Misplaced Pages

Lie detection: Difference between revisions

Article snapshot taken from Wikipedia with creative commons attribution-sharealike license. Give it a read and then ask your questions in the chat. We can research this topic together.
Browse history interactively← Previous editContent deleted Content addedVisualWikitext
Revision as of 15:39, 3 March 2013 edit88.151.79.116 (talk) 21st century: typo fix← Previous edit Latest revision as of 00:18, 25 December 2024 edit undoBattyBot (talk | contribs)Bots1,934,080 editsm Non-verbal behavior: Fixed citation wikilink(s) and general fixes, replaced: | via=pressreader → | via=PressReaderTag: AWB 
(432 intermediate revisions by more than 100 users not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
{{Short description|Assessment of statements to reveal deceit}}
'''Lie detection''', also referred to as deception detection, uses questioning techniques along with technology that records physiological functions to ascertain truth and falsehood in response. It is commonly used by law enforcement and has historically been an inexact science. There are a wide variety of technologies available for this purpose.<ref name=Parliament>{{cite web|title=Detecting Deception|url=http://www.parliament.uk/briefing-papers/POST-PN-375|publisher=Parliamentary Office of Science and Technology (UK)|accessdate=26 April 2012}}</ref> The most common and long used measure is the ], which is considered by the ] to be unreliable.<ref name=Adelson>{{cite journal|last=Adelson|first=Rachel|title=Detecting Deception|journal=Monitor on Psychology|year=2004|month=July|volume=37|issue=7|pages=70|url=http://www.apa.org/monitor/julaug04/detecting.aspx|accessdate=26 April 2012|publisher=American Psychological Association}}</ref>
{{Lead rewrite|date=February 2022|reason=there is a mismatch between the article body and the lead about accuracy}}

'''Lie detection''' is an assessment of a verbal statement with the goal to reveal a possible intentional deceit. Lie detection may refer to a cognitive process of detecting deception by evaluating message content as well as non-verbal cues.<ref>{{cite book|first1=Pär Anders|last1=Granhag|first2=Aldert|last2=Vrij|first3=Bruno|last3=Verschuere|title=Detecting Deception: Current Challenges and Cognitive Approaches (Wiley Series in Psychology of Crime, Policing and Law)|year=2015|pages=205|publisher=Wiley-Blackwell|location=Hoboken, NJ|isbn=978-1118509661}}</ref> It also may refer to questioning techniques used along with technology that record physiological functions to ascertain truth and falsehood in response. The latter is commonly used by law enforcement in the United States, but rarely in other countries because it is based on ].

There are a wide variety of technologies available for this purpose.<ref name="Parliament">{{cite web|title=Detecting Deception|url=https://post.parliament.uk/research-briefings/post-pn-375/|publisher=Parliamentary Office of Science and Technology (UK)|access-date=26 April 2012}}</ref> The most common and long used measure is the ]. A comprehensive 2003 review by the ] of existing research concluded that there was "little basis for the expectation that a polygraph test could have extremely high accuracy."<ref name="Polygraph and Lie Detection 2003">{{Cite book|url=https://www.nap.edu/catalog/10420/the-polygraph-and-lie-detection|title=The Polygraph and Lie Detection|publisher=National Research Council|year=2003| doi=10.17226/10420 |isbn=978-0-309-26392-4|language=en}}</ref>{{rp|2,212}} There is no evidence to substantiate that ] lie detection, such as by looking at body language, is an effective way to detect lies, even if it is widely used by law enforcement.<ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Vrij|first1=Aldert|last2=Hartwig|first2=Maria|last3=Granhag|first3=Pär Anders|date=2019|title=Reading Lies: Nonverbal Communication and Deception|journal=Annual Review of Psychology|volume=70|issue=1|pages=295–317|doi=10.1146/annurev-psych-010418-103135|pmid=30609913| s2cid=58562467 |issn=0066-4308| url=https://researchportal.port.ac.uk/portal/en/publications/reading-lies-nonverbal-communication-and-deception(3d64cf66-725e-4cb5-ae5a-5619417b1452).html |doi-access=free}}</ref><ref name="Seigel">{{cite journal |last1=Seigel |first1=Jessica |title=The truth about lying |journal=Knowable Magazine |date=25 March 2021 |doi=10.1146/knowable-032421-1 |url=https://knowablemagazine.org/article/mind/2021/the-truth-about-lying |access-date=8 December 2021 |doi-access=free}}</ref>

==General accuracy and limitations of assessment==

The cumulative research evidence suggests that machines do detect deception better than chance, but with significant error rates<ref name=USCongress>{{cite journal|title=Scientific Validity of Polygraph Testing: A Research Review and Evaluation{{snd}}A Technical Memorandum|journal=Washington, DC: U.S. Congress, Office of Technology Assessment|date=November 1983|issue=OTA-TM-H-15|url=http://ota.fas.org/reports/8320.pdf|access-date=3 September 2016}}</ref> and that strategies used to "beat" polygraph examinations, so-called countermeasures, may be effective.<ref>. ].</ref> Despite unreliability, results are admissible in court in some countries, such as Japan. Lie detector results are very rarely admitted in evidence in the US courts.<ref>{{cite web|url=https://www.usatoday.com/news/nation/2002-09-09-lie_x.htm|title=Telling the truth about lie detectors|work=usatoday.com}}</ref>

In 1983 the U.S. Congress Office of Technology Assessment published a review of the technology<ref name=USCongress/> and found:
:"...there is at present only limited scientific evidence for establishing the validity of polygraph testing. Even where the evidence seems to indicate that polygraph testing detects deceptive subjects better than chance, significant error rates are possible, and examiner and examinee differences and the use of countermeasures may further affect validity."<ref>{{Skeptoid|id=4422|number=422|date=8 July 2014|title=Lie Detection|access-date=22 June 2017}}</ref>

In the 2007 peer-reviewed academic article "Charlatanry in forensic speech science", the authors reviewed 50 years of lie detector research and came to the conclusion that there is no scientific evidence supporting that voice analysis lie detectors actually work.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Eriksson | first1 = A. | last2 = Lacerda | first2 = F. | year = 2007 | title = Charlantry in forensic speech science: A problem to be taken seriously | url = http://equinoxjournals.com/ojs/index.php/IJSLL/article/viewArticle/3775 | journal = International Journal of Speech, Language and the Law | volume = 14 | issue = 2 | doi = 10.1558/ijsll.2007.14.2.169 | access-date = 2009-01-28 | archive-date = 2010-01-13 | archive-url = https://web.archive.org/web/20100113045751/http://equinoxjournals.com/ojs/index.php/IJSLL/article/viewArticle/3775 | url-status = dead }}</ref> Lie detector manufacturer Nemesysco threatened to sue the academic publisher for libel resulting in removal of the article from online databases. In a letter to the publisher, Nemesysco's lawyers wrote that the authors of the article could be sued for defamation if they wrote on the subject again.<ref> {{Webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20090413003343/http://www.su.se/english/about/news_and_events/scientists_threatened_with_legal_action |date=2009-04-13 }}. ].</ref><ref> {{webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20101207025139/http://www.thelocal.se/17188/20090127 |date=2010-12-07 }}. '']''. January 27, 2009.</ref><ref>Online at: {{cite journal|author1=Anders Eriksson|author2=Francisco Lacerda|title=Charlatanry in forensic speech science: A problem to be taken seriously|journal=International Journal of Speech, Language and the Law|date=14 February 2007|volume=14.2 2007|page=169|url=https://www.cs.columbia.edu/~julia/papers/eriksson&lacerda07.pdf|access-date=3 September 2016|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20160802124557/http://www.cs.columbia.edu/~julia/papers/eriksson%26lacerda07.pdf|archive-date=2 August 2016|url-status=dead}}</ref>

Nevertheless, extraneous "noise" on the polygraph can come from embarrassment or anxiety and not be specific to lying.<ref name=Preston /> When subjects are aware of the assessment their resulting emotional response, especially anxiety, can impact the data. Additionally, ] can cause problems with data as certain disorders can lead a person to make a statement they believe to be truth but is actually a fabrication. As well as with all testing, the examiner can cause biases within the test with their interaction with the subject and interpretation of the data.<ref name=Parliament />


==History== ==History==
The study of physiological methods for deception tests measuring emotional disturbances began in the early 1900s. Benussi was the first to work on practical deception tests based on physiological changes. He detected changes in inspiration-expiration ratio—findings confirmed by N.E. Burtt. Burtt conducted studies that emphasized the changes in quantitative systolic blood-pressure. E.H. Marston studied blood-pressure and noted increase in systolic blood pressure of 10&nbsp;mm Hg or over indicated guilt through using the tycos sphygmomanometer, with which he reported 90-100% accuracy. His studies used students and actual court cases. Then in 1913 W.M. Marston determined systolic blood-pressure by oscillatory methods and his findings cite definite changes in blood pressure during the deception of criminal suspects. In 1921 Larson criticized Marston's intermittent blood pressure method because emotional changes were so brief they could be lost. To adjust for this he modified the Erlanger sphymograph to give a continuous blood pressure and pulse curve and used it to study 4,000 criminals.<ref name=Keeler>{{cite journal|last=Keeler|first=Leonarde|title=A Method for Detecting Deception|journal=American Journal of Police Science|year=1930|month=January|volume=1|issue=1|pages=38–51|url=http://www.jstor.org/stable/1147254|accessdate=26 April 2012}}</ref>


==21st century== ===20th century===
The study of physiological methods for deception tests measuring emotional disturbances began in the early 1900s. ] was the first to work on practical deception tests based on physiological changes. He detected changes in inspiration-expiration ratio—findings confirmed by N.E. Burtt. Burtt conducted studies that emphasized the changes in quantitative systolic blood-pressure. ] studied blood-pressure and noted increase in systolic blood pressure of 10&nbsp;mm Hg or over indicated guilt through using the tycos ], with which he reported 90–100% accuracy. His studies used students and actual court cases. Then in 1913 W.M. Marston determined systolic blood-pressure by oscillatory methods and his findings cite definite changes in blood pressure during the deception of criminal suspects. In 1921, ] criticized Marston's intermittent blood pressure method because emotional changes were so brief they could be lost. To adjust for this he modified the Erlanger ] to give a continuous blood pressure and pulse curve and used it to study 4,000 criminals.<ref name=Keeler>{{cite journal|last=Keeler|first=Leonarde|title=A Method for Detecting Deception|journal=American Journal of Police Science|date=January 1930|volume=1|issue=1|pages=38–51|doi=10.2307/1147254|jstor=1147254}}</ref> In the 1990s, a team of scientists, Stanley Abrams, Jean M. Verdier and Oleg Maltsev developed a new methodology contributing six coefficients that positively affect the accuracy of the lie detector analysis results.<ref>{{cite web |title=Historical Perspective: Some Polygraph Techniques |url=https://westernmorning.news/2018/10/03/historical-perspective-some-polygraph-techniques/ |website=Western Morning News |access-date=26 August 2020 |date=3 October 2018}}</ref>
A meta-analysis study from DePaulo and Morris found an "association between lying and increased pupil size, an indicator of tension and concentration." Additionally, those lying are perceived to appear more nervous than those telling the truth (which may be because the voices of those lying are higher pitched), while they also do not appear to be more fidgety, blink more, or have a less-relaxed posture but "are more likely than truth-tellers to press their lips together." However, highly motivated liars (those with higher stakes) "seem unusually still and make notably less eye contact with listeners."<ref name=Adelson/>


===21st century===
] has used the ] (FACS) and "when combined with voice and speech measures, reaches detection accuracy rates of up to 90 percent." However, there is currently no evidence to support such a claim. It is currently being automated for use in law enforcement and is still being improved to increase accuracy. His studies use micro-expressions, which last less than one-fifth of a second, and "may leak emotions someone wants to conceal, such as anger or guilt." However, "signs of emotion aren't necessarily signs of guilt. An innocent person may be apprehensive and appear guilty" Ekman reminds us.
Two meta-analyses conducted by 2004 found an association between lying and increased pupil size and compressed lips. Liars may stay still more, use fewer hand gestures, and make less eye contact. Liars may take more time to answer questions but on the other hand, if they have had time to prepare, they may answer more quickly than people telling the truth would, and talk less, and repeat phrases more. They do not appear to be more fidgety, blink more, or have a less-relaxed posture.<ref name="Adelson">{{cite journal|last=Adelson|first=Rachel|date=July 2004|title=Detecting Deception|url=http://www.apa.org/monitor/julaug04/detecting.aspx|journal=Monitor on Psychology|volume=37|issue=7|page=70|access-date=26 April 2012}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Bond CF |first1=Jr |last2=DePaulo |first2=BM |title=Accuracy of deception judgments. |journal=Personality and Social Psychology Review |date=2006 |volume=10 |issue=3 |pages=214–234 |doi=10.1207/s15327957pspr1003_2 |pmid=16859438|s2cid=15248114 }}</ref><ref>{{cite book |last1=DePaulo |first1=Bella M. |last2=Morris |first2=Wendy L. |editor1-last=Granhag |editor1-first=Pär Anders |editor2-last=Strömwall |editor2-first=Leif A. |title=The detection of deception in forensic contexts |date=2004 |publisher=Cambridge University Press |location=Cambridge |isbn=978-0521541572 |edition=Transferred to digital print. |chapter=Chapter 2: Discerning lies from truth: behavioural cues to deception and the indirect pathway of intuition |url-access=registration |url=https://archive.org/details/detectionofdecep0000unse }}</ref>
With regard to his studies, lies about emotions at the moment have the biggest payoff from face and voice cues while lies about beliefs and actions, such as crimes use cues from gestures and words are added. Ekman and his associates have validated many signs of deception, but do not publish all of them as not to educate criminals<ref name=Adelson />


] has used the ] (FACS) and "when combined with voice and speech measures, reaches detection accuracy rates of up to 90 percent." However, there is currently no evidence to support such a claim. It is currently being automated for use in law enforcement and is still being improved to increase accuracy. His studies use micro-expressions, which last less than one-fifth of a second, and "may leak emotions someone wants to conceal, such as anger or guilt." However, "signs of emotion aren't necessarily signs of guilt. An innocent person may be apprehensive and appear guilty," Ekman reminds us.
Depaulo and his graduate student Morris have been studying the verbal and written output of liars to find distinctive patterns. They have found that "liars take longer to start answering questions than truth-tellers--but when they have time to plan, liars actually start their answers more quickly than truth-tellers. And they talk less." When considering the perception of others, "liars seem more negative--more nervous and complaining, and less cooperative--than truth-tellers" and they additionally seem to withhold more information. Individuals lying sound "more discrepant and ambivalent, the structure of their stories is less logical, and their stories sound less plausible." Additionally, it has been observed that they are more likely than those telling the truth to repeat words and phrases, but they also use fewer hand movements to aid in the description of their actions.<ref name=Adelson />
With regard to his studies, lies about emotions at the moment have the biggest payoff from face and voice cues while lies about beliefs and actions, such as crimes use cues from gestures and words are added. Ekman and his associates have validated many signs of deception, but do not publish all of them so as not to educate criminals<ref name=Adelson />


] uses the method of ] and Word Count (LIWC), published by ], to conduct an analysis of written content. He claims it has accuracy in predicting lying. Pennebaker cites his method as "significantly more effective than human judges in correctly identifying deceptive or truthful writing samples"; there is a 67% accuracy rate with his method, while trained people have 52% accuracy. His studies have identified that deception carries three primary written markers. The first is fewer first-person pronouns. Those lying "avoid statements of ownership, distance themselves from their stories and avoid taking responsibility for their behavior" while also using more negative emotion words such as "hate, worthless and sad." Second, they use "few exclusionary words such as except, but or nor" when "distinguish what they did from what they did not do."<ref name=Adelson /> ] uses the method of ] and Word Count (LIWC), published by ], to conduct an analysis of written content. He claims it has accuracy in predicting lying. Pennebaker cites his method as "significantly more effective than human judges in correctly identifying deceptive or truthful writing samples"; there is a 67% accuracy rate with his method, while trained people have 52% accuracy. There were five experimental procedures used in this study. Study 1–3 asked participants to speak, hand write or type a true or false statement about abortion. The participants were randomly assigned to tell a true or false statement. Study 4 focused on feelings about friends and study 5 had the students involved in a mock crime and asked to lie. Human judges were asked to rate the truthfulness of the 400 communications dealing with abortion. The judges read or watched the statement and gave it a yes or no answer about if this statement was false or not. LIWC correctly classified 67% of the abortion communications and the judges correctly classified 52%. His studies have identified that deception carries three primary written markers. The first is fewer first-person pronouns such as 'I', 'me', 'my', 'mine', and 'myself' (singular), as well as 'we', 'us', 'our', and 'ourselves' (plural). Those lying "avoid statements of ownership, distance themselves from their stories and avoid taking responsibility for their behavior" while also using more ] words such as "hate, worthless and sad." Second, they use "few exclusionary words such as except, but or nor" when "distinguish what they did from what they did not do."<ref name=Adelson />


More recently evidence has been provided by the work of CA Morgan III and GA Hazlett that a computer analysis of cognitive interview derived speech content (i.e. response length and unique word count) provides a method for detecting deception that is both demonstrably better than professional judgments of professionals and useful at distinguishing between genuine and false adult claims of exposure to highly stressful, potentially traumatic events.<ref>{{cite journal|title=Efficacy of forensic statement analysis in distinguishing truthful from deceptive eyewitness accounts of highly stressful events|author=Morgan CA 3rd |journal= Journal of Forensic Sciences|display-authors=etal|pmid=21854383|doi=10.1111/j.1556-4029.2011.01896.x|volume=56|issue=5 |year=2011|pages=1227–1234|s2cid=25246267 }}</ref> This method shows particular promise as it is non confrontational as well as scientifically and cross culturally valid.
==General questioning and testing techniques==

There are typically three types of questions used:
==Questioning and testing techniques==
{{see also|Polygraph#Testing procedure}}
There are typically three types of questions used in polygraph testing or voice stress analysis testing:


Irrelevant questions establish a baseline to compare other answers by asking simple questions with clear true and false answers. Irrelevant questions establish a baseline to compare other answers by asking simple questions with clear true and false answers.
Line 21: Line 40:
Comparison questions have an indirect relationship to the event or circumstance, and they are designed to encourage the subject to lie. Comparison questions have an indirect relationship to the event or circumstance, and they are designed to encourage the subject to lie.


Relevant questions are compared against comparison questions (which should represent false answers) and irrelevant questions (which should represent true answers). They are about whatever is particularly in question.<ref name=Parliament /> Relevant questions are compared against comparison questions (which should represent false answers) and irrelevant questions (which should represent true answers). They are about whatever is particularly in question.


The ''control question test'' (CQT) uses control questions, with known answers, to serve as a physiological baseline in order to compare them with questions relevant to a particular incident. The control question should have a greater physiological response if truth was told and a lesser physiological response for lying.<ref name=Preston>{{cite journal|last=Preston|first=Elizabeth|title=Detecting Deception|journal=Observer|date=July 2002|volume=15|issue=6|url=http://www.psychologicalscience.org/index.php/uncategorized/detecting-deception.html|access-date=26 April 2012}}</ref> The ''guilty knowledge test'' (GKT) is a multiple-choice format in which answer choices or one correct answer and additional incorrect answers are read and the physiological response is recorded. The controls are the incorrect alternative answers. The greater physiological response should be to the correct answer.<ref name=Preston /> Its point is to determine if the subject has knowledge about a particular event.<ref name=Parliament/>
==The control question test and the guilty knowledge test==
The Control Question Test (CQT) uses control questions, with known answers, to serve as a physiological baseline in order to compare them with questions relevant to a particular incident. The control question should have a greater physiological response if truth was told and a lesser physiological response for lying.<ref name=Preston>{{cite journal|last=Preston|first=Elizabeth|title=Detecting Deception|journal=Observer|year=2002|month=July|volume=15|issue=6|url=http://www.psychologicalscience.org/index.php/uncategorized/detecting-deception.html|accessdate=26 April 2012}}</ref>
The Guilty Knowledge Test (GKT) is a multiple-choice format in which answer choices or one correct answer and additional incorrect answers are read and the physiological response is recorded. The controls are the incorrect alternative answers. The greater physiological response should be to the correct answer.<ref name=Preston /> Its point is to determine if the subject has knowledge about a particular event.<ref name=Parliament/>


In addition to the test skewing towards not finding people innocent, there are also issues where some offenders might have a greater physiological response to the control question than to the specific question, making it difficult to determine guilt using this method even when people are not using specific techniques to try and trick the test.<ref name="Iacono 1295–1308">{{Cite journal|last=Iacono|first=William G.|date=October 2008|title=Effective Policing: Understanding How Polygraph Tests Work and Are Used|url=http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/0093854808321529|journal=Criminal Justice and Behavior|language=en|volume=35|issue=10|pages=1295–1308|doi=10.1177/0093854808321529| s2cid=143302792 |issn=0093-8548}}</ref> Although the issues with the CQT false-positive and false-negative rates are discussed above, there are also methodological issues with how proponents of the CQT determine the accuracy of the test.<ref name="Iacono 1295–1308"/> Due to the fact that the accuracy of the CQT is often determined through whether an individual who is given the test provides the police a confession to a crime after the test is administered, this means that cases where someone was cleared of charges after taking a polygraph or, in a worst-case scenario, gives a false confession when they are actually innocent are not taken into account when it comes to determining the accuracy of the test.<ref name="Iacono 1295–1308"/> Another issue is that, due to how the CQT is administered and how the lie-detection process works, only people who are determined to be deceptive are further interrogated for a confession.<ref name="Iacono 1295–1308"/> This means that the polygraph outcome and the confession are not independent of one another, making it very difficult to use confessions as the sole determiner of the accuracy of the test.<ref name="Iacono 1295–1308"/> These methodological problems provide false evidence that supports the continued use of this test, despite the many flaws that the test possesses.<ref name="Iacono 1295–1308"/> While it could be said that including this test as a police tool is useful because it might sometimes provide accurate information, the probability of it causing undue hardship to people who are actually innocent, and wasting time in the process, makes this a very unreliable method for law enforcement officers to use.<ref name="Iacono 1295–1308"/>
Both are considered to be bias against those that are innocent for several reasons. Fear of consequences of the subject determines performance while strategies such as mental arithmetic (which at least one study has found to be ineffective, especially in students counting backward by seven), curling the toes, and biting the tongue can be used to skew responses—all of which can be found on the internet to teach individuals how to change the results of the tests. However, a study has found that in The Guilty Knowledge Test subjects can use the alternative answers their targets by focusing on them and making themselves look innocent.<ref name=Preston/>

Both are considered to be biased against those that are innocent, because the guilty who fear the consequences of being found out can be more motivated to cheat on the test. Various techniques (which can be found online) can teach individuals how to change the results of the tests, including curling the toes, and biting the tongue. ] was found to be ineffective by at least one study, especially in students counting backward by seven. A study has found that in the guilty knowledge test subjects can focusing on the alternative answers and make themselves look innocent.<ref name=Preston/>


==Polygraph== ==Polygraph==
{{main|Polygraph}}
]


Lie detection commonly involves the ]. The traditional lie detector test, or polygraph, is widely considered unreliable. It detects ] reactions.<ref name=Adelson /> These changes in body functions are not easily controlled by the conscious mind and include bodily reactions like skin conductivity and ].<ref>. BBC Science & Nature.</ref> They also may consider ], ], ], and muscular movement. These measures are supposed to indicate a short-term ] which can be from lying or significance to the subject. The problem becomes that they are also associated with mental effort, and ]; so they can be influenced by fear, anger, and surprise for example. This technique may also be used with CQT and GKT.<ref name=Parliament /> Lie detection commonly involves the ],<ref name=":0" /> and is used to test both styles of deception. It detects ] reactions,<ref name="Adelson" /> such as micro-expressions, breathing rate, skin conductivity, and ].<ref>. BBC Science & Nature.</ref> Micro-expressions are the brief and incomplete nonverbal changes in expression while the rest show an activation of the nervous system.<ref name=":0" /> These changes in body functions are not easily controlled by the conscious mind. They also may consider ], ], capillary dilation, and muscular movement. While taking a polygraph test the subject wears a blood pressure device to measure blood pressure fluctuations. Respiration is measured by wearing pneumographs around the chest, and finally electrodes are placed on the subject's fingers to measure skin conductivity. To determine truth it is assumed the subject will show more signs of fear when answering the control questions, known to the examiner, compared with the relevant questions, where the answers are not known. Polygraphs focus more on the exams predictive value of guilt by comparing the responses of the participant to control questions, irrelevant questions, and relevant questions to gauge arousal, which is then interpreted as a display of fear and deception is assumed.<ref name=":0" /> If a person is showing a deception there will be changes in the autonomic arousal responses to the relevant questions. Results are considered inconclusive if there is no fluctuation in any of the questions.<ref>{{cite web|title=The Truth About Lie Detectors (aka Polygraph Tests)| url=http://www.apa.org/research/action/polygraph.aspx|website=apa.org|access-date=4 September 2016}}</ref>


These measures are supposed to indicate a short-term ] which can be from lying or significance to the subject. The problem becomes that they are also associated with mental effort and ], so they can be influenced by fear, anger, and surprise for example. This technique may also be used with CQT and GKT.<ref name=Parliament />
Activities of the body not easily controlled by the conscious mind are compared under different circumstances. Usually this involves asking the subject control questions where the answers are known to the examiner and comparing them to questions where the answers are not known. Critics claim that "lie detection" by use of ] has no scientific validity because it is not a scientific procedure.<ref></ref> Government agencies, such as the ], ], ], and even the ] currently use polygraphs. They are regularly used by these agencies to screen employees.<ref name=Eye-Tracking>{{cite web|title=Education psychologists use eye0tracking method for detecting lies|url=http://www.news-medical.net/news/20100712/Educational-psychologists-use-eye-tracking-method-for-detecting-lies.aspx|publisher=psychologialscience.org|accessdate=26 April 2012}}</ref> The problem with evaluating the effectiveness of polygraphs through field studies is that the use of confessions overestimates accuracy. Someone who has failed the test is more likely to confess than someone who has passed, contributing to polygraph examiners not learning about mistakes they have made and thus improving.<ref name=Preston />


United States government agencies, such as the ], ], ], and even the ] currently use polygraphs. They are regularly used by these agencies to screen employees.<ref name=Eye-Tracking>{{cite web|title=Education psychologists use eye0tracking method for detecting lies|url=http://www.news-medical.net/news/20100712/Educational-psychologists-use-eye-tracking-method-for-detecting-lies.aspx|publisher=psychologialscience.org|access-date=26 April 2012|date=2010-07-12}}</ref>
==Cognitive polygraph==
Recent developments that permit non-invasive monitoring using ] technique showed that successful problem-solving employs a discrete knowledge strategy (DKS) that selects neural pathways represented in one hemisphere, while unsuccessful outcome implicates a non-discrete knowledge strategy (nDKS).<ref>Njemanze, P. C. (2005). Cerebral lateralization and general intelligence: Gender differences in a transcranial Doppler study. ''Brain and Language'', '''92''', 234–239</ref> A polygraphic test could be viewed as a working memory task. This suggests that the DKS model may have a correlate in mnemonic operations. In other words, the DKS model may have a discrete knowledge base (DKB) of essential components needed for task resolution, while for nDKS, DKB is absent and, hence, a "global" or bi-hemispheric search occurs. Based on the latter premise, a 'lie detector' system was designed as described in {{US Patent|6390978|United State Patent No. 6,390,979}}. A pattern of blood-flow-velocity changes is obtained in response to questions that include correct and incorrect answers. The wrong answer will elicit bi-hemispheric activation, from correct answer that activates unilateral response. Cognitive polygraphy based on this system is devoid of any subjective control of mental processes and, hence, high reliability and specificity; however, this is yet to be tested in forensic practice. See also cognitive ].


Critics claim that "lie detection" by use of ] has no scientific validity because it is not a scientific procedure.<ref></ref> People have found ways to try and cheat the system, such as taking sedatives to reduce anxiety; using antiperspirant to prevent sweating; and positioning pins or biting parts of the mouth after each question to demonstrate a constant physiological response.<ref>{{cite web|title=Polygraph Testing|url=https://epic.org/privacy/polygraph/|website=epic.org |publisher=Electronic Privacy Information Center|access-date=23 September 2016}}</ref> As technology and research have developed many have moved away from polygraphing because of the drawbacks of this style of detection. Supporters of polygraphing claim it has a 70% accuracy rate, 16% better than lie detection in the general population.<ref name=":1">{{Cite journal|doi = 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00594|title = Prospects of functional magnetic resonance imaging as lie detector|year = 2013|last1 = Rusconi|first1 = Elena|last2 = Mitchener-Nissen|first2 = Timothy|journal = Frontiers in Human Neuroscience|volume = 7|page = 594|pmid = 24065912|pmc = 3781577| doi-access=free }}</ref> Someone who has failed the test is more likely to confess than someone who has passed, contributing to ]s not learning about mistakes they have made and thus improving.<ref name=Preston />
==ERP==
] assess recognition, and therefore may or may not be effective in assessing deception. In ERP studies P3 amplitude waves are assessed, with these waves being large when an item is recognized.<ref name=Preston />
However, P100 amplitudes have been observed to have significant correlation to trustworthiness ratings, which the importance of will be discussed in the EEG section. This, along with other studies leads some to purport that because ERP studies rely on quick perceptual processes they "are integral to the detection of deception."<ref name=Heussen />


==Voice stress analysis==
==EEG==
] (also called voice risk analysis) uses computers to compare ], ], intensity and micro tremors. In this way voice analysis "detect minute variations in the voice thought to signal lying." It can even be used covertly over the phone, and has been used by banking and insurance companies as well as the government of the United Kingdom. Customers are assessed for truth in certain situations by banks and insurance companies where computers are used to record responses. Software then compares control questions to relevant questions assessed for deception. However, its reliability has been debated by peer-reviewed journals.<ref name=Parliament /> "When a person lies, an involuntary interference of the nerves causes the vocal cords to produce a distorted sound wave, namely a frequency level which is different from the one produced by the same person when telling the truth."<ref>S. Manes. "Lie Detector. (Lie Detector Software Truster)." Forbes, Oct. 5, 1998</ref>
], or EEG, measures brain activity through electrodes attached to the scalp of a subject. The object is to identify the recognition of meaningful data through this activity. Images or objects are shown to the subject while questioning techniques are implemented to determine recognition. This can include crime scene images, for example.<ref name=Parliament />


Several studies published in peer reviewed journals showed VSA to perform at chance level when it comes to detecting deception. Horvath, McCloughan, Weatherman, and Slowik, (2013),<ref>Horvath, F., McCloughan, J., Weatherman, D., & Slowik, S. (2013). "The Accuracy of auditors' and layered voice Analysis (LVA) Operators' Judgments of Truth and Deception During Police Questioning". ''Journal of Forensic Sciences'', 58, 385–392.</ref> for example, tested VSA on the recordings of interrogation of 74 suspects. Eighteen of these suspects later confessed, making the deception the most likely ground truth. With 48% accurate classification, VSA performed at chance level. Several other studies showed similar results (Damphousse, 2008; Harnsberger, Hollien, Martin, & Hollien, 2009).<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Damphousse | year = 2008 | title = Voice stress analysis: Only 15 percent of lies about drug use detected in field test | url =https://www.nij.gov/journals/259/pages/voice-stress-analysis.aspx | journal = NIJ Journal | volume = 259 | pages = 8–12 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Harnsberger | first1 = J. D. | last2 = Hollien | first2 = H. | last3 = Martin | first3 = C. A. | last4 = Hollien | first4 = K. A. | year = 2009 | title = Stress and Deception in Speech: Evaluating Layered Voice Analysis | journal = Journal of Forensic Sciences | volume = 54 | issue = 3| pages = 642–650 | doi=10.1111/j.1556-4029.2009.01026.x| pmid = 19432740 | s2cid = 205767994 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Hollien | first1 = H. | last2 = Harnsberger | first2 = J. D. | last3 = Martin | first3 = C. A. | last4 = Hollien | first4 = K. A. | year = 2008 | title = Evaluation of the NITV CVSA | journal = Journal of Forensic Sciences | volume = 53 | issue = 1| pages = 183–193 | doi=10.1111/j.1556-4029.2007.00596.x| pmid = 18279255 | s2cid = 205767046 }}</ref> In 2003, the National Research Council concluded "Overall, this research and the few controlled tests conducted over the past decade offer little or no scientific basis for the use of the computer voice stress analyser or similar voice measurement instruments."<ref name="Polygraph and Lie Detection 2003"/>{{RP|168}}
Perceived trustworthiness is interpreted by the individual from looking at a face, and this decreases when someone is lying. Such observations are "too subtle to be explicitly processed by observers, but does affect implicit cognitive and affective processes." These results, in a study by Heussen, Binkofski, and Jolij, were obtained through a study with an N400 paradigm including two conditions within the experiment: truthful faces and lying faces. Faces flashed for 100ms and then the participants rated them. However, the limitations of this study would be that it only had 15 participants and the ] age was 24.<ref name=Heussen>{{cite journal|last=Heussen|first=Yana|coauthors=Ferdinand Binkofski, Jacob Jolij|title=The semantics of the lying face - An EEG Study|journal=Internation Journal of Psychophysiology|year=2010|month=September|volume=77|issue=3|pages=206|doi=10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2010.06.006|url=http://www.sciencedirect.com.ezproxy.lib.vt.edu:8080/science/article/pii/S0167876010001509|accessdate=26 April 2012}}</ref>


==Facial EMG== ==Non-verbal behavior==
People often evaluate lies based on non-verbal behavior, but are quick to place too much merit in misleading indicators, such as: avoidance of eye contact, increased pauses between statements, and excessive movements originating from the hands or feet.<ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Akehurst|last2=Koehnken|title=Lay persons' and police officers' beliefs regarding deceptive behavior.|journal=Applied Cognitive Psychology|volume=10|issue=6|pages=461–471|doi=10.1002/(sici)1099-0720(199612)10:6<461::aid-acp413>3.3.co;2-u|year=1996}}</ref>
]
Devices such as the ] monitor large numbers of ]s over time slots and encodes them into large vectors which are classified as showing truthful or deceptive behavior by artificial intelligence or statistical classifiers.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Rothwell | first1 = J. | last2 = Bandar | first2 = Z. | last3 = O'Shea | first3 = J. | last4 = McLean | first4 = D. | year = 2006| title = Silent talker: a new computer-based system for the analysis of facial cues to deception | journal = Journal of Applied Cognitive Psychology | volume = 20 | issue = 6| pages = 757–777 | doi = 10.1002/acp.1204 | doi-access = free }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Rothwell | first1 = J. McLean | last2 = Bandar | first2 = J. | last3 = O'Shea | first3 = Z. | s2cid = 8968052 | year = 2006 | title = Charting the behavioural state of a person using a Backpropagation Neural Network | journal = Journal of Neural Computing and Applications | volume = 16| issue = 4–5| pages = 327–339| doi = 10.1007/s00521-006-0055-9 }}</ref>


Dr. Alan Hirsch, from the department of Neurology and Psychiatry at the Rush Presbyterian-St. Luke's Medical Center in Chicago, explained the "Pinocchio syndrome" or "Pinocchio effect" as: blood rushes to the nose when people lie. This extra blood may make the nose itchy. As a result, people who stretch the truth tend to either scratch their nose or touch it more often.<ref>{{cite web| url=https://www.pressreader.com/canada/the-telegram-st-johns/20100726/281689726095936| title=Can 'Pinocchio Syndrome' solve the niqab dilemma?| author=Dr. Game W. Gifford Jones| date=26 July 2010| accessdate=12 August 2021| publisher=]| via=]}}</ref>
==Eye-tracking==
John Kircher, Doug Hacker, Anne Cook, Dan Woltz and David Raskin have developed ] at the ] that they consider a polygraph alternative. This is not an emotional reaction like the polygraph and other methods but rather a cognitive reaction. This technology measures pupil dilation, response time, reading and rereading time, and errors. Data is recorded while subjects answer true false questions on a computer.<ref name=Eye-Tracking />


===Eye-tracking===
They have found that more effort is required by lying than giving the truth and thus their aim to find indications of hard work. Individuals not telling the truth might, for instance, have dilated pupils while also taking longer to answer the question.<ref name=Eye-Tracking />
John Kircher, Doug Hacker, Anne Cook, Dan Woltz and David Raskin have developed ] at the ] that they consider a polygraph alternative. This is not an emotional reaction like the polygraph and other methods but rather a cognitive reaction. This technology measures pupil dilation, response time, reading and rereading time, and errors. Data is recorded while subjects answer true or false questions on a computer.<ref name=Eye-Tracking />


Eye-tracking offers several benefits over the polygraph: lower cost, 1/5th of the time to conduct, subjects do not need to be "hooked up" to anything, and it does not require qualified polygraph examiners to give the test.<ref name=Eye-Tracking /> They have found that more effort is required by lying than giving the truth and thus their aim is to find indications of hard work. Individuals not telling the truth might, for instance, have dilated pupils while also taking longer to answer the question.<ref name=Eye-Tracking />


Eye-tracking claims to offer several benefits over the polygraph: lower cost, 1/5th of the time to conduct, subjects do not need to be "hooked up" to anything, and it does not require qualified polygraph examiners to give the test.<ref name=Eye-Tracking /> The technology has not been subject to peer review.
==Voice analysis==
Voice analysis uses computers to compare ], ], intensity and micro tremors. In this way voice analysis "detect minute variations in the voice thought to signal lying." It can even be used covertly over the phone, and has been used by banking and insurance companies as well as the government of the United Kingdom. Customers are assessed for truth in certain situations by banks and insurance companies where computers are used to record responses. Software then compares control questions to relevant questions assessed for deception. However, its reliability has been debated by peer-reviewed journals.<ref name=Parliament />


==Brain observations==
==fMRI==
], or the measurement of the time taken to perform mental operations, can be used to distinguish lying from truth-telling. One recent instrument using cognitive chronometry for this purpose is the ], or TARA.
Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging is a technique used for multiple purposes which shows the uses of oxygen by the brain, allowing for the identification of which portions of the brain are using more oxygen, and thus being used during a specific task. The technique is also used in drug development, a wide-variety of research, and diagnostically.<ref name=Parliament />


] uses ] and the multiple voxels activated in the brain evoked by a stimulus to determine what the brain has detected, and so whether it is familiar.
Studies using ] (fMRI) have shown that it has potential to be used as a method of lie detection.<ref>Spence SA, et al. Behavioural and functional anatomical correlates of deception in humans. Neuroreport. 2001 Sep 17;12(13):2849-53.</ref><ref>Langleben DD, et al. Brain activity during simulated deception: an event-related functional magnetic resonance study. Neuroimage. 2002 Mar;15(3):727-32.</ref><ref>Lee TM, et al. Lie detection by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Hum Brain Mapp. 2002 Mar;15(3):157-64.</ref><ref>Ganguli, I. (2007). Watching the Brain Lie: Can fMRI replace the polygraph? ''The Scientist'', '''21''', 40</ref> While a polygraph detects changes in activity in the ], fMRI has the potential to catch the lie at the 'source'. However, the neurobiological systems that relate to lying are currently poorly understood. The current consensus is that faced with a forced choice paradigm, in which a subject has the choice of telling the truth or spontaneously generating a lie, lying can be distinguished due to increased ] and ] activity. More specifically, the ]] and inferolateral prefrontal ] show net activation in the process of spontaneous lie generation (which involves suppression of the truthful response as well as generating a conceivable lie). Subjects are often offered monetary incentive if they can successfully deceive the process in hopes of generating a 'real world' scenario. Using this method, an initial 2005 study on individuals ( not group averages as previous studies) without pattern recognition and automation showed that lies can be distinguished 78% of the time.<ref name="Telling Truth From Lie in Individual Subjects With Fast Event-Related fMRI">Langleben, D., Loughead, J., Bilker, W., Ruparel, K., Childress, A., Busch, S., and Gur, R.(2005). Using pattern recognition training on this same initial data the accuracy became 93%. Telling truth from lie in individuals with fast event-related fMRI. ''Human Brain Mapping. 26'', 262–272.</ref> That statistic has risen, in one study, to 100% when predicting a lie in an individual when baseline lie/truth levels were closely studied with training from pattern recognition technology (machine learning). fMRI does rely upon the individual remaining still and safeguards in the analysis such that the questions can not be gamed by the participant (G. Ganis 2010). Studies have been done on Chinese individuals and their language and cultural differences did not to change results. To show the robustness of this fMRI technology, a study (S. Spence 2011) was done that showed fMRI lie detection / truth verification technology worked even in a group of 52 schizophrenic patients, 27 of whom were experiencing delusions at the time of the study.


] (fNIRS) also detects oxygen and activity in the brain like the fMRI, but instead it looks at blood oxygen levels. It is advantageous to the fMRI because it is portable, however its image resolution is of lower quality than the fMRI.<ref name=Parliament />
There are currently two companies ] Inc.<ref name="No Lie MRI Inc."></ref> and Cephos Corporation<ref name="Cephos Corporation"></ref> that are advancing this technology and offer it presently for commercial use. Recent attempts to introduce fMRI lie detection evidence in US Federal and State courts have been unsuccessful.
In 2007 on episode 93 of the TV program '']'', the three members of the build team attempted to fool a non-automated fMRI test done by Cephos Corporation. Although two of the members were unsuccessful at fooling Cephos, the third member was able to successfully fool Steve Laken of Cephos, according to this member by keeping his mind in constant activity. The one out of three failure rate suggested that fMRI-based lie detection required further development.<ref>http://mythbustersresults.com/episode93</ref> Also in 2007, the University of Pennsylvania group, used fMRI to test a Washington Post reporter. The reporter was asked to pretend to apply for a job using a resume that included among other true items, three specific biographical items that were false. The test was able to detect two out of 3 items the reporter lied about.<ref>Hakun, J.G. et al. (2009). Towards Clinical Trials of Lie Detection with fMRI. Neurocase 4(6)518-527. http://www.med.upenn.edu/langleben/SocialNeuro2009.pdf</ref>


As there are different styles of lying, a spontaneous or artificial deception is constructed based on a mixture of information already stored in semantic and ].<ref name=":0">{{Cite journal|doi = 10.1093/cercor/13.8.830|title = Neural Correlates of Different Types of Deception: An fMRI Investigation|year = 2003|last1 = Ganis|first1 = G.|last2 = Kosslyn|first2 = S. M.|last3 = Stose|first3 = S.|last4 = Thompson|first4 = W. L.|last5 = Yurgelun-Todd|first5 = D. A.|journal = Cerebral Cortex|volume = 13|issue = 8|pages = 830–836|pmid = 12853369|doi-access = free}}</ref> It is isolated and easier to generate because it lacks cross-checking into the larger picture. This style contrasts memorized lies that aren't as rich in detail but are retrieved from memory.<ref name=":0" /> They often fit into an actual scenario to make recall easier.
==fNIRS==
] also detects oxygen and activity in the brain like the fMRI, but instead it looks at blood oxygen levels. It is advantageous to the fMRI because it is portable, however its image resolution is of less quality than the fMRI.<ref name=Parliament />


===Functional transcranial Doppler (fTCD)===
==Brain observations==
Recent developments that permit non-invasive monitoring using ] technique showed that successful problem-solving employs a discrete knowledge strategy (DKS) that selects neural pathways represented in one hemisphere, while unsuccessful outcome implicates a non-discrete knowledge strategy (nDKS).<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Njemanze | first1 = P. C. | s2cid = 12440401 | year = 2005 | title = Cerebral lateralization and general intelligence: Gender differences in a transcranial Doppler study | journal = Brain and Language | volume = 92 | issue = 3| pages = 234–239 | doi=10.1016/j.bandl.2004.06.104 | pmid=15721956| citeseerx = 10.1.1.532.5734 }}</ref> A polygraphic test could be viewed as a working memory task. This suggests that the DKS model may have a correlate in mnemonic operations. In other words, the DKS model may have a discrete knowledge base (DKB) of essential components needed for task resolution, while for nDKS, DKB is absent and, hence, a "global" or bi-hemispheric search occurs. Based on the latter premise, a 'lie detector' system was designed as described in {{US Patent|6390979|United States Patent No. 6,390,979}}. A pattern of blood-flow-velocity changes is obtained in response to questions that include correct and incorrect answers. The wrong answer will elicit bi-hemispheric activation, from correct answer that activates unilateral response. Cognitive polygraphy based on this system is devoid of any subjective control of mental processes and, hence, high reliability and specificity; however, this is yet to be tested in forensic practice. See also cognitive ].
] is used to detect changes in brain waves.


===Event-related potentials (ERP)===
] or MERMER uses ] to determine if an image is familiar to the subject. It is proposed to be used for lie detection and determination of whether a subject has specialized knowledge of the type most commonly found among spies or terrorists.
{{main|Event-related potentials}}
] assess recognition, and therefore may or may not be effective in assessing deception. In ERP studies P3 amplitude waves are assessed, with these waves being large when an item is recognized.<ref name=Preston />
However, P100 amplitudes have been observed to have significant correlation to trustworthiness ratings, the importance of which will be discussed in the EEG section. This, along with other studies leads some to purport that because ERP studies rely on quick perceptual processes they "are integral to the detection of deception."<ref name=Heussen />


===Electroencephalography (EEG)===
], or the measurement of the time taken to perform mental operations, can be used to distinguish lying from truth-telling. One recent instrument using cognitive chronometry for this purpose is the ], or TARA.
{{main|Electroencephalography}}
], or EEG, measures brain activity through electrodes attached to the scalp of a subject. The object is to identify the recognition of meaningful data through this activity. Images or objects are shown to the subject while questioning techniques are implemented to determine recognition. This can include crime scene images, for example.<ref name=Parliament />


Perceived trustworthiness is interpreted by the individual from looking at a face, and this decreases when someone is lying. Such observations are "too subtle to be explicitly processed by observers, but affect implicit cognitive and affective processes." These results, in a study by Heussen, Binkofski, and Jolij, were obtained through a study with an ] paradigm including two conditions within the experiment: truthful faces and lying faces. Faces flashed for 100ms and then the participants rated them. However, the limitations of this study would be that it only had 15 participants and the ] age was 24.<ref name=Heussen>{{cite journal|last=Heussen|first=Yana|author2=Ferdinand Binkofski |author3=Jacob Jolij |title=The semantics of the lying face – An EEG Study|journal=International Journal of Psychophysiology|date=September 2010|volume=77|issue=3|page=206|doi=10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2010.06.006}}</ref>
] uses ] and the multiple voxels activated in the brain evoked by a stimulus to determine what the brain has detected, and so whether it is familiar.


Machine learning algorithms applied to EEG data have also been used to decode whether a subject believed or disbelieved a statement reaching ~90% accuracy. This work was an extension to work by Sam Harris and colleagues and further demonstrated that belief preceded disbelief in time, suggesting that the brain may initially accept statements as valid descriptions of the world (belief) prior to rejecting this notion (disbelief). Understanding how the brain assesses the veracity of a descriptive statement may be an important step in building neuroimaging based lie detection methods.<ref name=Douglas>{{cite journal|last=Douglas|first=Pamela K |author2=Edward Lau|author3=Ariana Anderson| author4=Wesley Kerr| author5=Austin Head| author6=Margalit Wollner| author7=Daniel Moyer| author8=Michael Durnhofer| author9=Wei Li| author10=Jen Bramen| author11=Mark S Cohen| title=Single trial decoding of belief decision making from EEG and fMRI data using independent components features|journal=Frontiers in Human Neuroscience|date= 2013|volume=7|page=392|doi=10.3389/fnhum.2013.00392|pmid=23914164 |pmc=3728485 |doi-access=free}}</ref>
==Non-verbal behavior==
Non-invasive lie detection using non-verbal behavior is performed by the ]. Silent Talker monitors large numbers of ]s over time slots and encodes them into large vectors which are classified as showing truthful or deceptive behavior by artificial intelligence or statistical classifiers. Silent Talker research has been peer-reviewed in the Journal of Applied Cognitive Psychology <ref>Rothwell, J. Bandar, Z. O'Shea, J. McLean, D. Silent talker: a new computer-based system for the analysis of facial cues to deception, Journal of Applied Cognitive Psychology, Volume 20, Issue 6, pages 757–777, September 2006. {{doi|10.1002/acp.1204}} Final article format: http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/acp.1204/abstract</ref> and in the Journal of Neural Computing and Applications.<ref>Rothwell, J. Bandar, Z. O'Shea, J. McLean, D. Charting the behavioural state of a person using a Backpropagation Neural Network. Journal of Neural Computing and Applications. {{doi|10.1007/s00521-006-0055-9}}. 2006.</ref> The architecture was invented between 2000 and 2002 by a team at ].


===Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI)===
Another computer-based system for detecting spontaneous ]] is being developed at Oxford and the ] by teams directed by Tomas Pfister. Traditionally, micro-expressions are very difficult to recognize through automated ] analysis because of their short duration and involuntariness. Their short duration means only a very limited number of frames are available for analysis using a standard 25fps camera and their involuntariness means eliciting a particular expression to add to a comprehensive training database requires considerable time and psychological insights. To counteract the short-video length, the team used ] to achieve a higher number of frames and Multiple Kernel Learning to improve classification. The end result was the Spontaneous Micro-expression Corpus (SMIC), which consists of 77 micro-expressions taken from 6 subjects. This is the first system that is able to recognize spontaneous facial micro-expressions with reliable accuracy, approximately 70% compared to the 50% by trained human analysts. As such, it will be a valuable tool for future computer vision studies geared towards automating the process of lie detection.<ref>T. Pfister, X. Li, G. Zhao, M. Pietikäinen, Recognising spontaneous facial micro-expressions, Poster presented at Int'l Conference on Computer Vision (ICCV), 2011. Available from http://tomas.pfister.fi</ref>
{{main|fMRI lie detection}}


Functional magnetic resonance imaging looks to the ] to compare time and topography of activity in the brain for lie detection. While a polygraph detects changes in activity in the ], fMRI has the potential to catch the lie at the 'source'.
==Drugs==
]s such as ] and ] (historically speaking) are used for the purposes of obtaining accurate information from an unwilling subject.<ref>Kapoor, P., Chugh, Y., Kapoor, A. K. & Sinha, U. S. (2008). . ''International Journal of Medical Toxicology & Legal Medicine.'' '''10''' (2)</ref> Information obtained by publicly disclosed truth drugs has been shown to be highly unreliable, with subjects apparently freely mixing fact and fantasy.<ref>Rowell Jr (1952). Admissibility of Evidence Obtained by Scientific Devices and Analyses. ''Florida Law Review.'' '''5''' (5).</ref> Much of the claimed effect relies on the belief of the subjects that they cannot tell a lie while under the influence of the drug.


fMRIs use electromagnets to create pulse sequences in the cells of the brain. The fMRI scanner then detects the different pulses and fields that are used to distinguish tissue structures and the distinction between layers of the brain, matter type, and the ability to see growths. The functional component allows researchers to see activation in the brain over time and assess efficiency and connectivity by comparing blood use in the brain, which allows for the identification of which portions of the brain are using more oxygen, and thus being used during a specific task.<ref name="Simpson2008">{{cite journal|vauthors=Simpson JR|year=2008|title=Functional MRI lie detection: too good to be true?|journal=J. Am. Acad. Psychiatry Law|volume=36|issue=4|pages=491–498|pmid=19092066}}</ref>
==General accuracy and limitations of assessments==
FMRI data have been examined through the lens of machine learning algorithms to decode whether subjects believed or disbelieved statements, ranging from mathematical, semantic to religious belief statements.<ref name="Douglas2011">{{cite journal|last=Douglas |first=PK|year=2011|title=Performance comparison of machine learning algorithms and number of independent components used in fMRI decoding of belief vs. disbelief. |journal=NeuroImage|volume=56|issue=2|pages=544–553 |doi=10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.11.002|pmid=21073969|pmc=3099263}}</ref>
Extraneous noise can come from embarrassment or anxiety and not be specific to lying.<ref name=Preston /> When subjects are aware of the assessment their resulting emotional response, especially anxiety, can impact the data. Additionally, ] can cause problems with data as certain disorders can lead a person to make a statement they believe to be truth but is actually a fabrication. As well as with all testing, the examiner can cause biases within the test with their interaction with the subject and interpretation of the data.<ref name=Parliament />
Current research in the field focuses on manipulating the psychological and thus measuring the psychological. However, Tyler Lorig proposes that the physiological needs to be measured in order to infer the psychological in order to make the research applicable.<ref name=Preston />


Historically, fMRI lie detector tests have not been allowed into evidence in legal proceedings, the most famous attempt being Harvey Nathan's insurance fraud case<ref name=":2">{{Cite journal|pmid = 19092068|year = 2008|last1 = Langleben|first1 = D. D.|last2 = Dattilio|first2 = F. M.|title = Commentary: The future of forensic functional brain imaging|journal = The Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law|volume = 36|issue = 4|pages = 502–4}}</ref> in 2007.<ref name=":1" /> The lack of legal support has not stopped companies like No Lie MRI and CEPHOS from offering private fMRI scans to test deception.
==Controversy==
While fMRI studies on deception have claimed detection accuracy as high as 90% many have problems with implementing this style of detection. Only yes or no answers can be used which allows for flexibility<ref name=":1" /> in the truth and style of lying. Some people are unable to take one such as those with medical conditions, claustrophobia, or implants.<ref name=":1" />
In the peer-reviewed academic article "Charlatanry in forensic speech science", the authors reviewed 50 years of lie detector research and came to the conclusion that there is no scientific evidence supporting that lie detectors actually work.<ref>Eriksson, A. and Lacerda, F. (2007). . ''International Journal of Speech Language and the Law.'' '''14''' (2).</ref> Lie detector manufacturer ] threatened to sue the academic publisher for libel resulting in removal of the article from online databases. In a letter to the publisher, Nemesysco's lawyers wrote that the authors of the article could be sued for defamation if they wrote on the subject again.<ref>. ].</ref><ref>. '']''. January 27, 2009.</ref>


==Drugs==
The cumulative research evidence suggests that machines do detect deception better than chance, but with significant error rates{{Citation needed|date=January 2010}} and that strategies used to "beat" polygraph examinations, so-called countermeasures, may be effective.<ref>. ].</ref> Despite unreliability, results are ] in court in some countries such as Japan. Lie detector results are very rarely admitted in evidence in the US courts. http://www.usatoday.com/news/nation/2002-09-09-lie_x.htm
{{main|Truth serum}}

]s such as ], ], and ] (historically speaking) are used for the purposes of obtaining accurate information from an unwilling subject.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Kapoor | first1 = P. | last2 = Chugh | first2 = Y. | last3 = Kapoor | first3 = A. K. | last4 = Sinha | first4 = U. S. | year = 2008 | title = Torture, terrorist and truth serum | url = http://www.indianjournals.com/ijor.aspx?target=ijor:ijmtlm&volume=10&issue=2&article=016 | journal = International Journal of Medical Toxicology & Legal Medicine | volume = 10 | issue = 2 }}</ref> Information obtained by publicly disclosed truth drugs has been shown to be highly unreliable, with subjects apparently freely mixing fact and fantasy.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Rowell | first1 = Jr | year = 1952 | title = Admissibility of Evidence Obtained by Scientific Devices and Analyses | journal = Florida Law Review | volume = 5 | issue = 5 }}</ref> Much of the claimed effect relies on the belief of the subjects that they cannot tell a lie while under the influence of the drug.
Clark Freshman, Professor of Law at ], Hastings College of Law, studies lies in negotiations and lies involving lawyers. Together with Michael Wheeler at ], he developed a series of clips of how lies – and concealed emotions – show up in real estate negotiations. His scholarship on lies and negotiations goes well beyond ]'s original theories. Freshman and Wheeler found ] of emotion and other clues in their research on negotiation{{Citation needed|date=October 2010}}.


== See also == == See also ==
* ] * ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ] * ]
* ]


== References == == References ==
{{reflist|2}} {{reflist}}

{{Authority control}}

{{Nonverbal communication}}


] ]

Latest revision as of 00:18, 25 December 2024

Assessment of statements to reveal deceit
The article's lead section may need to be rewritten. The reason given is: there is a mismatch between the article body and the lead about accuracy. Please help improve the lead and read the lead layout guide. (February 2022) (Learn how and when to remove this message)

Lie detection is an assessment of a verbal statement with the goal to reveal a possible intentional deceit. Lie detection may refer to a cognitive process of detecting deception by evaluating message content as well as non-verbal cues. It also may refer to questioning techniques used along with technology that record physiological functions to ascertain truth and falsehood in response. The latter is commonly used by law enforcement in the United States, but rarely in other countries because it is based on pseudoscience.

There are a wide variety of technologies available for this purpose. The most common and long used measure is the polygraph. A comprehensive 2003 review by the National Academy of Sciences of existing research concluded that there was "little basis for the expectation that a polygraph test could have extremely high accuracy." There is no evidence to substantiate that non-verbal lie detection, such as by looking at body language, is an effective way to detect lies, even if it is widely used by law enforcement.

General accuracy and limitations of assessment

The cumulative research evidence suggests that machines do detect deception better than chance, but with significant error rates and that strategies used to "beat" polygraph examinations, so-called countermeasures, may be effective. Despite unreliability, results are admissible in court in some countries, such as Japan. Lie detector results are very rarely admitted in evidence in the US courts.

In 1983 the U.S. Congress Office of Technology Assessment published a review of the technology and found:

"...there is at present only limited scientific evidence for establishing the validity of polygraph testing. Even where the evidence seems to indicate that polygraph testing detects deceptive subjects better than chance, significant error rates are possible, and examiner and examinee differences and the use of countermeasures may further affect validity."

In the 2007 peer-reviewed academic article "Charlatanry in forensic speech science", the authors reviewed 50 years of lie detector research and came to the conclusion that there is no scientific evidence supporting that voice analysis lie detectors actually work. Lie detector manufacturer Nemesysco threatened to sue the academic publisher for libel resulting in removal of the article from online databases. In a letter to the publisher, Nemesysco's lawyers wrote that the authors of the article could be sued for defamation if they wrote on the subject again.

Nevertheless, extraneous "noise" on the polygraph can come from embarrassment or anxiety and not be specific to lying. When subjects are aware of the assessment their resulting emotional response, especially anxiety, can impact the data. Additionally, psychological disorders can cause problems with data as certain disorders can lead a person to make a statement they believe to be truth but is actually a fabrication. As well as with all testing, the examiner can cause biases within the test with their interaction with the subject and interpretation of the data.

History

20th century

The study of physiological methods for deception tests measuring emotional disturbances began in the early 1900s. Vittorio Benussi was the first to work on practical deception tests based on physiological changes. He detected changes in inspiration-expiration ratio—findings confirmed by N.E. Burtt. Burtt conducted studies that emphasized the changes in quantitative systolic blood-pressure. William Moulton Marston studied blood-pressure and noted increase in systolic blood pressure of 10 mm Hg or over indicated guilt through using the tycos sphygmomanometer, with which he reported 90–100% accuracy. His studies used students and actual court cases. Then in 1913 W.M. Marston determined systolic blood-pressure by oscillatory methods and his findings cite definite changes in blood pressure during the deception of criminal suspects. In 1921, John Augustus Larson criticized Marston's intermittent blood pressure method because emotional changes were so brief they could be lost. To adjust for this he modified the Erlanger sphygmograph to give a continuous blood pressure and pulse curve and used it to study 4,000 criminals. In the 1990s, a team of scientists, Stanley Abrams, Jean M. Verdier and Oleg Maltsev developed a new methodology contributing six coefficients that positively affect the accuracy of the lie detector analysis results.

21st century

Two meta-analyses conducted by 2004 found an association between lying and increased pupil size and compressed lips. Liars may stay still more, use fewer hand gestures, and make less eye contact. Liars may take more time to answer questions but on the other hand, if they have had time to prepare, they may answer more quickly than people telling the truth would, and talk less, and repeat phrases more. They do not appear to be more fidgety, blink more, or have a less-relaxed posture.

Paul Ekman has used the Facial Action Coding System (FACS) and "when combined with voice and speech measures, reaches detection accuracy rates of up to 90 percent." However, there is currently no evidence to support such a claim. It is currently being automated for use in law enforcement and is still being improved to increase accuracy. His studies use micro-expressions, which last less than one-fifth of a second, and "may leak emotions someone wants to conceal, such as anger or guilt." However, "signs of emotion aren't necessarily signs of guilt. An innocent person may be apprehensive and appear guilty," Ekman reminds us. With regard to his studies, lies about emotions at the moment have the biggest payoff from face and voice cues while lies about beliefs and actions, such as crimes use cues from gestures and words are added. Ekman and his associates have validated many signs of deception, but do not publish all of them so as not to educate criminals

James Pennebaker uses the method of Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count (LIWC), published by Lawrence Erlbaum, to conduct an analysis of written content. He claims it has accuracy in predicting lying. Pennebaker cites his method as "significantly more effective than human judges in correctly identifying deceptive or truthful writing samples"; there is a 67% accuracy rate with his method, while trained people have 52% accuracy. There were five experimental procedures used in this study. Study 1–3 asked participants to speak, hand write or type a true or false statement about abortion. The participants were randomly assigned to tell a true or false statement. Study 4 focused on feelings about friends and study 5 had the students involved in a mock crime and asked to lie. Human judges were asked to rate the truthfulness of the 400 communications dealing with abortion. The judges read or watched the statement and gave it a yes or no answer about if this statement was false or not. LIWC correctly classified 67% of the abortion communications and the judges correctly classified 52%. His studies have identified that deception carries three primary written markers. The first is fewer first-person pronouns such as 'I', 'me', 'my', 'mine', and 'myself' (singular), as well as 'we', 'us', 'our', and 'ourselves' (plural). Those lying "avoid statements of ownership, distance themselves from their stories and avoid taking responsibility for their behavior" while also using more negative emotion words such as "hate, worthless and sad." Second, they use "few exclusionary words such as except, but or nor" when "distinguish what they did from what they did not do."

More recently evidence has been provided by the work of CA Morgan III and GA Hazlett that a computer analysis of cognitive interview derived speech content (i.e. response length and unique word count) provides a method for detecting deception that is both demonstrably better than professional judgments of professionals and useful at distinguishing between genuine and false adult claims of exposure to highly stressful, potentially traumatic events. This method shows particular promise as it is non confrontational as well as scientifically and cross culturally valid.

Questioning and testing techniques

See also: Polygraph § Testing procedure

There are typically three types of questions used in polygraph testing or voice stress analysis testing:

Irrelevant questions establish a baseline to compare other answers by asking simple questions with clear true and false answers.

Comparison questions have an indirect relationship to the event or circumstance, and they are designed to encourage the subject to lie.

Relevant questions are compared against comparison questions (which should represent false answers) and irrelevant questions (which should represent true answers). They are about whatever is particularly in question.

The control question test (CQT) uses control questions, with known answers, to serve as a physiological baseline in order to compare them with questions relevant to a particular incident. The control question should have a greater physiological response if truth was told and a lesser physiological response for lying. The guilty knowledge test (GKT) is a multiple-choice format in which answer choices or one correct answer and additional incorrect answers are read and the physiological response is recorded. The controls are the incorrect alternative answers. The greater physiological response should be to the correct answer. Its point is to determine if the subject has knowledge about a particular event.

In addition to the test skewing towards not finding people innocent, there are also issues where some offenders might have a greater physiological response to the control question than to the specific question, making it difficult to determine guilt using this method even when people are not using specific techniques to try and trick the test. Although the issues with the CQT false-positive and false-negative rates are discussed above, there are also methodological issues with how proponents of the CQT determine the accuracy of the test. Due to the fact that the accuracy of the CQT is often determined through whether an individual who is given the test provides the police a confession to a crime after the test is administered, this means that cases where someone was cleared of charges after taking a polygraph or, in a worst-case scenario, gives a false confession when they are actually innocent are not taken into account when it comes to determining the accuracy of the test. Another issue is that, due to how the CQT is administered and how the lie-detection process works, only people who are determined to be deceptive are further interrogated for a confession. This means that the polygraph outcome and the confession are not independent of one another, making it very difficult to use confessions as the sole determiner of the accuracy of the test. These methodological problems provide false evidence that supports the continued use of this test, despite the many flaws that the test possesses. While it could be said that including this test as a police tool is useful because it might sometimes provide accurate information, the probability of it causing undue hardship to people who are actually innocent, and wasting time in the process, makes this a very unreliable method for law enforcement officers to use.

Both are considered to be biased against those that are innocent, because the guilty who fear the consequences of being found out can be more motivated to cheat on the test. Various techniques (which can be found online) can teach individuals how to change the results of the tests, including curling the toes, and biting the tongue. Mental arithmetic was found to be ineffective by at least one study, especially in students counting backward by seven. A study has found that in the guilty knowledge test subjects can focusing on the alternative answers and make themselves look innocent.

Polygraph

Main article: Polygraph

Lie detection commonly involves the polygraph, and is used to test both styles of deception. It detects autonomic reactions, such as micro-expressions, breathing rate, skin conductivity, and heart rate. Micro-expressions are the brief and incomplete nonverbal changes in expression while the rest show an activation of the nervous system. These changes in body functions are not easily controlled by the conscious mind. They also may consider respiration rate, blood pressure, capillary dilation, and muscular movement. While taking a polygraph test the subject wears a blood pressure device to measure blood pressure fluctuations. Respiration is measured by wearing pneumographs around the chest, and finally electrodes are placed on the subject's fingers to measure skin conductivity. To determine truth it is assumed the subject will show more signs of fear when answering the control questions, known to the examiner, compared with the relevant questions, where the answers are not known. Polygraphs focus more on the exams predictive value of guilt by comparing the responses of the participant to control questions, irrelevant questions, and relevant questions to gauge arousal, which is then interpreted as a display of fear and deception is assumed. If a person is showing a deception there will be changes in the autonomic arousal responses to the relevant questions. Results are considered inconclusive if there is no fluctuation in any of the questions.

These measures are supposed to indicate a short-term stress response which can be from lying or significance to the subject. The problem becomes that they are also associated with mental effort and emotional state, so they can be influenced by fear, anger, and surprise for example. This technique may also be used with CQT and GKT.

United States government agencies, such as the Department of Defense, Homeland Security, Customs and Border Protection, and even the Department of Energy currently use polygraphs. They are regularly used by these agencies to screen employees.

Critics claim that "lie detection" by use of polygraphy has no scientific validity because it is not a scientific procedure. People have found ways to try and cheat the system, such as taking sedatives to reduce anxiety; using antiperspirant to prevent sweating; and positioning pins or biting parts of the mouth after each question to demonstrate a constant physiological response. As technology and research have developed many have moved away from polygraphing because of the drawbacks of this style of detection. Supporters of polygraphing claim it has a 70% accuracy rate, 16% better than lie detection in the general population. Someone who has failed the test is more likely to confess than someone who has passed, contributing to polygraph examiners not learning about mistakes they have made and thus improving.

Voice stress analysis

Voice stress analysis (also called voice risk analysis) uses computers to compare pitch, frequency, intensity and micro tremors. In this way voice analysis "detect minute variations in the voice thought to signal lying." It can even be used covertly over the phone, and has been used by banking and insurance companies as well as the government of the United Kingdom. Customers are assessed for truth in certain situations by banks and insurance companies where computers are used to record responses. Software then compares control questions to relevant questions assessed for deception. However, its reliability has been debated by peer-reviewed journals. "When a person lies, an involuntary interference of the nerves causes the vocal cords to produce a distorted sound wave, namely a frequency level which is different from the one produced by the same person when telling the truth."

Several studies published in peer reviewed journals showed VSA to perform at chance level when it comes to detecting deception. Horvath, McCloughan, Weatherman, and Slowik, (2013), for example, tested VSA on the recordings of interrogation of 74 suspects. Eighteen of these suspects later confessed, making the deception the most likely ground truth. With 48% accurate classification, VSA performed at chance level. Several other studies showed similar results (Damphousse, 2008; Harnsberger, Hollien, Martin, & Hollien, 2009). In 2003, the National Research Council concluded "Overall, this research and the few controlled tests conducted over the past decade offer little or no scientific basis for the use of the computer voice stress analyser or similar voice measurement instruments."

Non-verbal behavior

People often evaluate lies based on non-verbal behavior, but are quick to place too much merit in misleading indicators, such as: avoidance of eye contact, increased pauses between statements, and excessive movements originating from the hands or feet. Devices such as the Silent Talker Lie Detector monitor large numbers of microexpressions over time slots and encodes them into large vectors which are classified as showing truthful or deceptive behavior by artificial intelligence or statistical classifiers.

Dr. Alan Hirsch, from the department of Neurology and Psychiatry at the Rush Presbyterian-St. Luke's Medical Center in Chicago, explained the "Pinocchio syndrome" or "Pinocchio effect" as: blood rushes to the nose when people lie. This extra blood may make the nose itchy. As a result, people who stretch the truth tend to either scratch their nose or touch it more often.

Eye-tracking

John Kircher, Doug Hacker, Anne Cook, Dan Woltz and David Raskin have developed eye-tracking technology at the University of Utah that they consider a polygraph alternative. This is not an emotional reaction like the polygraph and other methods but rather a cognitive reaction. This technology measures pupil dilation, response time, reading and rereading time, and errors. Data is recorded while subjects answer true or false questions on a computer.

They have found that more effort is required by lying than giving the truth and thus their aim is to find indications of hard work. Individuals not telling the truth might, for instance, have dilated pupils while also taking longer to answer the question.

Eye-tracking claims to offer several benefits over the polygraph: lower cost, 1/5th of the time to conduct, subjects do not need to be "hooked up" to anything, and it does not require qualified polygraph examiners to give the test. The technology has not been subject to peer review.

Brain observations

Cognitive chronometry, or the measurement of the time taken to perform mental operations, can be used to distinguish lying from truth-telling. One recent instrument using cognitive chronometry for this purpose is the timed antagonistic response alethiometer, or TARA.

Brain-reading uses fMRI and the multiple voxels activated in the brain evoked by a stimulus to determine what the brain has detected, and so whether it is familiar.

Functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) also detects oxygen and activity in the brain like the fMRI, but instead it looks at blood oxygen levels. It is advantageous to the fMRI because it is portable, however its image resolution is of lower quality than the fMRI.

As there are different styles of lying, a spontaneous or artificial deception is constructed based on a mixture of information already stored in semantic and episodic memory. It is isolated and easier to generate because it lacks cross-checking into the larger picture. This style contrasts memorized lies that aren't as rich in detail but are retrieved from memory. They often fit into an actual scenario to make recall easier.

Functional transcranial Doppler (fTCD)

Recent developments that permit non-invasive monitoring using functional transcranial Doppler (fTCD) technique showed that successful problem-solving employs a discrete knowledge strategy (DKS) that selects neural pathways represented in one hemisphere, while unsuccessful outcome implicates a non-discrete knowledge strategy (nDKS). A polygraphic test could be viewed as a working memory task. This suggests that the DKS model may have a correlate in mnemonic operations. In other words, the DKS model may have a discrete knowledge base (DKB) of essential components needed for task resolution, while for nDKS, DKB is absent and, hence, a "global" or bi-hemispheric search occurs. Based on the latter premise, a 'lie detector' system was designed as described in United States Patent No. 6,390,979. A pattern of blood-flow-velocity changes is obtained in response to questions that include correct and incorrect answers. The wrong answer will elicit bi-hemispheric activation, from correct answer that activates unilateral response. Cognitive polygraphy based on this system is devoid of any subjective control of mental processes and, hence, high reliability and specificity; however, this is yet to be tested in forensic practice. See also cognitive biometrics.

Event-related potentials (ERP)

Main article: Event-related potentials

Event-related potentials assess recognition, and therefore may or may not be effective in assessing deception. In ERP studies P3 amplitude waves are assessed, with these waves being large when an item is recognized. However, P100 amplitudes have been observed to have significant correlation to trustworthiness ratings, the importance of which will be discussed in the EEG section. This, along with other studies leads some to purport that because ERP studies rely on quick perceptual processes they "are integral to the detection of deception."

Electroencephalography (EEG)

Main article: Electroencephalography

Electroencephalography, or EEG, measures brain activity through electrodes attached to the scalp of a subject. The object is to identify the recognition of meaningful data through this activity. Images or objects are shown to the subject while questioning techniques are implemented to determine recognition. This can include crime scene images, for example.

Perceived trustworthiness is interpreted by the individual from looking at a face, and this decreases when someone is lying. Such observations are "too subtle to be explicitly processed by observers, but affect implicit cognitive and affective processes." These results, in a study by Heussen, Binkofski, and Jolij, were obtained through a study with an N400 paradigm including two conditions within the experiment: truthful faces and lying faces. Faces flashed for 100ms and then the participants rated them. However, the limitations of this study would be that it only had 15 participants and the mean age was 24.

Machine learning algorithms applied to EEG data have also been used to decode whether a subject believed or disbelieved a statement reaching ~90% accuracy. This work was an extension to work by Sam Harris and colleagues and further demonstrated that belief preceded disbelief in time, suggesting that the brain may initially accept statements as valid descriptions of the world (belief) prior to rejecting this notion (disbelief). Understanding how the brain assesses the veracity of a descriptive statement may be an important step in building neuroimaging based lie detection methods.

Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI)

Main article: fMRI lie detection

Functional magnetic resonance imaging looks to the central nervous system to compare time and topography of activity in the brain for lie detection. While a polygraph detects changes in activity in the peripheral nervous system, fMRI has the potential to catch the lie at the 'source'.

fMRIs use electromagnets to create pulse sequences in the cells of the brain. The fMRI scanner then detects the different pulses and fields that are used to distinguish tissue structures and the distinction between layers of the brain, matter type, and the ability to see growths. The functional component allows researchers to see activation in the brain over time and assess efficiency and connectivity by comparing blood use in the brain, which allows for the identification of which portions of the brain are using more oxygen, and thus being used during a specific task. FMRI data have been examined through the lens of machine learning algorithms to decode whether subjects believed or disbelieved statements, ranging from mathematical, semantic to religious belief statements.

Historically, fMRI lie detector tests have not been allowed into evidence in legal proceedings, the most famous attempt being Harvey Nathan's insurance fraud case in 2007. The lack of legal support has not stopped companies like No Lie MRI and CEPHOS from offering private fMRI scans to test deception. While fMRI studies on deception have claimed detection accuracy as high as 90% many have problems with implementing this style of detection. Only yes or no answers can be used which allows for flexibility in the truth and style of lying. Some people are unable to take one such as those with medical conditions, claustrophobia, or implants.

Drugs

Main article: Truth serum

Truth drugs such as sodium thiopental, ethanol, and cannabis (historically speaking) are used for the purposes of obtaining accurate information from an unwilling subject. Information obtained by publicly disclosed truth drugs has been shown to be highly unreliable, with subjects apparently freely mixing fact and fantasy. Much of the claimed effect relies on the belief of the subjects that they cannot tell a lie while under the influence of the drug.

See also

References

  1. Granhag, Pär Anders; Vrij, Aldert; Verschuere, Bruno (2015). Detecting Deception: Current Challenges and Cognitive Approaches (Wiley Series in Psychology of Crime, Policing and Law). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley-Blackwell. p. 205. ISBN 978-1118509661.
  2. ^ "Detecting Deception". Parliamentary Office of Science and Technology (UK). Retrieved 26 April 2012.
  3. ^ The Polygraph and Lie Detection. National Research Council. 2003. doi:10.17226/10420. ISBN 978-0-309-26392-4.
  4. Vrij, Aldert; Hartwig, Maria; Granhag, Pär Anders (2019). "Reading Lies: Nonverbal Communication and Deception". Annual Review of Psychology. 70 (1): 295–317. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-010418-103135. ISSN 0066-4308. PMID 30609913. S2CID 58562467.
  5. Seigel, Jessica (25 March 2021). "The truth about lying". Knowable Magazine. doi:10.1146/knowable-032421-1. Retrieved 8 December 2021.
  6. ^ "Scientific Validity of Polygraph Testing: A Research Review and Evaluation – A Technical Memorandum" (PDF). Washington, DC: U.S. Congress, Office of Technology Assessment (OTA-TM-H-15). November 1983. Retrieved 3 September 2016.
  7. The Truth About Lie Detectors. American Psychological Association.
  8. "Telling the truth about lie detectors". usatoday.com.
  9. Dunning, Brian (8 July 2014). "Skeptoid #422: Lie Detection". Skeptoid. Retrieved 22 June 2017.
  10. Eriksson, A.; Lacerda, F. (2007). "Charlantry in forensic speech science: A problem to be taken seriously". International Journal of Speech, Language and the Law. 14 (2). doi:10.1558/ijsll.2007.14.2.169. Archived from the original on 2010-01-13. Retrieved 2009-01-28.
  11. All lies? Scientists threatened with legal action over lie detector article Archived 2009-04-13 at the Wayback Machine. Stockholm University.
  12. Threats over Swedish lie detector research Archived 2010-12-07 at the Wayback Machine. The Local. January 27, 2009.
  13. Online at: Anders Eriksson; Francisco Lacerda (14 February 2007). "Charlatanry in forensic speech science: A problem to be taken seriously" (PDF). International Journal of Speech, Language and the Law. 14.2 2007: 169. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2 August 2016. Retrieved 3 September 2016.
  14. ^ Preston, Elizabeth (July 2002). "Detecting Deception". Observer. 15 (6). Retrieved 26 April 2012.
  15. Keeler, Leonarde (January 1930). "A Method for Detecting Deception". American Journal of Police Science. 1 (1): 38–51. doi:10.2307/1147254. JSTOR 1147254.
  16. "Historical Perspective: Some Polygraph Techniques". Western Morning News. 3 October 2018. Retrieved 26 August 2020.
  17. ^ Adelson, Rachel (July 2004). "Detecting Deception". Monitor on Psychology. 37 (7): 70. Retrieved 26 April 2012.
  18. Bond CF, Jr; DePaulo, BM (2006). "Accuracy of deception judgments". Personality and Social Psychology Review. 10 (3): 214–234. doi:10.1207/s15327957pspr1003_2. PMID 16859438. S2CID 15248114.
  19. DePaulo, Bella M.; Morris, Wendy L. (2004). "Chapter 2: Discerning lies from truth: behavioural cues to deception and the indirect pathway of intuition". In Granhag, Pär Anders; Strömwall, Leif A. (eds.). The detection of deception in forensic contexts (Transferred to digital print. ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0521541572.
  20. Morgan CA 3rd; et al. (2011). "Efficacy of forensic statement analysis in distinguishing truthful from deceptive eyewitness accounts of highly stressful events". Journal of Forensic Sciences. 56 (5): 1227–1234. doi:10.1111/j.1556-4029.2011.01896.x. PMID 21854383. S2CID 25246267.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: numeric names: authors list (link)
  21. ^ Iacono, William G. (October 2008). "Effective Policing: Understanding How Polygraph Tests Work and Are Used". Criminal Justice and Behavior. 35 (10): 1295–1308. doi:10.1177/0093854808321529. ISSN 0093-8548. S2CID 143302792.
  22. ^ Ganis, G.; Kosslyn, S. M.; Stose, S.; Thompson, W. L.; Yurgelun-Todd, D. A. (2003). "Neural Correlates of Different Types of Deception: An fMRI Investigation". Cerebral Cortex. 13 (8): 830–836. doi:10.1093/cercor/13.8.830. PMID 12853369.
  23. Forensic Psychology. BBC Science & Nature.
  24. "The Truth About Lie Detectors (aka Polygraph Tests)". apa.org. Retrieved 4 September 2016.
  25. ^ "Education psychologists use eye0tracking method for detecting lies". psychologialscience.org. 2010-07-12. Retrieved 26 April 2012.
  26. The Lie Behind the Lie Detector by George W. Maschke and Gino J. Scalabrini
  27. "Polygraph Testing". epic.org. Electronic Privacy Information Center. Retrieved 23 September 2016.
  28. ^ Rusconi, Elena; Mitchener-Nissen, Timothy (2013). "Prospects of functional magnetic resonance imaging as lie detector". Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 7: 594. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00594. PMC 3781577. PMID 24065912.
  29. S. Manes. "Lie Detector. (Lie Detector Software Truster)." Forbes, Oct. 5, 1998
  30. Horvath, F., McCloughan, J., Weatherman, D., & Slowik, S. (2013). "The Accuracy of auditors' and layered voice Analysis (LVA) Operators' Judgments of Truth and Deception During Police Questioning". Journal of Forensic Sciences, 58, 385–392.
  31. Damphousse (2008). "Voice stress analysis: Only 15 percent of lies about drug use detected in field test". NIJ Journal. 259: 8–12.
  32. Harnsberger, J. D.; Hollien, H.; Martin, C. A.; Hollien, K. A. (2009). "Stress and Deception in Speech: Evaluating Layered Voice Analysis". Journal of Forensic Sciences. 54 (3): 642–650. doi:10.1111/j.1556-4029.2009.01026.x. PMID 19432740. S2CID 205767994.
  33. Hollien, H.; Harnsberger, J. D.; Martin, C. A.; Hollien, K. A. (2008). "Evaluation of the NITV CVSA". Journal of Forensic Sciences. 53 (1): 183–193. doi:10.1111/j.1556-4029.2007.00596.x. PMID 18279255. S2CID 205767046.
  34. Akehurst; Koehnken (1996). "Lay persons' and police officers' beliefs regarding deceptive behavior". Applied Cognitive Psychology. 10 (6): 461–471. doi:10.1002/(sici)1099-0720(199612)10:6<461::aid-acp413>3.3.co;2-u.
  35. Rothwell, J.; Bandar, Z.; O'Shea, J.; McLean, D. (2006). "Silent talker: a new computer-based system for the analysis of facial cues to deception". Journal of Applied Cognitive Psychology. 20 (6): 757–777. doi:10.1002/acp.1204.
  36. Rothwell, J. McLean; Bandar, J.; O'Shea, Z. (2006). "Charting the behavioural state of a person using a Backpropagation Neural Network". Journal of Neural Computing and Applications. 16 (4–5): 327–339. doi:10.1007/s00521-006-0055-9. S2CID 8968052.
  37. Dr. Game W. Gifford Jones (26 July 2010). "Can 'Pinocchio Syndrome' solve the niqab dilemma?". The Telegram. Retrieved 12 August 2021 – via PressReader.
  38. Njemanze, P. C. (2005). "Cerebral lateralization and general intelligence: Gender differences in a transcranial Doppler study". Brain and Language. 92 (3): 234–239. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.532.5734. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2004.06.104. PMID 15721956. S2CID 12440401.
  39. ^ Heussen, Yana; Ferdinand Binkofski; Jacob Jolij (September 2010). "The semantics of the lying face – An EEG Study". International Journal of Psychophysiology. 77 (3): 206. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2010.06.006.
  40. Douglas, Pamela K; Edward Lau; Ariana Anderson; Wesley Kerr; Austin Head; Margalit Wollner; Daniel Moyer; Michael Durnhofer; Wei Li; Jen Bramen; Mark S Cohen (2013). "Single trial decoding of belief decision making from EEG and fMRI data using independent components features". Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 7: 392. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00392. PMC 3728485. PMID 23914164.
  41. Simpson JR (2008). "Functional MRI lie detection: too good to be true?". J. Am. Acad. Psychiatry Law. 36 (4): 491–498. PMID 19092066.
  42. Douglas, PK (2011). "Performance comparison of machine learning algorithms and number of independent components used in fMRI decoding of belief vs. disbelief". NeuroImage. 56 (2): 544–553. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.11.002. PMC 3099263. PMID 21073969.
  43. Langleben, D. D.; Dattilio, F. M. (2008). "Commentary: The future of forensic functional brain imaging". The Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law. 36 (4): 502–4. PMID 19092068.
  44. Kapoor, P.; Chugh, Y.; Kapoor, A. K.; Sinha, U. S. (2008). "Torture, terrorist and truth serum". International Journal of Medical Toxicology & Legal Medicine. 10 (2).
  45. Rowell, Jr (1952). "Admissibility of Evidence Obtained by Scientific Devices and Analyses". Florida Law Review. 5 (5).
Nonverbal communication
Modalities
Physical
Speech
Social context
Other
Unconscious
Multi-faceted
Broader concepts
Further information
Disorders
Neuroanatomy
Applications
Technology
Key people
Related
Non-verbal language
Art and literature
Category: