Misplaced Pages

Biology and political orientation: Difference between revisions

Article snapshot taken from Wikipedia with creative commons attribution-sharealike license. Give it a read and then ask your questions in the chat. We can research this topic together.
Browse history interactively← Previous editContent deleted Content addedVisualWikitext
Revision as of 03:28, 26 June 2012 editAnarchangel (talk | contribs)Extended confirmed users3,534 edits ...."recognizing threats" is not in the study, only in the Time article, as the RK link shows. The Fox link is a summary of the NYDailyNews. Rmv "further reading" already cited← Previous edit Latest revision as of 10:53, 8 January 2025 edit undoWatch Atlas791 (talk | contribs)325 editsm minor edits 
(366 intermediate revisions by more than 100 users not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
{{short description|Correlation between human biology and political tendencies}}
<!-- Please do not remove or change this AfD message until the issue is settled -->
{{Politics sidebar|Related}}
{{Article for deletion/dated|page=Biology and political orientation|timestamp=20120624180710|year=2012|month=June|day=24|substed=yes|help=off}}
<!-- For administrator use only: {{Old AfD multi|page=Biology and political orientation|date=24 June 2012|result='''keep'''}} -->
<!-- End of AfD message, feel free to edit beyond this point -->
{{POV|date=June 2012}}
{{too few opinions|date=June 2012}}


A number of studies have found that ] can be linked with ].<ref name=Jost2001>{{cite journal|last=Jost|first=John T.|author2=Amodio, David M.|title=Political ideology as motivated social cognition: Behavioral and neuroscientific evidence|journal=Motivation and Emotion|date=13 November 2011|volume=36|issue=1|pages=55–64|doi=10.1007/s11031-011-9260-7|s2cid=10675844|url=http://www.psych.nyu.edu/jost/Jost-Amodio-2012.pdf}}</ref> This means that an individual's biology may predispose them to a particular political orientation and ] or, conversely, that subscription to certain ideologies may predispose them to measurable biological and ].
A number of studies have found that ] may be linked with political orientation.<ref name=Jost2001/>
{{multiple image
| header =
| footer =
| align = right
| image1 = Amyg.png
| width1 = 181
| caption1 = According to Ryota Kanai's study, conservatives have larger ]s (shown in red)
| alt1 =
| image2 = Anterior cingulate gyrus animation.gif
| width2 = 220
| caption2 = Kanai's study shows liberals have larger ]es (shown in red)
| alt2 =
}}


Studies have found that subjects with ] (or '']'' in the United States) political views have larger ]e,<ref name=RK/> report larger social networks and greater happiness than liberals, are more prone to express ] to moral infringements and are more sensitive to perceived threats.<ref name=YI/><ref name=JJ1/> Those with ] (or '']'' in the United States) political views are more likely to report greater relationship dissatisfaction and emotional distress than conservatives, to show more openness to experience as well as greater tolerance for uncertainty and disorder.<ref name=JV/><ref name=JJ2/>
==Heritability==
One approach to studying the role of genetics for a characteristic is to calculate the ] coefficient. This method is generally based on ] and assumes that any statistical difference between monozygotic and dizygotic twins is the sole result of genetic differences.


Genetic factors account for at least some of the variation of political views.<ref>{{Cite web |last=Dolan |first=Eric W. |date=2024-04-10 |title=Genetic variations help explain the link between cognitive ability and liberalism |url=https://www.psypost.org/genetic-variations-help-explain-the-link-between-cognitive-ability-and-liberalism/ |access-date=2024-04-12 |website=PsyPost - Psychology News |language=en-US}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Kleppesto |first1=Thomas Haarklau |last2=Czajkowski |first2=Nikolai Olavi |last3=Sheehy-Skeffington |first3=Jennifer |last4=Vassend |first4=Olav |last5=Roysamb |first5=Espen |last6=Eftedal |first6=Nikolai Haahjem |last7=Kunst |first7=Jonas R. |last8=Ystrom |first8=Eivind |last9=Thomsen |first9=Lotte |date=2024-02-22 |title=The genetic underpinnings of right-wing authoritarianism and social dominance orientation explain political attitudes beyond Big Five personality |url=https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/jopy.12921 |journal=Journal of Personality |language=en |doi=10.1111/jopy.12921 |pmid=38386613 |issn=0022-3506|hdl=10852/111202 |hdl-access=free }}</ref> From the perspective of ], conflicts regarding ] may have been common in the ancestral environment and humans may have developed psychological mechanisms for judging their own chances of succeeding in such conflicts. These mechanisms may affect political views. However, many of the studies linking biology to politics remain controversial and unreplicated.<ref>{{Cite journal |title=Biology and ideology: The anatomy of politics |last=Buchen |first=Lizzie |date=2012 |journal=Nature |volume=490 |issue=7421 |pages=466–468 |language=en |doi=10.1038/490466a |pmid = 23099382|bibcode=2012Natur.490..466B |doi-access=free }}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal |last=Davies |first=James |date=1983 |title=The Proper Biological Study of Politics |url=https://www.jstor.org/stable/3791065 |journal=Political Psychology |volume=4 |issue=4 |pages=731–743 |doi=10.2307/3791065 |jstor=3791065 }}</ref>
Twin studies as a source of heritability estimates have been superseded since it relies on several questionable assumptions regarding the similarity of the environment in which twins are reared.<ref>Jon Beckwith and Corey A. Morris. Twin Studies of Political Behavior: Untenable Assumptions? Perspectives on Politics (2008), 6 : pp 785-791.</ref><ref>Handbook of Social Psychology, Volume 1. Susan T. Fiske, Daniel T. Gilbert, Gardner Lindzey. p. 372.</ref>

A 2005 twin study study examined the attitudes regarding 28 different political issues such as capitalism, unions, X-rated movies, abortion, school prayer, divorce, property taxes, and the draft. Twins were asked if they agreed or disagreed or were uncertain about each issue. Genetic factors accounted for 53% of the variance of an overall score. However, self-identification as Republican and Democrat had a much lower heritability of 14%. This may be due to party affiliation being more sensitive to factors such as upbringing and life experiences. Another explanation is that some persons may remain loyal despite their party changing its ideological stance. When asked about the study, ] thus argued that his views remained unchanged but that the Democratic Party had changed its party line greatly. The strong heritability regarding political attitudes may explain a strong political polarization and why bipartisan compromises are difficult to achieve. Furthermore, men and women tend to marry persons with similar political views suggesting an increasing future divergence between the two sides.<ref>BENEDICT CAREY, Some Politics May Be Etched in the Genes, June 21, 2005, The New York Times, http://www.nytimes.com/2005/06/21/science/21gene.html?pagewanted=all</ref><ref>{{cite doi|10.1017/S0003055405051579}}</ref> Jost et al. wrote in a 2011 review that "Many studies involving quite diverse samples and methods suggest that political and religious views reflect a reasonably strong genetic basis, but this does not mean that ideological proclivities are unaffected by personal experiences or environmental factors."<ref name=Jost2001/>


==Brain studies== ==Brain studies==
===Structural differences===
According to a 2011 study<ref name=RK>{{cite journal|title=Political Orientations Are Correlated with Brain Structure in Young Adults |url=http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0960982211002892 |author=R. Kanai et al.|journal=Curr Biol|date=2011-04-05|pmid=21474316|doi=10.1016/j.cub.2011.03.017|volume=21|issue=8|pages=677–80}}</ref> by cognitive neuroscientist Ryota Kanai's group<ref name=RK/> at ] published in '']'', people with different political views have different ] structures.<ref name=RK/> The scientists performed ] scans on 90 volunteer young adult people's brains.<ref name=RK/><ref name=HL></ref> The results of the study showed that ]s had a larger ],<ref name=RK/><ref name=SD>] Ryota Kanai, Tom Feilden, Colin Firth and ]. ]</ref> a structure of the brain associated with greater sensitivity to ] and ] emotional learning.<ref name=HL/> ] had increased grey matter in the ],<ref name=RK/><ref name=SD/> a structure of the brain associated with monitoring uncertainty and handling conflicting information.<ref name=RK/><ref name=HL/>
:''"Although our data do not determine whether these regions play a causal role in the formation of political attitudes, they converge with previous work to suggest a possible link between brain structure and psychological mechanisms that mediate political attitudes."-<small>Political Orientations Are Correlated with Brain Structure in Young Adults''</small><ref name=RK/>
In an interview with ], Ryota Kanai said, "It's very unlikely that actual political orientation is directly encoded in these brain regions", and that, "more work is needed to determine how these brain structures mediate the formation of political attitude."<ref name=HL/><ref name=livescience> ]</ref><ref name=Jost2001/><ref name=NYD></ref>


] views (right) tended to have a larger amygdala than those who expressed ] views (left).]]
===Functional differences===
'']'' in 2007 reported a study<ref name=NN/> by scientists at ] and ] that showed that political orientation is related to differences in how the brain processes information. According to UCLA neurologist Dr. Marco Iacoboni, the study showed "there are two cognitive styles -- a liberal style and a conservative style."<ref name=LATimes2007/> The article "Neurocognitive correlates of liberalism and conservatism"<ref name=NN/> published in ''Nature Neuroscience'' "found that greater liberalism was associated with stronger conflict-related anterior cingulate activity, suggesting greater neurocognitive sensitivity to cues for altering a habitual response pattern."<ref name=NN>David M Amodio, John T Jost, Sarah L Master & Cindy M Yee, , '']''. Cited by </ref>


A 2011 study by cognitive neuroscientist Ryota Kanai at ] found structural ] differences between subjects of different political orientation in a ] of students at the same college.<ref name=RK>{{cite journal | title = Political Orientations Are Correlated with Brain Structure in Young Adults |last1=Kanai|first1=R|last2=Feilden|first2=T|last3=Firth|first3=C|last4=Rees|first4=G| journal = Current Biology| date = 2011 | doi = 10.1016/j.cub.2011.03.017 | volume = 21 | issue = 8 | pages= 677 – 80 | pmid = 21474316 | pmc = 3092984 | bibcode = 2011CBio...21..677K }}</ref> The researchers performed ] scans on the brains of 90 volunteer students who had indicated their political orientation on a five-point scale ranging from "very ]" to "very ]".<ref name=RK/><ref name=HL>Ninh, Amie (April 8, 2011). . '']''.</ref>
In an ] study published in '']'', three different patterns of brain activation were found to correlate with individualism, conservatism, and radicalism.<ref>{{cite journal |title=Individualism, conservatism, and radicalism as criteria for processing political beliefs: a parametric fMRI study | pmid = 19562629 | doi=10.1080/17470910902860308 | volume=4 | issue=5 | year=2009 | pages=367–83 | author=Zamboni G, Gozzi M, Krueger F, Duhamel JR, Sirigu A, Grafman J}} Zamboni G, Gozzi M, Krueger F, Duhamel JR, Sirigu A, Jordan Grafman. National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD, USA</ref> In general, fMRI responses in several portions of the brain have been linked to viewing of the faces of well-known politicians.<ref>{{cite journal|pmc=1828689|title=Politics on the Brain: An fMRI Investigation|author=Kristine Knudson et al.|date=2006-03|publisher=PubMed preprint (Soc Neurosci)|pmid=17372621}}</ref> Others believe that determining political affiliation from fMRI data is overreaching.<ref>{{cite journal | pmid = 19232374 | doi=10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.12.017 | volume=73 | issue=1 | title=Great expectations: what can fMRI research tell us about psychological phenomena? | year=2009 | month=July | pages=10–6 | author=Aue T, Lavelle LA, Cacioppo JT}}</ref>


Students who reported more conservative political views were found to have larger ],<ref name=RK/> a structure in the ]s whose primary function is in the formation, consolidation and processing of ], as well as positive and negative conditioning (emotional learning).<ref>{{cite book|last=Carlson|first=Neil R.|title=Physiology of Behavior|date=12 January 2012|publisher=Pearson|isbn=978-0205239399|page=364}}</ref> The amygdala is responsible for important roles in social interaction, such as the recognition of emotional cues in facial expressions and the monitoring of personal space,<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Bzdok D, Langner R, Caspers S, Kurth F, Habel U, Zilles K, Laird A, Eickhoff SB | title = ALE meta-analysis on facial judgments of trustworthiness and attractiveness | journal = Brain Structure & Function | volume = 215 | issue = 3–4 | pages = 209–23 | date = January 2011 | pmid = 20978908 | pmc = 4020344 | doi = 10.1007/s00429-010-0287-4 }}</ref><ref name="Kennedy">{{cite journal | vauthors = Kennedy DP, Gläscher J, Tyszka JM, Adolphs R | title = Personal space regulation by the human amygdala | journal = Nature Neuroscience | volume = 12 | issue = 10 | pages = 1226–7 | date = October 2009 | pmid = 19718035 | pmc = 2753689 | doi = 10.1038/nn.2381 }}</ref> with larger amygdalae correlating with larger and more complex social networks.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Bickart KC, Wright CI, Dautoff RJ, Dickerson BC, Barrett LF | title = Amygdala volume and social network size in humans | journal = Nature Neuroscience | volume = 14 | issue = 2 | pages = 163–4 | date = February 2011 | pmid = 21186358 | pmc = 3079404 | doi = 10.1038/nn.2724 }}</ref><ref>{{Cite magazine|url=https://healthland.time.com/2010/12/28/how-to-win-friends-have-a-big-amygdala/?xid=rss-topstories|title=How to Win Friends: Have a Big Amygdala?|magazine=Time|first=Maia|last=Szalavitz|date=28 December 2010|access-date=30 December 2010|url-status=live|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20110717061203/http://healthland.time.com/2010/12/28/how-to-win-friends-have-a-big-amygdala/?xid=rss-topstories|archive-date=17 July 2011|df=dmy-all}}</ref> It is also postulated to play a role in threat detection, including modulation of ] and ] to perceived threats.<ref>T.L. Brink. (2008) Psychology: A Student Friendly Approach. . pp 61.</ref><ref name="The human amygdala and the inductio">{{cite journal | vauthors = Feinstein JS, Adolphs R, Damasio A, Tranel D | title = The human amygdala and the induction and experience of fear | journal = Current Biology | volume = 21 | issue = 1 | pages = 34–8 | date = January 2011 | pmid = 21167712 | pmc = 3030206 | doi = 10.1016/j.cub.2010.11.042 | bibcode = 2011CBio...21...34F }}</ref><ref name = Staut-1998>{{cite journal | vauthors = Staut CC, Naidich TP | s2cid = 46862405 | title = Urbach-Wiethe disease (Lipoid proteinosis) | journal = Pediatric Neurosurgery | volume = 28 | issue = 4 | pages = 212–4 | date = April 1998 | pmid = 9732251 | doi = 10.1159/000028653 }}</ref> Conservative students were also found to have greater volume of gray matter in the left ] and the right ].<ref name=RK/> There is evidence that conservatives are more sensitive to ]<ref name=YI>{{cite journal|title= Conservatives are more easily disgusted than liberals. |url=http://yoelinbar.net/papers/disgust_conservatism.pdf | author=Y. Inbar |journal= Cognition and Emotion |date=2008|volume=23|issue=4 |pages=714–725 |doi=10.1080/02699930802110007|display-authors=etal|citeseerx=10.1.1.372.3053 |s2cid=7411404 }}</ref> and one role of the insula is in the modulation of social emotions, such as the feeling of disgust to specific sights, smells and norm violations.<ref>{{cite journal |vauthors=Sanfey AG, Rilling JK, Aronson JA, Nystrom LE, Cohen JD |s2cid=7111382 |title=The neural basis of economic decision-making in the Ultimatum Game |journal=Science |volume=300 |issue=5626 |pages=1755–8 |date=June 2003 |pmid=12805551 |doi=10.1126/science.1082976 |bibcode=2003Sci...300.1755S }}</ref><ref name=Wicker>{{cite journal|title=Both of us disgusted in My insula: The common neural basis of seeing and feeling disgust|author=B. Wicker|journal=Neuron|date=2003|volume=40|issue=3|pages=655–664|display-authors=etal|doi=10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00679-2|pmid=14642287|doi-access=free}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |vauthors=Wright P, He G, Shapira NA, Goodman WK, Liu Y |title=Disgust and the insula: fMRI responses to pictures of mutilation and contamination |journal=NeuroReport |volume=15 |issue=15 |pages=2347–51 |date=October 2004 |pmid=15640753 |doi=10.1097/00001756-200410250-00009|s2cid=6864309 }}</ref>
==Gene associations studies==
"A Genome-Wide Analysis of Liberal and Conservative Political Attitudes" by Peter K. Hatemi et al. traces DNA research involving 13,000 subjects. The study identifies several genes potentially connected with political positions.<ref> Peter K. Hatemi United States Studies Centre, University of Sydney and Queensland Institute of Medical Research, Nathan A. Gillespie Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, Lindon J. Eaves Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, Brion S. Maher Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, Bradley T. Webb Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, Andrew C. Heath Washington University St. Louis, Sarah E. Medland Queensland Institute of Medical Research, David C. Smyth Queensland Institute of Medical Research, Harry N. Beeby Queensland Institute of Medical Research, Scott D. Gordon Queensland Institute of Medical Research, Grant W. Montgomery Queensland Institute of Medical Research, Ghu Zhu Queensland Institute of Medical Research, Enda M. Byrne Queensland Institute of Medical Research, Nicholas G. Martin Queensland Institute of Medical Research, The Journal of Politics, Vol. 73, No. 1, January 2011, Pp. 1–15 ISSN 0022-3816 "The under-standing that we cannot yet accurately map how genes influence brain processes and biological mechanisms which in turn interact with our upbringing,s ocial life, personal experience, the weather, diet, etc, to somehow be expressed in part as a Conservative-Liberal orientation, is the exact reason that genome-wide analyses are valuable and necessary for political science.''</ref> Genes associated with both threat perceptivity and cognitive flexibility predicted results on a liberal-conservative scale.<ref name=Jost2001/>


Students who reported more liberal political views were found to have a larger volume of grey matter in the ],<ref name=RK/> a structure of the brain associated with emotional awareness and the emotional processing of pain.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Lane RD, Reiman EM, Axelrod B, Yun LS, Holmes A, Schwartz GE | title = Neural correlates of levels of emotional awareness. Evidence of an interaction between emotion and attention in the anterior cingulate cortex | journal = Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience | volume = 10 | issue = 4 | pages = 525–35 | date = July 1998 | pmid = 9712681 | doi = 10.1162/089892998562924 | s2cid = 27743177 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Price DD | s2cid = 15250446 | title = Psychological and neural mechanisms of the affective dimension of pain | journal = Science | volume = 288 | issue = 5472 | pages = 1769–72 | date = June 2000 | pmid = 10846154 | doi = 10.1126/science.288.5472.1769 | bibcode = 2000Sci...288.1769P }}</ref> The anterior cingulate cortex becomes active in situations of uncertainty,<ref name=HC>{{cite journal|title= Neural activity in the human brain relating to uncertainty and arousal during anticipation. | author=H. Critchley |journal= Neuron |date=2001|volume=29|issue=2 |pages=537–545 |doi=10.1016/s0896-6273(01)00225-2| pmid=11239442 |display-authors=etal|hdl=21.11116/0000-0001-A313-1 | s2cid=10995076 |hdl-access=free }}</ref> and is postulated to play a role in ] detection, such as the monitoring and processing of conflicting stimuli or information.<ref name=Bush00>{{cite journal | vauthors = Bush G, Luu P, Posner MI | title = Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex | journal = Trends in Cognitive Sciences | volume = 4 | issue = 6 | pages = 215–222 | date = June 2000 | pmid = 10827444 | doi = 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01483-2 | s2cid = 16451230 }}</ref>
A variant of the ] gene (DRD4-7R), affecting novelty seeking, has been described as a "liberal gene".<ref name=HL />


The authors concluded, "Although our data do not determine whether these regions play a causal role in the formation of political attitudes, they converge with previous work to suggest a possible link between brain structure and psychological mechanisms that mediate political attitudes."<ref name=RK/> In an interview with ], Ryota Kanai said, "It's very unlikely that actual political orientation is directly encoded in these brain regions", and that "more work is needed to determine how these brain structures mediate the formation of political attitude."<ref name=Jost2001/><ref name=HL/><ref name=livescience>{{cite web | url=http://www.livescience.com/13608-brain-political-ideology-liberal-conservative.html | title=Politics on the Brain: Scans Show Whether You Lean Left or Right | date=7 April 2011 | publisher=] | access-date=September 25, 2012}}</ref><ref name=NYD>{{cite news | url=http://articles.nydailynews.com/2011-04-08/entertainment/29415110_1_brain-structure-political-orientation-liberals-and-conservatives | archive-url=https://archive.today/20120710003053/http://articles.nydailynews.com/2011-04-08/entertainment/29415110_1_brain-structure-political-orientation-liberals-and-conservatives | url-status=dead | archive-date=July 10, 2012 | title=The liberal brain? Scans show liberals and conservatives have different brain structures | newspaper=] | date=April 8, 2011 | access-date=September 25, 2012 | author=Kattalia, Kathryn}}</ref> Kanai and colleagues added that it is necessary to conduct a longitudinal study to determine whether the changes in brain structure that we observed lead to changes in political behavior or whether political attitudes and behavior instead result in changes of brain structure.
==Functional assays==
===Threats===
Persons judged by both themselves and others as being more fearful, even in ], are more likely to have, or to have in the future, more conservative views.<ref name=Jost2001/>


A 2024 study by Petalas et al achieved a partial replication of Kanai et al, using a larger sample size of 928 subjects, making it the largest preregistered replication study in political neuroscience to date. A positive relationship between the size of the amygdala and right-wing political views was found but at approximately a third of the effect size of the original study (''r'' = 0.068 vs ''r'' = 0.23). The study also did not find a replication of the original finding of a positive relationship between a larger volume of grey matter in the anterior cingulate cortex and left-wing political view.<ref>Petalas, D. P., Schumacher, G., & Scholte, S. H. (2024). Is political ideology correlated with brain structure? A preregistered replication. iScience.</ref>
Persons with right-wing views had greater ] response, indicating greater ] response, to threatening images than those with left-wing views in one study. There was no difference for positive or neutral images. Holding right-wing views was also associated with a stronger ] as measured by strength of eyeblink in response to unexpected noise.<ref name=Jost2001/>


==Functional differences==
Studies have found that conservatives are more perceptive to threatening faces.<ref name=livescience/>


===Psychometrics===
Conservatism is associated with being more sensitive to ].<ref name=Jost2001/>


Various studies suggest measurable differences in the psychological traits of ] and ]. Conservatives are more likely to report larger social networks, greater happiness and self-esteem than liberals, are more reactive to perceived threats and more likely to interpret ambiguous facial expressions as threatening.<ref name=JJ3>{{cite journal|title=Why Are Conservatives Happier Than Liberals? | author1=Napier J | author2=Jost JT |journal= Psychological Science|date=2008|volume=19|issue=6|pages=562-72 |doi=10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02124.x |pmid=}}</ref><ref name=JV>{{cite journal|title= Political leanings vary with facial expression processing and psychosocial functioning. | author=J. Vigil |journal= Group Processes & Intergroup Relations |date=2010|volume=13|issue=5 |pages=547–558 |doi=10.1177/1368430209356930| s2cid=59571553 |display-authors=etal}}</ref><ref name=JJ1>{{cite journal|title=The end of the end of ideology. |url=http://www.psych.nyu.edu/jost/Jost%282006%29The-End-of-the-End-of-Ideology.pdf | author=J. Jost |journal= American Psychologist|date=2006|volume=61|issue=7|pages=651–670 |doi=10.1037/0003-066x.61.7.651|display-authors=etal |pmid=17032067}}</ref><ref name=JJ2>{{cite journal|title= Political conservatism as motivated social cognition. |url=http://contracabal.com/PDF/Jost-1.pdf | author=J. Jost |journal= Psychological Bulletin |date=2003|volume=129|issue=3|pages=339–375 |doi=10.1037/0033-2909.129.3.339 |pmid=12784934|s2cid=1778256 |display-authors=etal}}</ref> Liberals are more likely to report greater emotional distress, relationship dissatisfaction and experiential hardship than conservatives, and show more openness to experience as well as greater tolerance for uncertainty and disorder.<ref name=JV/><ref name=JJ2/><ref name=JJ3/>
What is familiar may be less threatening than what is unfamiliar, which has been argued to explain why conservatives are more suspicious of change than liberals.<ref name=Jost2001>{{cite doi|10.1007/s11031-011-9260-7}}</ref>


===<span id="Behavioral Studies"></span>Behavioral studies===
Research has found that political orientation is influenced by short-term events related to fear and disgust. Thus, simply asking persons to wash their hands caused their views to become more conservative. Reminding persons of the existence of threats such as ] caused political views to become more conservative.<ref name=Jost2001/> In a survey of the perceived severity of moral transgressions, conservatives were more affected by the taste of a bitter drink than liberals."...taste perception significantly affected moral judgments, such that physical disgust (induced via a bitter taste) elicited feelings of moral disgust. Further, this effect was more pronounced in participants with politically conservative views".<ref>{{cite journal | pmid = 21307274 | doi=10.1177/0956797611398497 | volume=22 | issue=3 | title=A bad taste in the mouth: gustatory disgust influences moral judgment | year=2011 | month=March | pages=295–9 | author=Eskine KJ, Kacinik NA, Prinz JJ}}</ref><!--- On moral choices {{cite journal | pmid =20699405 | doi=10.1177/0146167210380605 | volume=36 | issue=9 | title=When values and attributions collide: liberals' and conservatives' values motivate attributions for alleged misdeeds | year=2010 | month=September | pages=1241–54 | author=Morgan GS, Mullen E, Skitka LJ}} may be relevant but seems hard to summarize - should see full text --->


A study by David Amodio ''et al.'' at ] and the ], found differences in how self-described liberal and conservative research participants responded to changes in patterns.<ref name=NN>David M Amodio, John T Jost, Sarah L Master & Cindy M Yee, , '']''. Cited by </ref> Participants were asked to tap a keyboard when the letter "M" appeared on a computer monitor and to refrain from tapping when they saw a "W". The letter "M" appeared four times more frequently than "W", conditioning participants to press the keyboard when a letter appears. Liberal participants made fewer mistakes than conservatives during testing and their ] readings showed more activity in the anterior cingulate cortex, the part of the brain that deals with conflicting information, during the experiment, suggesting that they were better able to detect conflicts in established patterns. Amodio warned against concluding that a particular political orientation is superior. He said: "The tendency of conservatives to block distracting information could be a good thing depending on the situation."<ref>{{cite web|url=http://psychcentral.com/news/2007/09/10/brains-of-liberals-conservatives-may-work-differently/1691.html|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20161013125516/http://psychcentral.com/news/2007/09/10/brains-of-liberals-conservatives-may-work-differently/1691.html|archive-date=2016-10-13|title=Brains of Liberals, Conservatives May Work Differently|publisher=Psych Central|date=2007-10-20}}</ref><ref name=LATimes2007>{{cite news|url=http://www.latimes.com/news/obituaries/la-sci-politics10sep10,0,2687256.story|title=Study finds left-wing brain, right-wing brain|newspaper=]|date=2007-09-10}}</ref>
The ] increased public support for conservative views as compared to shortly before. Among survivors, three times as many become more conservative as compared to becoming more liberal during the 18 months after the attacks.<ref name=Jost2001/>


A 2017 study both replicated the original study and also found that conservatives performed better in a task in which choosing the simple strategy was the more optimal solution; while both liberals and conservatives started the task attempting the more complex but less effective strategy, conservatives switched to the simple strategy more quickly than liberals.<ref>Bernabel, R. T., & Oliveira, A. (2017). Conservatism and liberalism predict performance in two nonideological cognitive tasks. Politics and the Life Sciences, 36(2), 49-59.</ref> Amodio's original study was also replicated by Weissflog ''et al.'' (2013)<ref>Weissflog, M., Choma, B. L., Dywan, J., van Noordt, S. J., & Segalowitz, S. J. (2013). The political (and physiological) divide: Political orientation, performance monitoring, and the anterior cingulate response. Social neuroscience, 8(5), 434-447.</ref> and Inzlicht ''et al.'' (2009).<ref>Inzlicht, Michael, Ian McGregor, Jacob B. Hirsh, and Kyle Nash. "Neural markers of religious conviction." Psychological science 20, no. 3 (2009): 385-392.</ref> Conversely, Kremláček ''et al.'' (2019)<ref>Kremláček, Jan, Daniel Musil, Jana Langrová, and Martin Palecek. "Neural correlates of liberalism and conservatism in a post-Communist country." Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 13 (2019): 119.</ref> and Wendell (2016)<ref>Wendell, D. G. (2016). Loyola eCommons Neural Correlates of Political Attitudes: Emotion and Ideology in the Brain. Available online at: http://ecommons.luc.edu/luc_diss/2299, Loyola University Chicago, pages 133-178 Accessed 27/07/2023</ref> did not replicate Amodio's results. Both studies also argued that Weissflog and Inzlicht's results were not as concordant with the Amodio's findings as originally claimed.
===Cognitive tests===
A study by scientists at New York University and the University of California, Los Angeles found differences in how self-described liberal and conservative research participants responded to changes in patterns. Participants were asked to tap a keyboard when the letter 'M' appeared on a computer monitor and to refrain from tapping when they saw a 'W'. The letter 'M' appeared four times more frequently than 'W', conditioning participants to press the keyboard on almost every trial. Liberal participants made fewer mistakes than conservatives when they saw the rare 'W', indicating to the researchers that these participants were better able to accept changes or conflicts in established patterns. The participants were also wired to an ] that recorded activity in their anterior cingulate cortex, the part of the brain that detects conflicts between a habitual tendency and a more appropriate response. Liberals were significantly more likely than conservatives to show activity in the brain circuits that deal with conflicts during the experiment, and this correlated with their greater accuracy in the test. The lead author of the study David Amodio warned against concluding that a particular political orientation is superior. "The tendency of conservatives to block distracting information could be a good thing depending on the situation, he said."<ref>{{cite web|url=http://psychcentral.com/news/2007/09/10/brains-of-liberals-conservatives-may-work-differently/1691.html|title=Brains of Liberals, Conservatives May Work Differently|publisher=Psych Central|date=2007-10-20}}</ref><ref name=LATimes2007>{{cite web|url=http://www.latimes.com/news/obituaries/la-sci-politics10sep10,0,2687256.story|title=Study finds left-wing brain, right-wing brain|publisher=Los Angeles Times|date=2007-09-10}}</ref>


A study of subjects' reported level of ] linked to various scenarios showed that people who scored highly on the "disgust sensitivity" scale held more politically conservative views,<ref name=YI/> which some researchers believe could be partially explained by personality traits.<ref>Xu, Xiaowen, Annika K. Karinen, Hanah A. Chapman, Jordan B. Peterson, and Jason E. Plaks. "An orderly personality partially explains the link between trait disgust and political conservatism." Cognition and Emotion (2019).</ref> However, the findings of a 2019 study suggest that sensitivity to disgust among conservatives varies according to the elicitors used, and that using an elicitor-unspecific scale caused the differences in sensitivity to disappear between those of different political orientations.<ref>Elad-Strenger, Julia, Jutta Proch, and Thomas Kessler. "Is Disgust a "Conservative" Emotion?." Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin (2019): 0146167219880191.</ref>
According to a 2010 study by ], ] data from the "]" survey averaged 106 for adolescents who self-identified as "very liberal", versus 95 for those calling themselves "very conservative".<ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.asanet.org/press/20100223/Evolution_and_Intelligence.cfm|title=Intelligent People Have "Unnatural" Preferences and Values That Are Novel in Human Evolutionary History|publisher=American Sociological Association press release|date=2010-02-23}}</ref><ref name=Kanazawa2010>{{cite journal|url=http://spq.sagepub.com/cgi/content/abstract/0190272510361602v1|title=Why Liberals and Atheists Are More Intelligent|author=Satoshi Kanazawa|journal=Social Psychology Quarterly|year=2010|doi=10.1177/0190272510361602}}</ref><ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2010/02/100224132655.htm|title=Liberals and Atheists Smarter? Intelligent People Have Values Novel in Human Evolutionary History, Study Finds|publisher=]|date=2010-02-24}}</ref><ref name=CNN>{{cite web|url=http://www.cnn.com/2010/HEALTH/02/26/liberals.atheists.sex.intelligence/index.html?section=cnn_latest|title=Liberalism, atheism, male sexual exclusivity linked to IQ|author=Elizabeth Landau|date=2010-02-26|publisher=]}}</ref><ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.upi.com/Health_News/2010/03/02/Higher-IQ-linked-to-liberalism-atheism/UPI-68381267513202/|title=Higher IQ linked to liberalism, atheism|date=2010-03-02|publisher=]}}</ref><ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.thestar.com/living/article/773018--are-liberals-and-atheists-smarter|title=Are liberals and atheists smarter? Psychologist links teen IQ levels with adult views on religion, politics and family|author=Nicole Baute|publisher=Toronto Star|date=2010-03-01}}</ref> An unrelated study in 2009 found that among students applying to U.S. universities, conservatism correlated negatively with SAT, Vocabulary, and Analogy test scores though there was a greater correlation with economic differences.<ref>{{cite journal|url=|title=Conservatism and cognitive ability|author=Larry Stankov|journal=Intelligence|volume=37|issue=3|date=2009-05|pages=294–304|doi=10.1016/j.intell.2008.12.007}}</ref> A 2012 study stated that "In an analysis of two large-scale, nationally representative United Kingdom data sets (N = 15,874), we found that lower general intelligence (g) in childhood predicts greater racism in adulthood, and this effect was largely mediated via socially conservative ideology. A secondary analysis of a U.S. data set confirmed a predictive effect of poor abstract-reasoning skills on antihomosexual prejudice, a relation partially mediated by both authoritarianism and low levels of intergroup contact."<ref>{{cite journal|url=http://pss.sagepub.com/content/early/2012/01/04/0956797611421206.abstract|title=Bright Minds and Dark Attitudes: Lower Cognitive Ability Predicts Greater Prejudice Through Right-Wing Ideology and Low Intergroup Contact|author=Gordon Hodson and Michael A. Busseri|date=2012-01-05|journal=Psychological Science|pages=(early online publication)}}</ref><ref name=LiveScience2>{{cite web|url=http://www.livescience.com/18132-intelligence-social-conservatism-racism.html|title=Low IQ & Conservative Beliefs Linked to Prejudice|author=Stephanie Pappas|date=2012-01-26|publisher=]}}</ref>


A 2018 study in the United States looking at levels of ] (the tendency to favour analytic reasoning over instinctive or "gut" responses) found that those who voted for ] in the ] had lower levels of cognitive reflection than ] voters or third-party voters. However, this effect was mostly driven by Democrats who voted for Trump, while amongst Republicans, Clinton and Trump voters had more similar levels of cognitive reflection. Republicans who voted for third-party candidates or those who identified as libertarian had the highest levels of cognitive reflection.<ref>], and David G. Rand. "Cognitive reflection and the 2016 US Presidential election." Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 45, no. 2 (2019): 224-239.</ref>
Kanazawa argued that ] developed in order to help solve evolutionarily new problems while for evolutionarily recurring problems there were more specific instincts proscribing how to behave. Those having higher general intelligence are furthermore argued to be relatively more likely than those with lower general intelligence to ignore and act contrary to instinctual responses. To be more altruistic towards those being more genetically similar and acquaintances than towards strangers as well as being religious (see also ]) are argued to be evolutionarily based instincts. Higher support for social welfare for strangers as well as atheism are therefore argued to be more relatively more common among those with higher intelligence. Leadership professor James Bailey commented that publicly advocating such "unconventional" values may even have become a way of demonstrating to everyone one's own superior intelligence and elite status.<ref name=Kanazawa2010/><ref name=CNN/>


===Physiology===
] of '']'' disputed claims that Democratic-leaning states of the United States are smarter on average than Republican-leaning states, noting that a study published in '']'' in 2000 was a hoax, and a 2006 study by Satoshi Kanazawa was grossly flawed by failing to note that students in Southern states who did not take the SAT scholastic test were often taking the competing ACT exam instead.<ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.straightdope.com/columns/read/3033/are-blue-states-smarter-than-red-states|title=Are blue states smarter than red states?|date=2012-01-20|author=Cecil Adams|publisher=The Straight Dope}}</ref>


People with right-wing views had greater ] response, indicating greater ] response, to threatening images than those with left-wing views in one study.<ref name="Oxley2008">{{cite journal |last1=Oxley |first1=D. R. |last2=Smith |first2=K. B. |last3=Alford |first3=J. R. |last4=Hibbing |first4=M. V. |last5=Miller |first5=J. L. |last6=Scalora |first6=M. |last7=Hatemi |first7=P. K. |last8=Hibbing |first8=J. R. |title=Political Attitudes Vary with Physiological Traits |journal=Science |date=19 September 2008 |volume=321 |issue=5896 |pages=1667–1670 |doi=10.1126/science.1157627|pmid=18801995 |bibcode=2008Sci...321.1667O |s2cid=7215269 |url=https://digitalcommons.unl.edu/poliscifacpub/26 }}</ref><ref name=Jost2001/> There was no difference for positive or neutral images. Holding right-wing views was also associated with a stronger ] as measured by strength of eyeblink in response to unexpected noise. Subsequent studies with substantially greater statistical power have failed to replicate these effects.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Bakker |first1=Bert N. |last2=Schumacher |first2=Gijs |last3=Gothreau |first3=Claire |last4=Arceneaux |first4=Kevin |title=Conservatives and liberals have similar physiological responses to threats |journal=Nature Human Behaviour |date=June 2020 |volume=4 |issue=6 |pages=613–621 |doi=10.1038/s41562-020-0823-z|pmid=32042109 |pmc=7306406 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Fournier |first1=Patrick |last2=Soroka |first2=Stuart |last3=Nir |first3=Lilach |title=Negativity Biases and Political Ideology: A Comparative Test across 17 Countries |journal=American Political Science Review |date=August 2020 |volume=114 |issue=3 |pages=775–791 |doi=10.1017/S0003055420000131|s2cid=225441710 }}</ref>
Rindermann el a. (2011), studying the situation in ], argued that the results, as well as the results from many previous studies in other nations, support that high intelligence is associated with supporting centrist, ] positions. High intelligence is also associated with having any political opinions at all. In Brazil longer education was associated with less centrist and more leftist political views. The researchers argued that the more intelligent tend to support centrist, meritocratic positions as being best for both society and themselves. This centrist position can vary somewhat depending on the surrounding society and tends to be more centrist-left in more strict class based societies restricting advancement based on merit and more centrist-right in societies having egalitarian policies also restricting the more intelligent. On the other hand, longer education may promote less centrist views by students through various processes increasingly being aligned with the dominant norms in the educational system.<ref name=Rindmann2011>{{cite doi|10.1016/j.intell.2011.11.005}}</ref>


In an ] study published in '']'', three different patterns of brain activation were found to correlate with individualism, conservatism, and radicalism.<ref>{{cite journal |title=Individualism, conservatism, and radicalism as criteria for processing political beliefs: a parametric fMRI study | pmid = 19562629 | doi=10.1080/17470910902860308 | volume=4 | issue=5 | year=2009 | pages=367–83 |vauthors=Zamboni G, Gozzi M, Krueger F, Duhamel JR, Sirigu A, Grafman J | journal=Social Neuroscience| s2cid = 15652205 }} Zamboni G, Gozzi M, Krueger F, Duhamel JR, Sirigu A, Jordan Grafman. National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD, USA</ref> In general, fMRI responses in several portions of the brain have been linked to viewing of the faces of well-known politicians.<ref>{{cite journal|pmc=1828689|title=Politics on the Brain: An fMRI Investigation|author=Kristine Knudson|date=March 2006|pmid=17372621|doi=10.1080/17470910600670603|volume=1|issue=1|journal=Soc Neurosci|pages=25–40|display-authors=etal}}</ref> Others believe that determining political affiliation from fMRI data is overreaching.<ref>{{cite journal | pmid = 19232374 | doi=10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.12.017 | volume=73 | issue=1 | title=Great expectations: what can fMRI research tell us about psychological phenomena? |date=July 2009 | pages=10–6 |vauthors=Aue T, Lavelle LA, Cacioppo JT | journal=International Journal of Psychophysiology| url=https://boris.unibe.ch/71224/2/AueLavelleCacioppo_2009_IJP.pdf }}</ref>
===Other variables===
Liberals have higher ]. Political ideology is even associated with interior decoration with the offices of liberals being rated as more colorful, comfortable, distinctive, and stylish.<ref name=livescience/>


==Genetic studies==
Conservatives score higher on ] and ].<ref>Chris Mooney. "Liberals and conservatives don’t just vote differently. They think differently." April 13, 2012. The Washington Post. http://www.washingtonpost.com/opinions/liberals-and-conservatives-dont-just-vote-differently-they-think-differently/2012/04/12/gIQAzb1kDT_story.html</ref>


===Heritability===
In research conducted by ] psychologist ] regarding moral stereotypes in politics, conservatives and moderates were adept at guessing how liberals would answer questions, while liberals, particularly those who described themselves as "very liberal", were least able to guess how the conservatives would answer. <ref>{{cite web|urlhttp://www.nytimes.com/2012/03/22/opinion/kristof-politics-odors-and-soap.html?_r=3&src=tp&smid=fb-share#comments|title=Politics, Odor and Soap|date=2012-03-22|author=Nicholas D. Kristof|publisher=The New York Times}}</ref>
] compares differences in genetic factors in individuals to the total ] of observable characteristics ("]s") in a population, to determine the heritability coefficient. Factors including ], ] and random chance can all contribute to the variation in individuals' phenotypes.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Raj | first1 = A | last2 = van Oudenaarden | first2 = A | year = 2008 | title = Nature, nurture, or chance: stochastic gene expression and its consequences | journal = Cell| volume = 135| issue = 2| pages = 216–26| doi = 10.1016/j.cell.2008.09.050 | pmid = 18957198 | pmc=3118044}}</ref>


The use of ] assumes the elimination of non-genetic differences by finding the statistical differences between ] (identical) twins, which have almost the same genes, and ] (fraternal) twins.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Martin |first1=Nicholas |last2=Boomsma |first2=Dorret |last3=Machin |first3=Geoffrey |title=A twin-pronged attack on complex traits |journal=] |date=17 December 1997 |volume=17 |issue=4 |pages=387–92 |doi=10.1038/ng1297-387 |pmid=9398838 |s2cid=2028886 |url=https://genepi.qimr.edu.au/contents/publications/staff/MartinDorret_NatGen1997_TwinProng387_392.pdf}}</ref> The similarity of the environment in which twins are reared has been questioned.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Beckwith |first1=Jon |last2=Morris |first2=Corey A. |title=Twin Studies of Political Behavior: Untenable Assumptions? |journal=Perspectives on Politics |date=December 2008 |volume=6 |issue=4 |pages=785–91 |doi=10.1017/S1537592708081917 |s2cid=55630117 |url=https://www.cambridge.org/core/journals/perspectives-on-politics/article/twin-studies-of-political-behavior-untenable-assumptions/AB2BB4C87F5E776BF2C5C563235A5255}}</ref><ref>{{cite book |last1=Fiske |first1=Susan T. |last2=Gilbert |first2=Daniel T. |last3=Lindzey |first3=Gardner |title=Handbook of Social Psychology |date=15 February 2010 |publisher=John Wiley & Sons |page=372 |edition=5th |url=https://honors.illinoisstate.edu/downloads/SwannBosson.pdf}}</ref>
From an ] perspective, conflicts regarding ] may have been a recurrent issue in the ancestral environment. Humans may therefore have developed psychological mechanisms for judging their chance of succeeding in such conflicts which will affect their political views. For males physical strength may have been an important factor in deciding the outcome of such conflicts. Therefore, a prediction is that males having having high physical strength and low SES will support redistribution while males having both high SES and high physical strength will oppose redistribution. Cross-cultural research found this to be the case. For females their physical strength had no influence on their political views which was as expected since females rarely have physical strength above that of the average male.<ref name=AEP>Michael Bang Petersen. The evolutionary psychology of Mass Politics. In {{cite doi|10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199586073.001.0001}}</ref> A study on political attitudes among ] actors found that, while the actors were generally leftist, male actors with great physical strength were more likely to support the Republican stance on foreign issues and foreign military interventions.<ref>Strong men more likely to vote Conservative, 11 Apr 2012, The Telegraph, http://www.telegraph.co.uk/news/newstopics/howaboutthat/9197597/Strong-men-more-likely-to-vote-Conservative.html</ref>


A twin study in 2005 by Alford ''et al.'' examined the attitudes regarding 28 different political issues such as capitalism, unions, X-rated movies, abortion, school prayer, divorce, property taxes, and the draft. Twins were asked if they agreed or disagreed or were uncertain about each issue. Genetic factors accounted for 53% of the variance of an overall score. However, self-identification as ] and ] had a much lower heritability of 14%. It is worthwhile to note that identical twins correlated in opinion at a rate of 0.66 while fraternal twins correlated in opinion by 0.44. This likely occurs because identical twins share 100% of their DNA while fraternal twins share on average only 50% of their DNA.<ref>{{cite news | url=https://www.nytimes.com/2005/06/21/science/21gene.html?pagewanted=all | title=Some Politics May Be Etched in the Genes | newspaper=] | date=June 21, 2005 | access-date=September 25, 2012 | author=Carey, Benedict}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal | last1 = Alford | first1 = J. R. | last2 = Funk | first2 = C. L. | last3 = Hibbing | first3 = J. R. | title = Are Political Orientations Genetically Transmitted? | doi = 10.1017/S0003055405051579 | journal = American Political Science Review | volume = 99 | issue = 2 | pages = 153–167 | year = 2005 | citeseerx = 10.1.1.622.476 | s2cid = 3820911 }}</ref> However, Jonathan Kaplan argued that the role of individual genes is often extremely small due to many human physical traits being polygenic and may be overstated,<ref>Kincaid, Harold, and Jeroen Van Bouwel, eds. The Oxford Handbook of Philosophy of Political Science. Oxford University Press, 2023, pg 19-32</ref> observing that the study by Alford ''et al.'' made a case for the role of the ] region being involved in numerous psychological and personality traits, yet Border ''et al.'' (2019) found that multiple associations with 5-HTTLPR were spurious and underpowered.<ref>Border, Richard, Emma C. Johnson, Luke M. Evans, Andrew Smolen, Noah Berley, Patrick F. Sullivan, and Matthew C. Keller. "No support for historical candidate gene or candidate gene-by-interaction hypotheses for major depression across multiple large samples." American Journal of Psychiatry 176, no. 5 (2019): 376-387.</ref>
Several studies have found that the ] of the candidates influences election results. A study of political candidates in ] found that the right-wing candidates were more physically attractive than the left-wing candidates. Physical attractiveness also had a greater influence on the election results for the right-wing candidates than for the left-wing candidates. The results were stronger for municipal elections than for national elections. Possible explanations include that more physical attractive people tend to be more anti-egalitarian and right-wing or that the left is more rational.<ref>CONSERVATIVE CANDIDATES ARE OFTEN BETTER-LOOKING, Wed Feb 23, 201, DIscoveryNews/AFP, http://news.discovery.com/human/conservative-candidates-attractiveness-elections-110223.html</ref>


Jost ''et al.'' wrote in a 2011 review that "Many studies involving quite diverse samples and methods suggest that political and religious views reflect a reasonably strong genetic basis, but this does not mean that ideological proclivities are unaffected by personal experiences or environmental factors."<ref name=Jost2001/>

===Gene association studies===
In 2014, a study was performed on genomic data from 12,000 twin pairs from Australia, the USA, Denmark, Sweden and Hungary to examine genetic influences on political ideology. The study's ] did not provide any definitive evidence of a specific genetic marker related to ideology. The authors remarked that, as with any complex trait, a single gene or small group of genes would not influence ideology directly but that there would likely be "thousands of genetic variants of very small effects and constellations of genes interacting with each other and the environment to influence behavior, indirectly".<ref name=HPK>{{cite journal| title = Genetic Influences on Political Ideologies: Twin Analyses of 19 Measures of Political Ideologies from Five Democracies and Genome-Wide Findings from Three Populations | last1 = Hatemi|first1=PK|last2=Medland|first2=SE|last3=Klemmensen|first3=R|last4=Oskarrson|first4=S|last5=Littvay|first5=L|last6=Dawes|first6=C|last7=Verhulst|first7=B|last8=McDermott|first8=R|last9=Nørgaard|first9=AS|last10=Klofstad|first10=C|last11=Christensen|first11=K|last12=Johannesson|first12=M|last13=Magnusson|first13=PKE|last14=Eaves|first14=LJ|last15=Martin|first15=NG | journal = Behav Genet | date = 2014 | volume = 44 | issue = 3 |pages = 282–294 | pmid = 24569950 | doi = 10.1007/s10519-014-9648-8 | pmc = 4038932 }}</ref>

"A Genome-Wide Analysis of Liberal and Conservative Political Attitudes" by Peter K. Hatemi ''et al.'' traces DNA research involving 13,000 subjects. The study identifies several genes potentially{{qualify evidence}} connected with political ideology.<ref>{{Cite journal | last1 = Hatemi | first1 = P. K. | last2 = Gillespie | first2 = N. A. | last3 = Eaves | first3 = L. J. | last4 = Maher | first4 = B. S. | last5 = Webb | first5 = B. T. | last6 = Heath | first6 = A. C. | last7 = Medland | first7 = S. E. | last8 = Smyth | first8 = D. C. | last9 = Beeby | first9 = H. N. | last10 = Gordon | first10 = S. D. | last11 = Montgomery | first11 = G. W. | last12 = Zhu | first12 = G. | last13 = Byrne | first13 = E. M. | last14 = Martin | first14 = N. G. | title = A Genome-Wide Analysis of Liberal and Conservative Political Attitudes | doi = 10.1017/S0022381610001015 | journal = The Journal of Politics | volume = 73 | pages = 271–285 | year = 2011 | citeseerx = 10.1.1.662.2987 }}</ref>

==Evolutionary psychology==
From an ] perspective, conflicts regarding ] may have been a recurrent issue in the ancestral environment. Humans may therefore have developed psychological mechanisms for judging their chance of succeeding in such conflicts which will affect their political views. For males, physical strength may have been an important factor in deciding the outcome of such conflicts. Therefore, a prediction is that males that have high physical strength and low ] (SES) will support redistribution while males that have both high SES and high physical strength will oppose redistribution. Cross-cultural research found this to be the case; for females, their physical strength had no influence on their political views which was as expected since females rarely have physical strength above that of the average male.<ref name=AEP>Michael Bang Petersen. The evolutionary psychology of Mass Politics. In {{Cite book | last1 = Roberts | first1 = S. C. | editor1-last = Roberts | doi = 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199586073.001.0001 | editor1-first = S. Craig | title = Applied Evolutionary Psychology | year = 2011 | publisher = Oxford University Press| isbn = 9780199586073 }}</ref> A study on political attitudes among ] actors found that, while the actors were generally more left-leaning, male actors with great physical strength were more likely to support the Republican stance on foreign issues and foreign military interventions.<ref>{{cite web | url=https://www.telegraph.co.uk/news/newstopics/howaboutthat/9197597/Strong-men-more-likely-to-vote-Conservative.html | title=Strong men more likely to vote Conservative | publisher=] | date=April 11, 2012 | access-date=September 25, 2012}}</ref>

An alternative evolutionary explanation for political diversity is that it is a ], like those of gender and blood type, resulting from ]. Tim Dean has suggested that we live in such a ] whereby the advantage gained by having any one particular moral strategy diminishes as it becomes very common, causing evolution to produce individuals with a diversity of moral strategies.<ref name=Dean2012>{{Cite journal | last1 = Dean | first1 = T. | title = Evolution and Moral Diversity | doi = 10.4148/biyclc.v7i0.1775 | journal = The Baltic International Yearbook of Cognition, Logic and Communication | volume = 7 | year = 2012 | doi-access = free }}</ref> Alford ''et al.'' posit that political variation could offer groups different strategies of solving problems, thus variation is maintained by virtue of being adaptive at the group level.<ref>Alford, John R., Carolyn L. Funk, and John R. Hibbing. "Are political orientations genetically transmitted?." American political science review 99, no. 2 (2005): 153-167.</ref>

==Criticism==
Studies linking genes and biology with political ideology have been criticised. Evan Charney publishing in '']'' argues that Alford et al.'s research is methodologically flawed, their data does not support their conclusions, and the creation of 'liberal' and 'conservative' ']' is untenable. <ref>{{cite journal |last1=Charney |first1=Evan |title=Genes and Ideologies |journal=Perspectives on Politics |date=9 June 2008 |volume=6 |issue=2 |doi=10.1017/S1537592708080626 |url=https://cambridge.org/core/journals/perspectives-on-politics/article/abs/genes-and-ideologies/40C24984B31AE45C8E166BADD107A48D |access-date=2024-01-08}}</ref> Furthermore, Doron Shultziner has also criticised the methodology and interpretation of ] used in the research supporting a connection between genetics and political ideology; arguing that identical twins are more likely to have similar political views is because they react to the environment in the same way. Shultziner further argues that the usage of the term "heritable" in the research has been questionable. <ref>{{cite journal |last1=Shultziner |first1=Doron |title=Genes and Politics: A New Explanation and Evaluation of Twin Study Results and Association Studies in Political Science |journal=Political Analysis |date=2013 |volume=21 |issue=3 |doi=10.1093/pan/mps035 |url=https://www.cambridge.org/core/journals/political-analysis/article/abs/genes-and-politics-a-new-explanation-and-evaluation-of-twin-study-results-and-association-studies-in-political-science/7F2F1CB976B7DD2D89CC254802C09659 |access-date=2024-01-08}}</ref>
==See also== ==See also==
* ]
* ]
* ]
{{Subject bar|portal1=Neuroscience|portal2=Psychology|portal3=Politics}}
* ]
* ]


==References== ==References==
{{reflist|2}} {{reflist|30em}}


==Further reading== ==Further reading==
* . "JSTOR."
* - The New York Times,
* . ''The New York Times''.
* - ABC
* . ABC News
* - The Washington Post
* ''The Washington Post''
* . ''The Economist'', October 6, 2012.
* {{Cite journal |date=May 2014 |title=Genetic Influences on Political Ideologies: Twin Analyses of 19 Measures of Political Ideologies from Five Democracies and Genome-Wide Findings from Three Populations |journal=Behavior Genetics |language=en |volume=44 |issue=3 |pages=282–294 |doi=10.1007/s10519-014-9648-8 |pmid=24569950 |pmc=4038932 |issn=0001-8244 |last1=Hatemi |first1=Peter K. |last2=Medland |first2=Sarah E. |last3=Klemmensen |first3=Robert |last4=Oskarsson |first4=Sven |last5=Littvay |first5=Levente |last6=Dawes |first6=Christopher T. |last7=Verhulst |first7=Brad |last8=McDermott |first8=Rose |last9=Nørgaard |first9=Asbjørn Sonne |last10=Klofstad |first10=Casey A. |last11=Christensen |first11=Kaare |last12=Johannesson |first12=Magnus |last13=Magnusson |first13=Patrik K. E. |last14=Eaves |first14=Lindon J. |last15=Martin |first15=Nicholas G. }}

==External links==
*

{{Political spectrum}}
{{Portal bar|Psychology|Politics}}


]
{{DEFAULTSORT:Differences Between Conservative And Liberal Brain}}
]
]
] ]
]
]
] ]
] ]

Latest revision as of 10:53, 8 January 2025

Correlation between human biology and political tendencies
Part of the Politics series
Politics
Primary topics
Political systems
Academic disciplines
Public administration
Policy
Government branches
Related topics
Subseries
icon Politics portal

A number of studies have found that human biology can be linked with political orientation. This means that an individual's biology may predispose them to a particular political orientation and ideology or, conversely, that subscription to certain ideologies may predispose them to measurable biological and health outcomes.

Studies have found that subjects with right-wing (or conservative in the United States) political views have larger amygdalae, report larger social networks and greater happiness than liberals, are more prone to express disgust to moral infringements and are more sensitive to perceived threats. Those with left-wing (or liberal in the United States) political views are more likely to report greater relationship dissatisfaction and emotional distress than conservatives, to show more openness to experience as well as greater tolerance for uncertainty and disorder.

Genetic factors account for at least some of the variation of political views. From the perspective of evolutionary psychology, conflicts regarding redistribution of wealth may have been common in the ancestral environment and humans may have developed psychological mechanisms for judging their own chances of succeeding in such conflicts. These mechanisms may affect political views. However, many of the studies linking biology to politics remain controversial and unreplicated.

Brain studies

In the 2011 study by neuroscientist Ryota Kanai (n=90 students), the subjects who expressed conservative views (right) tended to have a larger amygdala than those who expressed liberal views (left).

A 2011 study by cognitive neuroscientist Ryota Kanai at University College London found structural brain differences between subjects of different political orientation in a convenience sample of students at the same college. The researchers performed MRI scans on the brains of 90 volunteer students who had indicated their political orientation on a five-point scale ranging from "very liberal" to "very conservative".

Students who reported more conservative political views were found to have larger amygdala, a structure in the temporal lobes whose primary function is in the formation, consolidation and processing of memory, as well as positive and negative conditioning (emotional learning). The amygdala is responsible for important roles in social interaction, such as the recognition of emotional cues in facial expressions and the monitoring of personal space, with larger amygdalae correlating with larger and more complex social networks. It is also postulated to play a role in threat detection, including modulation of fear and aggression to perceived threats. Conservative students were also found to have greater volume of gray matter in the left insula and the right entorhinal cortex. There is evidence that conservatives are more sensitive to disgust and one role of the insula is in the modulation of social emotions, such as the feeling of disgust to specific sights, smells and norm violations.

Students who reported more liberal political views were found to have a larger volume of grey matter in the anterior cingulate cortex, a structure of the brain associated with emotional awareness and the emotional processing of pain. The anterior cingulate cortex becomes active in situations of uncertainty, and is postulated to play a role in error detection, such as the monitoring and processing of conflicting stimuli or information.

The authors concluded, "Although our data do not determine whether these regions play a causal role in the formation of political attitudes, they converge with previous work to suggest a possible link between brain structure and psychological mechanisms that mediate political attitudes." In an interview with LiveScience, Ryota Kanai said, "It's very unlikely that actual political orientation is directly encoded in these brain regions", and that "more work is needed to determine how these brain structures mediate the formation of political attitude." Kanai and colleagues added that it is necessary to conduct a longitudinal study to determine whether the changes in brain structure that we observed lead to changes in political behavior or whether political attitudes and behavior instead result in changes of brain structure.

A 2024 study by Petalas et al achieved a partial replication of Kanai et al, using a larger sample size of 928 subjects, making it the largest preregistered replication study in political neuroscience to date. A positive relationship between the size of the amygdala and right-wing political views was found but at approximately a third of the effect size of the original study (r = 0.068 vs r = 0.23). The study also did not find a replication of the original finding of a positive relationship between a larger volume of grey matter in the anterior cingulate cortex and left-wing political view.

Functional differences

Psychometrics

Various studies suggest measurable differences in the psychological traits of liberals and conservatives. Conservatives are more likely to report larger social networks, greater happiness and self-esteem than liberals, are more reactive to perceived threats and more likely to interpret ambiguous facial expressions as threatening. Liberals are more likely to report greater emotional distress, relationship dissatisfaction and experiential hardship than conservatives, and show more openness to experience as well as greater tolerance for uncertainty and disorder.

Behavioral studies

A study by David Amodio et al. at New York University and the University of California, Los Angeles, found differences in how self-described liberal and conservative research participants responded to changes in patterns. Participants were asked to tap a keyboard when the letter "M" appeared on a computer monitor and to refrain from tapping when they saw a "W". The letter "M" appeared four times more frequently than "W", conditioning participants to press the keyboard when a letter appears. Liberal participants made fewer mistakes than conservatives during testing and their electroencephalograph readings showed more activity in the anterior cingulate cortex, the part of the brain that deals with conflicting information, during the experiment, suggesting that they were better able to detect conflicts in established patterns. Amodio warned against concluding that a particular political orientation is superior. He said: "The tendency of conservatives to block distracting information could be a good thing depending on the situation."

A 2017 study both replicated the original study and also found that conservatives performed better in a task in which choosing the simple strategy was the more optimal solution; while both liberals and conservatives started the task attempting the more complex but less effective strategy, conservatives switched to the simple strategy more quickly than liberals. Amodio's original study was also replicated by Weissflog et al. (2013) and Inzlicht et al. (2009). Conversely, Kremláček et al. (2019) and Wendell (2016) did not replicate Amodio's results. Both studies also argued that Weissflog and Inzlicht's results were not as concordant with the Amodio's findings as originally claimed.

A study of subjects' reported level of disgust linked to various scenarios showed that people who scored highly on the "disgust sensitivity" scale held more politically conservative views, which some researchers believe could be partially explained by personality traits. However, the findings of a 2019 study suggest that sensitivity to disgust among conservatives varies according to the elicitors used, and that using an elicitor-unspecific scale caused the differences in sensitivity to disappear between those of different political orientations.

A 2018 study in the United States looking at levels of cognitive reflection (the tendency to favour analytic reasoning over instinctive or "gut" responses) found that those who voted for Donald Trump in the 2016 US presidential election had lower levels of cognitive reflection than Hillary Clinton voters or third-party voters. However, this effect was mostly driven by Democrats who voted for Trump, while amongst Republicans, Clinton and Trump voters had more similar levels of cognitive reflection. Republicans who voted for third-party candidates or those who identified as libertarian had the highest levels of cognitive reflection.

Physiology

People with right-wing views had greater skin conductance response, indicating greater sympathetic nervous system response, to threatening images than those with left-wing views in one study. There was no difference for positive or neutral images. Holding right-wing views was also associated with a stronger startle reflex as measured by strength of eyeblink in response to unexpected noise. Subsequent studies with substantially greater statistical power have failed to replicate these effects.

In an fMRI study published in Social Neuroscience, three different patterns of brain activation were found to correlate with individualism, conservatism, and radicalism. In general, fMRI responses in several portions of the brain have been linked to viewing of the faces of well-known politicians. Others believe that determining political affiliation from fMRI data is overreaching.

Genetic studies

Heritability

Heritability compares differences in genetic factors in individuals to the total variance of observable characteristics ("phenotypes") in a population, to determine the heritability coefficient. Factors including genetics, environment and random chance can all contribute to the variation in individuals' phenotypes.

The use of twin studies assumes the elimination of non-genetic differences by finding the statistical differences between monozygotic (identical) twins, which have almost the same genes, and dizygotic (fraternal) twins. The similarity of the environment in which twins are reared has been questioned.

A twin study in 2005 by Alford et al. examined the attitudes regarding 28 different political issues such as capitalism, unions, X-rated movies, abortion, school prayer, divorce, property taxes, and the draft. Twins were asked if they agreed or disagreed or were uncertain about each issue. Genetic factors accounted for 53% of the variance of an overall score. However, self-identification as Republican and Democrat had a much lower heritability of 14%. It is worthwhile to note that identical twins correlated in opinion at a rate of 0.66 while fraternal twins correlated in opinion by 0.44. This likely occurs because identical twins share 100% of their DNA while fraternal twins share on average only 50% of their DNA. However, Jonathan Kaplan argued that the role of individual genes is often extremely small due to many human physical traits being polygenic and may be overstated, observing that the study by Alford et al. made a case for the role of the 5-HTTLPR region being involved in numerous psychological and personality traits, yet Border et al. (2019) found that multiple associations with 5-HTTLPR were spurious and underpowered.

Jost et al. wrote in a 2011 review that "Many studies involving quite diverse samples and methods suggest that political and religious views reflect a reasonably strong genetic basis, but this does not mean that ideological proclivities are unaffected by personal experiences or environmental factors."

Gene association studies

In 2014, a study was performed on genomic data from 12,000 twin pairs from Australia, the USA, Denmark, Sweden and Hungary to examine genetic influences on political ideology. The study's genome-wide association analysis did not provide any definitive evidence of a specific genetic marker related to ideology. The authors remarked that, as with any complex trait, a single gene or small group of genes would not influence ideology directly but that there would likely be "thousands of genetic variants of very small effects and constellations of genes interacting with each other and the environment to influence behavior, indirectly".

"A Genome-Wide Analysis of Liberal and Conservative Political Attitudes" by Peter K. Hatemi et al. traces DNA research involving 13,000 subjects. The study identifies several genes potentially connected with political ideology.

Evolutionary psychology

From an evolutionary psychology perspective, conflicts regarding redistribution of wealth may have been a recurrent issue in the ancestral environment. Humans may therefore have developed psychological mechanisms for judging their chance of succeeding in such conflicts which will affect their political views. For males, physical strength may have been an important factor in deciding the outcome of such conflicts. Therefore, a prediction is that males that have high physical strength and low socioeconomic status (SES) will support redistribution while males that have both high SES and high physical strength will oppose redistribution. Cross-cultural research found this to be the case; for females, their physical strength had no influence on their political views which was as expected since females rarely have physical strength above that of the average male. A study on political attitudes among Hollywood actors found that, while the actors were generally more left-leaning, male actors with great physical strength were more likely to support the Republican stance on foreign issues and foreign military interventions.

An alternative evolutionary explanation for political diversity is that it is a polymorphism, like those of gender and blood type, resulting from frequency-dependent selection. Tim Dean has suggested that we live in such a moral ecosystem whereby the advantage gained by having any one particular moral strategy diminishes as it becomes very common, causing evolution to produce individuals with a diversity of moral strategies. Alford et al. posit that political variation could offer groups different strategies of solving problems, thus variation is maintained by virtue of being adaptive at the group level.

Criticism

Studies linking genes and biology with political ideology have been criticised. Evan Charney publishing in Perspectives on Politics argues that Alford et al.'s research is methodologically flawed, their data does not support their conclusions, and the creation of 'liberal' and 'conservative' 'phenotypes' is untenable. Furthermore, Doron Shultziner has also criticised the methodology and interpretation of twin studies used in the research supporting a connection between genetics and political ideology; arguing that identical twins are more likely to have similar political views is because they react to the environment in the same way. Shultziner further argues that the usage of the term "heritable" in the research has been questionable.

See also

References

  1. ^ Jost, John T.; Amodio, David M. (13 November 2011). "Political ideology as motivated social cognition: Behavioral and neuroscientific evidence" (PDF). Motivation and Emotion. 36 (1): 55–64. doi:10.1007/s11031-011-9260-7. S2CID 10675844.
  2. ^ Kanai, R; Feilden, T; Firth, C; Rees, G (2011). "Political Orientations Are Correlated with Brain Structure in Young Adults". Current Biology. 21 (8): 677–80. Bibcode:2011CBio...21..677K. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2011.03.017. PMC 3092984. PMID 21474316.
  3. ^ Y. Inbar; et al. (2008). "Conservatives are more easily disgusted than liberals" (PDF). Cognition and Emotion. 23 (4): 714–725. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.372.3053. doi:10.1080/02699930802110007. S2CID 7411404.
  4. ^ J. Jost; et al. (2006). "The end of the end of ideology" (PDF). American Psychologist. 61 (7): 651–670. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.61.7.651. PMID 17032067.
  5. ^ J. Vigil; et al. (2010). "Political leanings vary with facial expression processing and psychosocial functioning". Group Processes & Intergroup Relations. 13 (5): 547–558. doi:10.1177/1368430209356930. S2CID 59571553.
  6. ^ J. Jost; et al. (2003). "Political conservatism as motivated social cognition" (PDF). Psychological Bulletin. 129 (3): 339–375. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.129.3.339. PMID 12784934. S2CID 1778256.
  7. Dolan, Eric W. (2024-04-10). "Genetic variations help explain the link between cognitive ability and liberalism". PsyPost - Psychology News. Retrieved 2024-04-12.
  8. Kleppesto, Thomas Haarklau; Czajkowski, Nikolai Olavi; Sheehy-Skeffington, Jennifer; Vassend, Olav; Roysamb, Espen; Eftedal, Nikolai Haahjem; Kunst, Jonas R.; Ystrom, Eivind; Thomsen, Lotte (2024-02-22). "The genetic underpinnings of right-wing authoritarianism and social dominance orientation explain political attitudes beyond Big Five personality". Journal of Personality. doi:10.1111/jopy.12921. hdl:10852/111202. ISSN 0022-3506. PMID 38386613.
  9. Buchen, Lizzie (2012). "Biology and ideology: The anatomy of politics". Nature. 490 (7421): 466–468. Bibcode:2012Natur.490..466B. doi:10.1038/490466a. PMID 23099382.
  10. Davies, James (1983). "The Proper Biological Study of Politics". Political Psychology. 4 (4): 731–743. doi:10.2307/3791065. JSTOR 3791065.
  11. ^ Ninh, Amie (April 8, 2011). "Liberal vs. Conservative: Does the Difference Lie in the Brain?". Time.
  12. Carlson, Neil R. (12 January 2012). Physiology of Behavior. Pearson. p. 364. ISBN 978-0205239399.
  13. Bzdok D, Langner R, Caspers S, Kurth F, Habel U, Zilles K, Laird A, Eickhoff SB (January 2011). "ALE meta-analysis on facial judgments of trustworthiness and attractiveness". Brain Structure & Function. 215 (3–4): 209–23. doi:10.1007/s00429-010-0287-4. PMC 4020344. PMID 20978908.
  14. Kennedy DP, Gläscher J, Tyszka JM, Adolphs R (October 2009). "Personal space regulation by the human amygdala". Nature Neuroscience. 12 (10): 1226–7. doi:10.1038/nn.2381. PMC 2753689. PMID 19718035.
  15. Bickart KC, Wright CI, Dautoff RJ, Dickerson BC, Barrett LF (February 2011). "Amygdala volume and social network size in humans". Nature Neuroscience. 14 (2): 163–4. doi:10.1038/nn.2724. PMC 3079404. PMID 21186358.
  16. Szalavitz, Maia (28 December 2010). "How to Win Friends: Have a Big Amygdala?". Time. Archived from the original on 17 July 2011. Retrieved 30 December 2010.
  17. T.L. Brink. (2008) Psychology: A Student Friendly Approach. "Unit 4: The Nervous System". pp 61.
  18. Feinstein JS, Adolphs R, Damasio A, Tranel D (January 2011). "The human amygdala and the induction and experience of fear". Current Biology. 21 (1): 34–8. Bibcode:2011CBio...21...34F. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2010.11.042. PMC 3030206. PMID 21167712.
  19. Staut CC, Naidich TP (April 1998). "Urbach-Wiethe disease (Lipoid proteinosis)". Pediatric Neurosurgery. 28 (4): 212–4. doi:10.1159/000028653. PMID 9732251. S2CID 46862405.
  20. Sanfey AG, Rilling JK, Aronson JA, Nystrom LE, Cohen JD (June 2003). "The neural basis of economic decision-making in the Ultimatum Game". Science. 300 (5626): 1755–8. Bibcode:2003Sci...300.1755S. doi:10.1126/science.1082976. PMID 12805551. S2CID 7111382.
  21. B. Wicker; et al. (2003). "Both of us disgusted in My insula: The common neural basis of seeing and feeling disgust". Neuron. 40 (3): 655–664. doi:10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00679-2. PMID 14642287.
  22. Wright P, He G, Shapira NA, Goodman WK, Liu Y (October 2004). "Disgust and the insula: fMRI responses to pictures of mutilation and contamination". NeuroReport. 15 (15): 2347–51. doi:10.1097/00001756-200410250-00009. PMID 15640753. S2CID 6864309.
  23. Lane RD, Reiman EM, Axelrod B, Yun LS, Holmes A, Schwartz GE (July 1998). "Neural correlates of levels of emotional awareness. Evidence of an interaction between emotion and attention in the anterior cingulate cortex". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 10 (4): 525–35. doi:10.1162/089892998562924. PMID 9712681. S2CID 27743177.
  24. Price DD (June 2000). "Psychological and neural mechanisms of the affective dimension of pain". Science. 288 (5472): 1769–72. Bibcode:2000Sci...288.1769P. doi:10.1126/science.288.5472.1769. PMID 10846154. S2CID 15250446.
  25. H. Critchley; et al. (2001). "Neural activity in the human brain relating to uncertainty and arousal during anticipation". Neuron. 29 (2): 537–545. doi:10.1016/s0896-6273(01)00225-2. hdl:21.11116/0000-0001-A313-1. PMID 11239442. S2CID 10995076.
  26. Bush G, Luu P, Posner MI (June 2000). "Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 4 (6): 215–222. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01483-2. PMID 10827444. S2CID 16451230.
  27. "Politics on the Brain: Scans Show Whether You Lean Left or Right". LiveScience. 7 April 2011. Retrieved September 25, 2012.
  28. Kattalia, Kathryn (April 8, 2011). "The liberal brain? Scans show liberals and conservatives have different brain structures". New York Daily News. Archived from the original on July 10, 2012. Retrieved September 25, 2012.
  29. Petalas, D. P., Schumacher, G., & Scholte, S. H. (2024). Is political ideology correlated with brain structure? A preregistered replication. iScience.
  30. ^ Napier J; Jost JT (2008). "Why Are Conservatives Happier Than Liberals?". Psychological Science. 19 (6): 562–72. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02124.x.
  31. David M Amodio, John T Jost, Sarah L Master & Cindy M Yee, Neurocognitive correlates of liberalism and conservatism, Nature Neuroscience. Cited by 69 other studies
  32. "Brains of Liberals, Conservatives May Work Differently". Psych Central. 2007-10-20. Archived from the original on 2016-10-13.
  33. "Study finds left-wing brain, right-wing brain". Los Angeles Times. 2007-09-10.
  34. Bernabel, R. T., & Oliveira, A. (2017). Conservatism and liberalism predict performance in two nonideological cognitive tasks. Politics and the Life Sciences, 36(2), 49-59.
  35. Weissflog, M., Choma, B. L., Dywan, J., van Noordt, S. J., & Segalowitz, S. J. (2013). The political (and physiological) divide: Political orientation, performance monitoring, and the anterior cingulate response. Social neuroscience, 8(5), 434-447.
  36. Inzlicht, Michael, Ian McGregor, Jacob B. Hirsh, and Kyle Nash. "Neural markers of religious conviction." Psychological science 20, no. 3 (2009): 385-392.
  37. Kremláček, Jan, Daniel Musil, Jana Langrová, and Martin Palecek. "Neural correlates of liberalism and conservatism in a post-Communist country." Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 13 (2019): 119.
  38. Wendell, D. G. (2016). Loyola eCommons Neural Correlates of Political Attitudes: Emotion and Ideology in the Brain. Available online at: http://ecommons.luc.edu/luc_diss/2299, Loyola University Chicago, pages 133-178 Accessed 27/07/2023
  39. Xu, Xiaowen, Annika K. Karinen, Hanah A. Chapman, Jordan B. Peterson, and Jason E. Plaks. "An orderly personality partially explains the link between trait disgust and political conservatism." Cognition and Emotion (2019).
  40. Elad-Strenger, Julia, Jutta Proch, and Thomas Kessler. "Is Disgust a "Conservative" Emotion?." Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin (2019): 0146167219880191.
  41. Pennycook, Gordon, and David G. Rand. "Cognitive reflection and the 2016 US Presidential election." Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 45, no. 2 (2019): 224-239.
  42. Oxley, D. R.; Smith, K. B.; Alford, J. R.; Hibbing, M. V.; Miller, J. L.; Scalora, M.; Hatemi, P. K.; Hibbing, J. R. (19 September 2008). "Political Attitudes Vary with Physiological Traits". Science. 321 (5896): 1667–1670. Bibcode:2008Sci...321.1667O. doi:10.1126/science.1157627. PMID 18801995. S2CID 7215269.
  43. Bakker, Bert N.; Schumacher, Gijs; Gothreau, Claire; Arceneaux, Kevin (June 2020). "Conservatives and liberals have similar physiological responses to threats". Nature Human Behaviour. 4 (6): 613–621. doi:10.1038/s41562-020-0823-z. PMC 7306406. PMID 32042109.
  44. Fournier, Patrick; Soroka, Stuart; Nir, Lilach (August 2020). "Negativity Biases and Political Ideology: A Comparative Test across 17 Countries". American Political Science Review. 114 (3): 775–791. doi:10.1017/S0003055420000131. S2CID 225441710.
  45. Zamboni G, Gozzi M, Krueger F, Duhamel JR, Sirigu A, Grafman J (2009). "Individualism, conservatism, and radicalism as criteria for processing political beliefs: a parametric fMRI study". Social Neuroscience. 4 (5): 367–83. doi:10.1080/17470910902860308. PMID 19562629. S2CID 15652205. Zamboni G, Gozzi M, Krueger F, Duhamel JR, Sirigu A, Jordan Grafman. National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD, USA
  46. Kristine Knudson; et al. (March 2006). "Politics on the Brain: An fMRI Investigation". Soc Neurosci. 1 (1): 25–40. doi:10.1080/17470910600670603. PMC 1828689. PMID 17372621.
  47. Aue T, Lavelle LA, Cacioppo JT (July 2009). "Great expectations: what can fMRI research tell us about psychological phenomena?" (PDF). International Journal of Psychophysiology. 73 (1): 10–6. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.12.017. PMID 19232374.
  48. Raj, A; van Oudenaarden, A (2008). "Nature, nurture, or chance: stochastic gene expression and its consequences". Cell. 135 (2): 216–26. doi:10.1016/j.cell.2008.09.050. PMC 3118044. PMID 18957198.
  49. Martin, Nicholas; Boomsma, Dorret; Machin, Geoffrey (17 December 1997). "A twin-pronged attack on complex traits" (PDF). Nature Genetics. 17 (4): 387–92. doi:10.1038/ng1297-387. PMID 9398838. S2CID 2028886.
  50. Beckwith, Jon; Morris, Corey A. (December 2008). "Twin Studies of Political Behavior: Untenable Assumptions?". Perspectives on Politics. 6 (4): 785–91. doi:10.1017/S1537592708081917. S2CID 55630117.
  51. Fiske, Susan T.; Gilbert, Daniel T.; Lindzey, Gardner (15 February 2010). Handbook of Social Psychology (PDF) (5th ed.). John Wiley & Sons. p. 372.
  52. Carey, Benedict (June 21, 2005). "Some Politics May Be Etched in the Genes". The New York Times. Retrieved September 25, 2012.
  53. Alford, J. R.; Funk, C. L.; Hibbing, J. R. (2005). "Are Political Orientations Genetically Transmitted?". American Political Science Review. 99 (2): 153–167. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.622.476. doi:10.1017/S0003055405051579. S2CID 3820911.
  54. Kincaid, Harold, and Jeroen Van Bouwel, eds. The Oxford Handbook of Philosophy of Political Science. Oxford University Press, 2023, pg 19-32
  55. Border, Richard, Emma C. Johnson, Luke M. Evans, Andrew Smolen, Noah Berley, Patrick F. Sullivan, and Matthew C. Keller. "No support for historical candidate gene or candidate gene-by-interaction hypotheses for major depression across multiple large samples." American Journal of Psychiatry 176, no. 5 (2019): 376-387.
  56. Hatemi, PK; Medland, SE; Klemmensen, R; Oskarrson, S; Littvay, L; Dawes, C; Verhulst, B; McDermott, R; Nørgaard, AS; Klofstad, C; Christensen, K; Johannesson, M; Magnusson, PKE; Eaves, LJ; Martin, NG (2014). "Genetic Influences on Political Ideologies: Twin Analyses of 19 Measures of Political Ideologies from Five Democracies and Genome-Wide Findings from Three Populations". Behav Genet. 44 (3): 282–294. doi:10.1007/s10519-014-9648-8. PMC 4038932. PMID 24569950.
  57. Hatemi, P. K.; Gillespie, N. A.; Eaves, L. J.; Maher, B. S.; Webb, B. T.; Heath, A. C.; Medland, S. E.; Smyth, D. C.; Beeby, H. N.; Gordon, S. D.; Montgomery, G. W.; Zhu, G.; Byrne, E. M.; Martin, N. G. (2011). "A Genome-Wide Analysis of Liberal and Conservative Political Attitudes". The Journal of Politics. 73: 271–285. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.662.2987. doi:10.1017/S0022381610001015.
  58. Michael Bang Petersen. The evolutionary psychology of Mass Politics. In Roberts, S. C. (2011). Roberts, S. Craig (ed.). Applied Evolutionary Psychology. Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199586073.001.0001. ISBN 9780199586073.
  59. "Strong men more likely to vote Conservative". The Telegraph. April 11, 2012. Retrieved September 25, 2012.
  60. Dean, T. (2012). "Evolution and Moral Diversity". The Baltic International Yearbook of Cognition, Logic and Communication. 7. doi:10.4148/biyclc.v7i0.1775.
  61. Alford, John R., Carolyn L. Funk, and John R. Hibbing. "Are political orientations genetically transmitted?." American political science review 99, no. 2 (2005): 153-167.
  62. Charney, Evan (9 June 2008). "Genes and Ideologies". Perspectives on Politics. 6 (2). doi:10.1017/S1537592708080626. Retrieved 2024-01-08.
  63. Shultziner, Doron (2013). "Genes and Politics: A New Explanation and Evaluation of Twin Study Results and Association Studies in Political Science". Political Analysis. 21 (3). doi:10.1093/pan/mps035. Retrieved 2024-01-08.

Further reading

External links

Political spectrum
Political positions
Political ideologies
  • Anarchism
  • Marxism
  • Communism
  • Socialism
  • Social democracy
  • Liberalism
  • Christian democracy
  • Conservatism
  • Nationalism
  • Fascism
  • Nazism
  • Religious politics
  • Buddhist socialism
  • Christian left
  • Jewish left
  • Islamic socialism
  • Christian right
  • Hindutva
  • Islamism
  • Religious Zionism
  • Political pejoratives
  • Baizuo
  • Champagne socialist
  • Hard left
  • Liberal elite
  • Loony left
  • Moonbat
  • Red fascist
  • Regressive left
  • Social fascism
  • Social imperialism
  • Social justice warrior
  • Soft left
  • Wingnut
  • Models
  • Horseshoe theory
  • Left–right political spectrum
  • Nolan Chart
  • Overton window
  • Open–closed political spectrum
  • Pournelle chart
  • Right-wing authoritarianism
  • See also
  • Anti-authoritarianism
  • Democratic transition
  • Libertarianism
  • Syndicalism
  • Progressivism
  • Green politics
  • Identity politics
  • Corporatism
  • Authoritarianism
  • Totalitarianism
  • Revolutionary
  • Reactionary
  • Anti-establishment
  • Collectivism
  • Communitarianism
  • The Establishment
  • Individualism
  • Hardline
  • Populism
  • Radical politics
  • Triangulation
  • Portals: Categories: