Oxford: Oxford University Press. * {{cite book |last=Segerstråle |first=Ullica |title=Defenders of the truth: The sociobiology debate |year=2000 |publisher=Oxford University Press |isbn=978-0-19-286215-0 }}

* {{cite book | author=Gisela Kaplan, Lesley J Rogers | title=Gene Worship: Moving Beyond the Nature/Nurture Debate over Genes, Brain, and Gender | publisher=Other Press | year=2003 | isbn=1-59051-034-8}}
{{refend}}
==External links== ==External links==
{{Commons category}}
* (Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy) - &
{{Wikiquote}}
*
* (Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy)
* Interviews with leading sociobiologists.
*
* - ]
* – interviews with leading sociobiologists
* - An Attempted Sociobiological Explanation of the scientific basis for Political Group Formation.
* by ]
*
*

{{sociobiology|state=expanded}} {{sociobiology|state=expanded}}
{{Branches of biology}}
{{Biology_nav}}
{{evolutionary psychology}} {{evolutionary psychology}}
{{ethology}}
{{Nikolaas Tinbergen}}
{{Authority control}}


]
]
]
]
] ]
] ]
] ]
] ]

]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]
]

Latest revision as of 18:41, 24 December 2024

Subdiscipline of biology regarding social behavior For the book by E. O. Wilson, see Sociobiology: The New Synthesis.
Part of a series on
Evolutionary biology
Darwin's finches by John Gould
Processes and outcomes
Natural history
History of evolutionary theory
Fields and applications
Social implications

Sociobiology is a field of biology that aims to explain social behavior in terms of evolution. It draws from disciplines including psychology, ethology, anthropology, evolution, zoology, archaeology, and population genetics. Within the study of human societies, sociobiology is closely allied to evolutionary anthropology, human behavioral ecology, evolutionary psychology, and sociology.

Sociobiology investigates social behaviors such as mating patterns, territorial fights, pack hunting, and the hive society of social insects. It argues that just as selection pressure led to animals evolving useful ways of interacting with the natural environment, so also it led to the genetic evolution of advantageous social behavior.

While the term "sociobiology" originated at least as early as the 1940s; the concept did not gain major recognition until the publication of E. O. Wilson's book Sociobiology: The New Synthesis in 1975. The new field quickly became the subject of controversy. Critics, led by Richard Lewontin and Stephen Jay Gould, argued that genes played a role in human behavior, but that traits such as aggressiveness could be explained by social environment rather than by biology. Sociobiologists responded by pointing to the complex relationship between nature and nurture. Among sociobiologists, the controversy between laying weight to different levels of selection was settled between D.S. Wilson and E.O. Wilson in 2007.

Definition

E. O. Wilson defined sociobiology as "the extension of population biology and evolutionary theory to social organization".

Sociobiology is based on the premise that some behaviors (social and individual) are at least partly inherited and can be affected by natural selection. It begins with the idea that behaviors have evolved over time, similar to the way that physical traits are thought to have evolved. It predicts that animals will act in ways that have proven to be evolutionarily successful over time. This can, among other things, result in the formation of complex social processes conducive to evolutionary fitness.

The discipline seeks to explain behavior as a product of natural selection. Behavior is therefore seen as an effort to preserve one's genes in the population. Inherent in sociobiological reasoning is the idea that certain genes or gene combinations that influence particular behavioral traits can be inherited from generation to generation.

For example, newly dominant male lions often kill cubs in the pride that they did not sire. This behavior is adaptive because killing the cubs eliminates competition for their own offspring and causes the nursing females to come into heat faster, thus allowing more of his genes to enter into the population. Sociobiologists would view this instinctual cub-killing behavior as being inherited through the genes of successfully reproducing male lions, whereas non-killing behavior may have died out as those lions were less successful in reproducing.

History

E. O. Wilson, a central figure in the history of sociobiology, from the publication in 1975 of his book Sociobiology: The New Synthesis

The philosopher of biology Daniel Dennett suggested that the political philosopher Thomas Hobbes was the first proto-sociobiologist, arguing that in his 1651 book Leviathan Hobbes had explained the origins of morals in human society from an amoral sociobiological perspective.

The geneticist of animal behavior John Paul Scott coined the word sociobiology at a 1948 conference on genetics and social behavior, which called for a conjoint development of field and laboratory studies in animal behavior research. With John Paul Scott's organizational efforts, a "Section of Animal Behavior and Sociobiology" of the Ecological Society of America was created in 1956, which became a Division of Animal Behavior of the American Society of Zoology in 1958. In 1956, E. O. Wilson came in contact with this emerging sociobiology through his PhD student Stuart A. Altmann, who had been in close relation with the participants to the 1948 conference. Altmann developed his own brand of sociobiology to study the social behavior of rhesus macaques, using statistics, and was hired as a "sociobiologist" at the Yerkes Regional Primate Research Center in 1965. Wilson's sociobiology is different from John Paul Scott's or Altmann's, insofar as he drew on mathematical models of social behavior centered on the maximization of the genetic fitness by W. D. Hamilton, Robert Trivers, John Maynard Smith, and George R. Price. The three sociobiologies by Scott, Altmann and Wilson have in common to place naturalist studies at the core of the research on animal social behavior and by drawing alliances with emerging research methodologies, at a time when "biology in the field" was threatened to be made old-fashioned by "modern" practices of science (laboratory studies, mathematical biology, molecular biology).

Once a specialist term, "sociobiology" became widely known in 1975 when Wilson published his book Sociobiology: The New Synthesis, which sparked an intense controversy. Since then "sociobiology" has largely been equated with Wilson's vision. The book pioneered and popularized the attempt to explain the evolutionary mechanics behind social behaviors such as altruism, aggression, and nurturance, primarily in ants (Wilson's own research specialty) and other Hymenoptera, but also in other animals. However, the influence of evolution on behavior has been of interest to biologists and philosophers since soon after the discovery of evolution itself. Peter Kropotkin's Mutual Aid: A Factor of Evolution, written in the early 1890s, is a popular example. The final chapter of the book is devoted to sociobiological explanations of human behavior, and Wilson later wrote a Pulitzer Prize winning book, On Human Nature, that addressed human behavior specifically.

Edward H. Hagen writes in The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology that sociobiology is, despite the public controversy regarding the applications to humans, "one of the scientific triumphs of the twentieth century." "Sociobiology is now part of the core research and curriculum of virtually all biology departments, and it is a foundation of the work of almost all field biologists. " Sociobiological research on nonhuman organisms has increased dramatically and continuously in the world's top scientific journals such as Nature and Science. The more general term behavioral ecology is commonly substituted for the term sociobiology in order to avoid the public controversy.

Theory

Sociobiologists maintain that human behavior, as well as nonhuman animal behavior, can be partly explained as the outcome of natural selection. They contend that in order to fully understand behavior, it must be analyzed in terms of evolutionary considerations.

Natural selection is fundamental to evolutionary theory. Variants of hereditary traits which increase an organism's ability to survive and reproduce will be more greatly represented in subsequent generations, i.e., they will be "selected for". Thus, inherited behavioral mechanisms that allowed an organism a greater chance of surviving and/or reproducing in the past are more likely to survive in present organisms. That inherited adaptive behaviors are present in nonhuman animal species has been multiply demonstrated by biologists, and it has become a foundation of evolutionary biology. However, there is continued resistance by some researchers over the application of evolutionary models to humans, particularly from within the social sciences, where culture has long been assumed to be the predominant driver of behavior.

Nikolaas Tinbergen, whose work influenced sociobiology

Sociobiology is based upon two fundamental premises:

  • Certain behavioral traits are inherited,
  • Inherited behavioral traits have been honed by natural selection. Therefore, these traits were probably "adaptive" in the environment in which the species evolved.

Sociobiology uses Nikolaas Tinbergen's four categories of questions and explanations of animal behavior. Two categories are at the species level; two, at the individual level. The species-level categories (often called "ultimate explanations") are

  • the function (i.e., adaptation) that a behavior serves and
  • the evolutionary process (i.e., phylogeny) that resulted in this functionality.

The individual-level categories (often called "proximate explanations") are

Sociobiologists are interested in how behavior can be explained logically as a result of selective pressures in the history of a species. Thus, they are often interested in instinctive, or intuitive behavior, and in explaining the similarities, rather than the differences, between cultures. For example, mothers within many species of mammals – including humans – are very protective of their offspring. Sociobiologists reason that this protective behavior likely evolved over time because it helped the offspring of the individuals which had the characteristic to survive. This parental protection would increase in frequency in the population. The social behavior is believed to have evolved in a fashion similar to other types of nonbehavioral adaptations, such as a coat of fur, or the sense of smell.

Individual genetic advantage fails to explain certain social behaviors as a result of gene-centred selection. E.O. Wilson argued that evolution may also act upon groups. The mechanisms responsible for group selection employ paradigms and population statistics borrowed from evolutionary game theory. Altruism is defined as "a concern for the welfare of others". If altruism is genetically determined, then altruistic individuals must reproduce their own altruistic genetic traits for altruism to survive, but when altruists lavish their resources on non-altruists at the expense of their own kind, the altruists tend to die out and the others tend to increase. An extreme example is a soldier losing his life trying to help a fellow soldier. This example raises the question of how altruistic genes can be passed on if this soldier dies without having any children.

Within sociobiology, a social behavior is first explained as a sociobiological hypothesis by finding an evolutionarily stable strategy that matches the observed behavior. Stability of a strategy can be difficult to prove, but usually, it will predict gene frequencies. The hypothesis can be supported by establishing a correlation between the gene frequencies predicted by the strategy, and those expressed in a population.

Altruism between social insects and littermates has been explained in such a way. Altruistic behavior, behavior that increases the reproductive fitness of others at the apparent expense of the altruist, in some animals has been correlated to the degree of genome shared between altruistic individuals. A quantitative description of infanticide by male harem-mating animals when the alpha male is displaced as well as rodent female infanticide and fetal resorption are active areas of study. In general, females with more bearing opportunities may value offspring less, and may also arrange bearing opportunities to maximize the food and protection from mates.

An important concept in sociobiology is that temperament traits exist in an ecological balance. Just as an expansion of a sheep population might encourage the expansion of a wolf population, an expansion of altruistic traits within a gene pool may also encourage increasing numbers of individuals with dependent traits.

Studies of human behavior genetics have generally found behavioral traits such as creativity, extroversion, aggressiveness, and IQ have high heritability. The researchers who carry out those studies are careful to point out that heritability does not constrain the influence that environmental or cultural factors may have on those traits.

Various theorists have argued that in some environments criminal behavior might be adaptive. The evolutionary neuroandrogenic (ENA) theory, by sociologist/criminologist Lee Ellis, posits that female sexual selection has led to increased competitive behavior among men, sometimes resulting in criminality. In another theory, Mark van Vugt argues that a history of intergroup conflict for resources between men have led to differences in violence and aggression between men and women. The novelist Elias Canetti also has noted applications of sociobiological theory to cultural practices such as slavery and autocracy.

Support for premise

Genetic mouse mutants illustrate the power that genes exert on behavior. For example, the transcription factor FEV (aka Pet1), through its role in maintaining the serotonergic system in the brain, is required for normal aggressive and anxiety-like behavior. Thus, when FEV is genetically deleted from the mouse genome, male mice will instantly attack other males, whereas their wild-type counterparts take significantly longer to initiate violent behavior. In addition, FEV has been shown to be required for correct maternal behavior in mice, such that offspring of mothers without the FEV factor do not survive unless cross-fostered to other wild-type female mice.

A genetic basis for instinctive behavioral traits among non-human species, such as in the above example, is commonly accepted among many biologists; however, attempting to use a genetic basis to explain complex behaviors in human societies has remained extremely controversial.

Reception

Steven Pinker argues that critics have been overly swayed by politics and a fear of biological determinism, accusing among others Stephen Jay Gould and Richard Lewontin of being "radical scientists", whose stance on human nature is influenced by politics rather than science, while Lewontin, Steven Rose and Leon Kamin, who drew a distinction between the politics and history of an idea and its scientific validity, argue that sociobiology fails on scientific grounds. Gould grouped sociobiology with eugenics, criticizing both in his book The Mismeasure of Man. When Napoleon Chagnon scheduled sessions on sociobiology at the 1976 American Anthropological Association convention, other scholars attempted to cancel them with what Chagnon later described as "Impassioned accusations of racism, fascism and Nazism"; Margaret Mead's support caused the sessions to occur as scheduled.

Noam Chomsky has expressed views on sociobiology on several occasions. During a 1976 meeting of the Sociobiology Study Group, as reported by Ullica Segerstråle, Chomsky argued for the importance of a sociobiologically informed notion of human nature. Chomsky argued that human beings are biological organisms and ought to be studied as such, with his criticism of the "blank slate" doctrine in the social sciences (which would inspire a great deal of Steven Pinker's and others' work in evolutionary psychology), in his 1975 Reflections on Language. Chomsky further hinted at the possible reconciliation of his anarchist political views and sociobiology in a discussion of Peter Kropotkin's Mutual Aid: A Factor of Evolution, which focused more on altruism than aggression, suggesting that anarchist societies were feasible because of an innate human tendency to cooperate.

Wilson has claimed that he had never meant to imply what ought to be, only what is the case. However, some critics have argued that the language of sociobiology readily slips from "is" to "ought", an instance of the naturalistic fallacy. Pinker has argued that opposition to stances considered anti-social, such as ethnic nepotism, is based on moral assumptions, meaning that such opposition is not falsifiable by scientific advances. The history of this debate, and others related to it, are covered in detail by Cronin (1993), Segerstråle (2000), and Alcock (2001).

See also

References

Informational notes

  1. Biological determinism was a philosophy underlying the social Darwinian and eugenics movements of the early 20th century, and controversies in the history of intelligence testing.

Citations

  1. Wilson, Edward O. (2000-03-24), "The Social Insects", Sociobiology, Harvard University Press, pp. 397–437, doi:10.2307/j.ctvjnrttd.22, ISBN 978-0-674-74416-5
  2. Nielsen, François (1994). "Sociobiology and Sociology". Annual Review of Sociology. 20 (1): 267–303. doi:10.1146/annurev.so.20.080194.001411. ISSN 0360-0572.
  3. de Sousa, Ronald (1990-01-01). "The sociology of sociobiology". International Studies in the Philosophy of Science. 4 (3): 271–283. doi:10.1080/02698599008573367. ISSN 0269-8595.
  4. Freedman, Daniel G. (January 1985). "Sociobiology and the human dimension". Ethology and Sociobiology. 6 (2): 121–122. doi:10.1016/0162-3095(85)90006-8. ISSN 0162-3095.
  5. ^ Wilson, David Sloan; Wilson, Edward O. (2007). "Rethinking The Theoretical Foundation of Sociobiology". The Quarterly Review of Biology. 82 (4): 327–348. doi:10.1086/522809. PMID 18217526. S2CID 37774648.
  6. Wilson, E. O. (1978). On Human Nature. Harvard. p. x. ISBN 978-0674016385.
  7. Mohammed, Sulma I.; Alfarouk, Khalid O.; Elhassan, Ahmed M.; Hamad, Kamal; Ibrahim, Muntaser E. (2019). "Sociobiological Transition and Cancer". The Genetics of African Populations in Health and Disease. pp. 217–232. doi:10.1017/9781139680295.010. ISBN 9781139680295. S2CID 214321882.
  8. Packer, Craig; Pusey, Anne E. (1983). "Adaptations of Female Lions to Infanticide by Incoming Males" (PDF). Am. Nat. 121 (5): 716–728. Bibcode:1983ANat..121..716P. doi:10.1086/284097. S2CID 84927815. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2015-12-29. Retrieved 2017-04-03.
  9. Dennett, Daniel (1995). Darwin's Dangerous Idea. Simon and Schuster. pp. 453–454. ISBN 978-0140167344.
  10. "The Life of J.P. Scott". Bowling Green State University. Retrieved 14 December 2016.
  11. Dobzhansky, Theodosius (September 1966). "Are Naturalists Old-Fashioned?". The American Naturalist. 100 (915): 541–550. Bibcode:1966ANat..100..541D. doi:10.1086/282448. S2CID 129104506.
  12. Walsh, Bryan (17 August 2011). "All-Time 100 Nonfiction Books". Time.
  13. The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology, edited by David M. Buss, John Wiley & Sons, 2005. Chapter 5 by Edward H. Hagen
  14. Wilson, 1975. Chapter 5. "Group Selection and Altruism"
  15. Tessman, Irwin (1995). "Human altruism as a courtship display". Forum. 74 (1): 157–158. Bibcode:1995Oikos..74..157T. doi:10.2307/3545685. JSTOR 3545685.
  16. Johnson, Wendy; Turkheimer, E.; Gottesman, Irving; Bouchard, Thomas (2009). "Beyond Heritability: Twin Studies in Behavioral Research" (PDF). Current Directions in Psychological Science. 18 (4): 217–220. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01639.x. PMC 2899491. PMID 20625474. Archived from the original (PDF) on Sep 11, 2010. Retrieved 29 June 2010. Moreover, even highly heritable traits can be strongly manipulated by the environment, so heritability has little if anything to do with controllability. For example, height is on the order of 90% heritable, yet North and South Koreans, who come from the same genetic background, presently differ in average height by a full 6 inches (Pak, 2004; Schwekendiek, 2008).
  17. Turkheimer, Eric (April 2008). "A Better Way to Use Twins for Developmental Research" (PDF). LIFE Newsletter. 2 (1): 2–5. Archived from the original (PDF) on Nov 25, 2011. Retrieved 29 October 2010. But back to the question: What does heritability mean? Almost everyone who has ever thought about heritability has reached a commonsense intuition about it: One way or another, heritability has to be some kind of index of how genetic a trait is. That intuition explains why so many thousands of heritability coefficients have been calculated over the years. . . . Unfortunately, that fundamental intuition is wrong. Heritability isn't an index of how genetic a trait is. A great deal of time has been wasted in the effort of measuring the heritability of traits in the false expectation that somehow the genetic nature of psychological phenomena would be revealed.
  18. Mealey, Linda (1995). "The Sociobiology of Sociopathy: An Integrated Evolutionary Model". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 18 (3): 523–541. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00039595. S2CID 53956461. Archived from the original on 2002-10-26. Retrieved 27 October 2020.
  19. Hernán, Roberto; Kujal, Praveen (2015), Branas-Garza, Pablo; Cabrales, Antonio (eds.), "Gender Differences in Cooperation and Competition", Experimental Economics: Volume 1: Economic Decisions, London: Palgrave Macmillan UK, pp. 154–168, doi:10.1057/9781137538192_10, ISBN 978-1-137-53819-2
  20. Elias Canetti, Crowds and Power. Harmondsworth: Penguin, 1981, pp. 444–445.
  21. Hendricks TJ, Fyodorov DV, Wegman LJ, Lelutiu NB, Pehek EA, Yamamoto B, Silver J, Weeber EJ, Sweatt JD, Deneris ES. Pet-1 ETS gene plays a critical role in 5-HT neuron development and is required for normal anxiety-like and aggressive behaviour]. Neuron. 2003 Jan 23;37(2):233–47
  22. Lerch-Haner, JK; Frierson, D; Crawford, LK; Beck, SG; Deneris, ES (Sep 2008). "Serotonergic transcriptional programming determines maternal behavior and offspring survival". Nat Neurosci. 11 (9): 1001–1003. doi:10.1038/nn.2176. PMC 2679641. PMID 19160496.
  23. Fisher, Helen (16 October 1994). "'Wilson,' They Said, 'Your All Wet!'". New York Times. Retrieved 21 July 2015.
  24. Gould, Stephen Jay (16 November 1978). "Sociobiology: the art of storytelling". New Scientist. 80 (1129): 530–533.
  25. Allen, Garland E. (1984). "The Roots of Biological Determinism: review of The Mismeasure of Man by Stephen Jay Gould". Journal of the History of Biology. 17 (1): 141–145. doi:10.1007/bf00397505. JSTOR 4330882. PMID 11611452. S2CID 29672121.
  26. Pinker, Steven (2002). The Blank Slate: The Modern Denial of Human Nature. New York: Penguin Books. p. 149. ISBN 978-0-14-200334-3. A surprising number of intellectuals, particularly on the left, do deny that there is such a thing as inborn talent, especially intelligence. Stephen Jay Gould's 191 bestseller The Mismeasure of Man was written to debunk 'the abstraction of intelligence as a single entity ... and the use of these numbers to rank people in a single series of worthiness'
  27. ^ Richard Lewontin; Leon Kamin; Steven Rose (1984). Not in Our Genes: Biology, Ideology, and Human Nature. Pantheon Books. ISBN 978-0-394-50817-7.
  28. Gould, Stephen Jay (1996). The Mismeasure of Man. p. Introduction to the Revised Edition.
  29. Eakin, Emily (2013-02-17). "Who Are the Real Savages?". The New York Times Magazine. p. 32. ISSN 0362-4331. Retrieved 2024-07-03.
  30. Segerstråle 2000, p. 205.
  31. Chomsky, Noam (1975), Reflections on Language:10. New York: Pantheon Books.
  32. Chomsky, Noam (1995). "Rollback, Part II." Z Magazine 8 (Feb.): 20–31.
  33. Pinker, Steven (2002). The Blank Slate: The Modern Denial of Human Nature. New York: Viking. p. 145

Bibliography

External links

Sociobiology
Topics
Supporters
Opponents
Branches of biology
See also
Biology
Biology
Overview
Chemical basis
Cells
Genetics
Evolution
Diversity
Plant form
and function
Animal form
and function
Ecology
Research
methods
Laboratory
techniques
Field techniques
Branches
Glossaries
Evolutionary psychology
Evolutionary
processes
Areas
Cognition /
Emotion
Culture
Development
Human factors /
Mental health
Sex
Sex differences
Related subjects
Academic disciplines
Research topics
Theoretical positions
Ethology
Branches
Ethologists
Societies
Journals
Nikolaas Tinbergen
Selected works
General
Related
Categories: