Revision as of 21:55, 25 June 2016 view sourceRA0808 (talk | contribs)Extended confirmed users, New page reviewers, Pending changes reviewers, Rollbackers100,313 editsm Reverted edits by 45.50.116.179 (talk) (HG) (3.1.20)← Previous edit | Latest revision as of 21:31, 9 January 2025 view source Platosghostlybeard (talk | contribs)392 editsm →Religious and other cultural contexts: I added a brief point with appropriate reference to the scholarly literature and a link to another Misplaced Pages article.Tag: Visual edit | ||
(540 intermediate revisions by more than 100 users not shown) | |||
Line 1: | Line 1: | ||
{{short description|Event occurring in the mind while sleeping}} | |||
{{Other uses}} | |||
{{Other uses|Dream (disambiguation)|Dreams (disambiguation)}} | |||
]]] | |||
{{pp-vandalism|small=yes}} | |||
] dreaming of a confrontation with ], shown inside a ]]] | |||
''' |
A '''dream''' is a succession of ]s, ]s, ], and ] that usually occur involuntarily in the ] during certain stages of ].<ref>{{cite web |url=http://www.thefreedictionary.com/dream |title=Dream |publisher=The American Heritage Dictionary of the English Language, Fourth Edition. 2000 |access-date=7 May 2009}}</ref> Humans spend about two hours dreaming per night,<ref>{{cite web |year=2006 |title=Brain Basics: Understanding Sleep |url=http://www.ninds.nih.gov/disorders/brain_basics/understanding_sleep.htm |access-date=16 December 2007 |publisher=] |url-status=live |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20071011011207/http://www.ninds.nih.gov/disorders/brain_basics/understanding_sleep.htm |archive-date=11 October 2007}}</ref> and each dream lasts around 5–20 minutes, although the dreamer may perceive the dream as being much longer than this.<ref name="HSWDream">{{cite book |year=2006 |title=How Dream Works |author=Lee Ann Obringer |url=http://science.howstuffworks.com/dream3.htm |access-date=4 May 2006 |url-status=live |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20060418032147/http://science.howstuffworks.com/dream3.htm |archive-date=18 April 2006}}</ref> | ||
The content and function of dreams have been topics of scientific, philosophical and religious interest throughout ]. ], practiced by the ]ns in the third millennium BCE<ref>{{cite book |last1=Krippner |first1=Stanley |last2=Bogzaran |first2=Fariba |last3=Carvalho |first3=Andre Percia de |date=2002 |title=Extraordinary Dreams and How To Work with Them |location=Albany, NY |publisher=State University of New York Press |quote=Clay tablets have been found, dating to about 2500 B.C.E., that contain interpretive material for Babylonian and ]n dreamers. |page=9 |isbn=0-7914-5257-3}}</ref> and even earlier by the ancient ]ians,<ref>{{cite book |last=Seligman |first=K |date=1948 |title=Magic, Supernaturalism and Religion |location=New York |publisher=Random House}}</ref><ref name="BlackGreen1992">{{cite book |last1=Black |first1=Jeremy |first2=Anthony |last2=Green |title=Gods, Demons and Symbols of Ancient Mesopotamia: An Illustrated Dictionary |location=Austin |publisher=University of Texas Press |year=1992 |isbn=0714117056 |pages=71–72, 89–90}}</ref> figures prominently in religious texts in several traditions, and has played a lead role in psychotherapy.<ref name="Freud1">{{cite book |last=Freud |first=Sigmund |translator=James Strachey |editor=James Strachey |author-link=Sigmund Freud |date=1965 |title=The Interpretation of Dreams |location=New York |publisher=Avon}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Schredl |first1=Michael |last2=Bohusch |first2=Claudia |last3=Kahl |first3=Johanna |last4=Mader |first4=Andrea |last5=Somesan |first5=Alexandra |title=The Use of Dreams in Psychotherapy |journal=The Journal of Psychotherapy Practice and Research |year=2000 |volume=9 |issue=2 |pages=81–87}}</ref> The scientific study of dreams is called ].<ref>{{cite journal |last=Kavanau |first=J.L. |title=Sleep, memory maintenance, and mental disorders |journal=Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences |year=2000 |volume=12 |issue=2 |pages=199–208 |doi=10.1176/jnp.12.2.199 |pmid=11001598 |issn = 0895-0172 }}</ref> Most modern dream study focuses on the neurophysiology of dreams and on proposing and testing hypotheses regarding dream function. It is not known where in the brain dreams originate, if there is a single origin for dreams or if multiple regions of the brain are involved, or what the purpose of dreaming is for the body or mind. | |||
Dreams mainly occur in the ]—when ] is high and resembles that of being awake. REM sleep is revealed by continuous movements of the eyes during sleep. At times, dreams may occur during other ]. However, these dreams tend to be much less ] or memorable.<ref name=hobson>Hobson, J.A. (2009) ''REM sleep and dreaming: towards a theory of protoconsciousness'', Nature Reviews, 10(11)</ref> | |||
The human dream experience and what to make of it has undergone sizable shifts over the course of history.<ref name="Dodds1">{{cite book |last= Dodds |first=E. R. |author-link=E. R. Dodds |date=1951 |title=The Greeks and the Irrational |location=Berkeley |publisher=University of California Press |quote=The Greeks never spoke as we do of ''having'' a dream, but always of ''seeing'' a dream.... |page= 105}}</ref><ref name="Packer1">{{cite book |last=Packer |first=Sharon |date=2002 |title=Dreams in Myth, Medicine, and Movies |location=Westport, CT |publisher=Praeger Publishers |quote=…any more ancient cultures think that dreams are imposed by a force that resides outside the individual. |page=85 |isbn=0-275-97243-7}}</ref> Long ago, according to writings from ] and ], dreams dictated post-dream behaviors to an extent that was sharply reduced in later millennia.{{clarify|date=August 2023}} These ancient writings about dreams highlight visitation dreams, where a dream figure, usually a deity or a prominent forebear, commands the dreamer to take specific actions, and which may predict future events.<ref>{{cite book |last=Macrobius |author-link=Macrobius |translator=] |date=1952 |orig-date=430 |title=Commentary on the Dream of Scipio |location=New York |publisher=Columbia University Press |quote=We call a dream oracular in which a parent, or a pious or revered man, or a priest, or even a god clearly reveals what will or will not transpire, and what action to take or to avoid. |page=90}}</ref><ref>Dodds (1951), referring to the type of dream described by Macrobius: "This last type is not, I think, at all common in our own dream-experience. But there is considerable evidence that dreams of this sort were familiar in antiquity." (p. 107).</ref><ref name=Krippner1>{{cite book |last1=Krippner |first1=Stanley |last2=Bogzaran |first2=Fariba |last3=Carvalho |first3=André Percia de |date=2002 |title=Extraordinary Dreams and How To Work with Them |location=Albany |publisher=State University of New York Press |quote=The Egyptian papyrus of Deral-Madineh was written about 1300 B.C.E. and gives instructions on how to obtain a dream message from a god. |page=10 |isbn=0-7914-5257-3}}</ref> Framing the dream experience varies across cultures as well as through time. | |||
The length of a dream can vary; they may last for a few seconds, or approximately 20–30 minutes.<ref name=hobson/> People are more likely to remember the dream if they are awakened during the REM phase. The average person has three to five dreams per night, and some may have up to seven;<ref>Empson, J. (2002). ''Sleep and dreaming'' (3rd ed.)., New York: Palgrave/St. Martin's Press</ref> however, most dreams are immediately or quickly forgotten.<ref>Cherry, Kendra. (2015). "." ''About Education: Psychology''. About.com.</ref> Dreams tend to last longer as the night progresses. During a full eight-hour night sleep, most dreams occur in the typical two hours of REM.<ref>{{cite web|last=Ann |first=Lee |url=http://science.howstuffworks.com/environmental/life/human-biology/dream2.htm |title=HowStuffWorks "Dreams: Stages of Sleep" |publisher=Science.howstuffworks.com |date=January 27, 2005 |accessdate=August 11, 2012}}</ref> | |||
Dreaming and sleep are intertwined. Dreams occur mainly in the ]—when ] is high and resembles that of being awake. Because REM sleep is detectable in many species, and because research suggests that all mammals experience REM,<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Lesku |first1=J. A. |last2=Meyer |first2=L. C. R. |last3=Fuller |first3=A. |last4=Maloney |first4=S. K. |last5=Dell'Omo |first5=G. |last6=Vyssotski |first6=A. L. |last7=Rattenborg |first7=N. C. |year=2011 |title=Ostriches sleep like platypuses |journal=PLOS ONE |volume=6 |issue=8 |pages=1–7 |doi=10.1371/journal.pone.0023203 |pmid=21887239 |pmc=3160860 |bibcode= 2011PLoSO...623203L |doi-access=free}}</ref> linking dreams to REM sleep has led to conjectures that animals dream. However, humans dream during non-REM sleep, also, and not all REM awakenings elicit dream reports.<ref name=Solms1>{{cite journal |last=Solms |first=Mark |author-link=Mark Solms |title=Dreaming and REM sleep are controlled by different brain mechanisms |journal=Behavioral and Brain Sciences |year=2000 |volume=23 |issue=6 |pages=843–850 |doi=10.1017/S0140525X00003988 |pmid=11515144 |s2cid=7264870 |quote=Dreaming and REM sleep are incompletely correlated. Between 5 and 30% of REM awakenings do not elicit dream reports; and at least 5–10% of NREM awakenings do elicit dream reports that are indistinguishable from REM....}}</ref> To be studied, a dream must first be reduced to a verbal report, which is an account of the subject's memory of the dream, not the subject's dream experience itself. So, dreaming by non-humans is currently unprovable, as is dreaming by human fetuses and pre-verbal infants.<ref>{{cite book |last=Bulkeley |first=Kelly |title=Dreaming in the world's religions: A comparative history |year=2008 |isbn=978-0-8147-9956-7 |page=14 |publisher=NYU Press |quote=Do animals dream? We currently have no means of proving it one way or the other, just as we have no way to determine whether human fetuses and newborns are genuinely dreaming before they develop the ability to speak and relate their experiences.}}</ref> | |||
In modern times, dreams have been seen as a connection to the ]. They range from normal and ordinary to overly ] and bizarre. Dreams can have varying natures, such as being ], exciting, ], ], adventurous, or ]. The events in dreams are generally outside the control of the dreamer, with the exception of ]ing, where the dreamer is ].<ref>{{cite web|last1=Lite|first1=Jordan|title=How Can You Control Your Dreams?|url=http://www.scientificamerican.com/article/how-to-control-dreams/|website=Scientific America|date=July 29, 2010}}</ref> Dreams can at times make a ] thought occur to the person or give a sense of ].<ref>Domhoff, W. (2002). The scientific study of dreams. APA Press</ref> | |||
== Subjective experience and content == | |||
Opinions about the meaning of dreams have varied and shifted through time and culture. Most people today appear to endorse the (]) theory of dreams - that dreams reveal insight into hidden desires and emotions. Other prominent theories include those suggesting that dreams assist in memory formation, problem solving, or simply are a product of random brain activation.<ref name=":0">{{Cite journal|title = When dreaming is believing: The (motivated) interpretation of dreams.|url = http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/a0013264|journal = Journal of Personality and Social Psychology|pages = 249–264|volume = 96|issue = 2|doi = 10.1037/a0013264|first = Carey K.|last = Morewedge|first2 = Michael I.|last2 = Norton}}</ref> The earliest recorded dreams were acquired from materials dating back approximately 5000 years, in ], where they were documented on ]s. In the ] and ] periods, the people believed that dreams were direct messages from one and/or multiple ], from deceased persons, and that they ]. Some cultures practiced ] with the intention of cultivating dreams that are of ].<ref>{{cite book |title=The Discarded Image |author=C.S. Lewis |publisher= Canto, ] |isbn=978-0-521-47735-2 |authorlink= C.S. Lewis}}</ref> | |||
{{Further|Oneiromancy}} | |||
] (1848–1906)]] | |||
Preserved writings from early Mediterranean civilizations indicate a relatively abrupt change in subjective dream experience between ] antiquity and the beginnings of the ].<ref>{{cite book |last=Damasio |first=Antonio |author-link= Antonio Damasio |date=2010 |title=Self Comes to Mind |location=New York |publisher=Pantheon Books |quote=…I sympathize with Julian Jaynes's claim that something of great import may have happened to the human mind during the relatively brief interval of time between the events narrated in the ''Iliad'' and those that make up the ''Odyssey''. |page=289 |isbn=978-0-307-37875-0}}</ref> | |||
], who developed the discipline of ], wrote extensively about dream theories and their interpretations in the early 1900s.<ref>Freud, S. (1900). The interpretation of dreams. London: Hogarth Press</ref> He explained dreams as manifestations of our deepest desires and anxieties, often relating to ] ] or obsessions. Furthermore, he believed that virtually every dream topic, regardless of its content, represented the release of sexual tension.<ref>{{Cite book|title = The Dream Dictionary from A to Z|last = Cheung|first = Theresa|publisher = HarperElement|year = 2006|location = China|pages = 437}}</ref> In '']'' (1899), Freud developed a psychological technique to interpret dreams and devised a series of guidelines to understand the symbols and motifs that appear in our dreams. | |||
In visitation dreams reported in ancient writings, dreamers were largely passive in their dreams, and visual content served primarily to frame authoritative auditory messaging.<ref>{{Citation |last=Nielsen |first=Tore A. |contribution=Reality Dreams and Their Effects on Spiritual Belief: A Revision of Animism Theory |editor-last1=Gackenbach |editor-first1=Jayne |editor-last2=Sheikh |editor-first2=Anees A. |title=Dream Images: A Call to Mental Arms |year=1991 |pages=233–264 |place=Amityville, NY |publisher=Baywood |isbn=0-89503-056-X}}</ref><ref name="Dodds1"/><ref>{{cite journal|last=Atwan |first=Robert |title=The Interpretation of Dreams, The Origin of Consciousness, and the Birth of Tragedy |journal=Research Communication in Psychology, Psychiatry and Behavior |year=1981 |volume=6 |issue=2 |pages=163–182}}</ref> ], the king of the Sumerian city-state of ] (reigned {{circa}} 2144–2124 BCE), rebuilt the temple of ] as the result of a dream in which he was told to do so.<ref name="BlackGreen1992"/> After antiquity, the passive hearing of visitation dreams largely gave way to visualized narratives in which the dreamer becomes a character who actively participates. | |||
==Cultural meaning== | |||
{{Main|Dream interpretation}} | |||
From the 1940s to 1985, ] collected more than 50,000 dream reports at ]. In 1966, Hall and Robert Van de Castle published ''The Content Analysis of Dreams'', in which they outlined a coding system to study 1,000 dream reports from college students.<ref name="hallcontent">Hall, C., & Van de Castle, R. (1966). The Content Analysis of Dreams. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. {{webarchive |url=https://web.archive.org/web/20070412100915/http://psych.ucsc.edu/dreams/Info/content_analysis.html |date=12 April 2007}}</ref> Results indicated that participants from varying parts of the world demonstrated similarity in their dream content. The only residue of antiquity's authoritative dream figure in the Hall and Van de Castle listing of dream characters is the inclusion of God in the category of prominent persons.<ref>{{cite web |url=https://dreams.ucsc.edu/Coding/characters.html |title=The Classification and Coding of Characters |last1=Schneider |first1=Adam |last2=Domhoff |first2=G. William |publisher=University of California at Santa Cruz |access-date=17 July 2021}}</ref> Hall's complete dream reports were made publicly available in the mid-1990s by his protégé ]. More recent studies of dream reports, while providing more detail, continue to cite the Hall study favorably.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Schredl |first1=Michael |last2=Ciric |first2=Petra |last3=Götz |first3=Simon |last4=Wittmann |first4=Lutz |date=November 2004 |title=Typical Dreams: Stability and Gender Differences |journal=The Journal of Psychology |volume=138 |issue=6 |doi=10.3200/JRLP.138.6.485-494 |pmid=15612605 |pages=485–494 |s2cid=13554573}}</ref> | |||
===Ancient history=== | |||
] is a common term within the ] creation narrative of ] for a personal, or group, ] and for what may be understood as the "timeless time" of formative creation and perpetual creating.<ref> environment.gov.au, June 23, 2006</ref> | |||
] (1858–1925).]] | |||
The ] in Mesopotamia left evidence of dreams dating back to 3100 BC. According to these early recorded stories, gods and kings, like the 7th century BC scholar-king ], paid close attention to dreams. In his archive of clay tablets, some accounts of the story of the legendary king ] were found.<ref>Seligman, K. (1948), ''Magic, Supernaturalism and Religion''. New York: Random House</ref> | |||
In the Hall study, the most common emotion experienced in dreams was ]. Other emotions included ], ], ], ], and ]. ]s were much more common than positive ones.<ref name="hallcontent"/> The Hall data analysis showed that sexual dreams occur no more than 10% of the time and are more prevalent in young to mid-teens.<ref name="hallcontent"/> Another study showed that 8% of both men's and women's dreams have sexual content.<ref>Zadra, A., {{webarchive |url=https://web.archive.org/web/20070927222607/http://www.journalsleep.org/PDF/AbstractBook2007.pdf |date=27 September 2007}}, ''Sleep'' Volume 30, Abstract Supplement, 2007 A376.</ref> In some cases, sexual dreams may result in ]s or ]s. These are colloquially known as "wet dreams".<ref>{{cite web |url=http://www.measuredhs.com/pubs/pdf/FR157/04Chapter04.pdf |title=Badan Pusat Statistik "Indonesia Young Adult Reproductive Health Survey 2002–2004" p. 27 |access-date=4 April 2013 |url-status=live |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20121209171035/http://measuredhs.com/pubs/pdf/FR157/04Chapter04.pdf |archive-date=9 December 2012}}</ref> | |||
The Mesopotamians believed that the soul, or some part of it, moves out from the body of the sleeping person and actually visits the places and persons the dreamer sees in their sleep. Sometimes the god of dreams is said to carry the dreamer.<ref>Caillois,R. (1966). ''Logical and Philosophical Problems of the Dream''. In G.E. Von Grunebaum & R. Caillos (Eds.), ''The Dream and Human Societies''(pp. 23-52). London, England: Cambridge University Press.</ref> Babylonians and Assyrians divided dreams into "good," which were sent by the gods, and "bad," sent by demons - they also believed that their dreams were ] and prophecies.<ref>. Oppenheim, L.A. (1966). ''Mantic Dreams in the Ancient Near East'' in G. E. Von Grunebaum & R. Caillois (Eds.), ''The Dream and Human Societies'' (pp. 341-350). London, England: Cambridge University Press.</ref> | |||
The visual nature of dreams is generally highly phantasmagoric; that is, different locations and objects continuously blend into each other. The visuals (including locations, people, and objects) are generally reflective of a person's memories and experiences, but conversation can take on highly exaggerated and bizarre forms. Some dreams may even tell elaborate stories wherein the dreamer enters entirely new, complex worlds and awakes with ideas, thoughts and feelings never experienced prior to the dream. | |||
In ], as far back as 2000 BC, the Egyptians wrote down their dreams on ]. People with vivid and significant dreams were thought blessed and were considered special.<ref>Lincoln, J.S. (1935). ''The dream in primitive cultures'' London: Cressett.</ref> Ancient Egyptians believed that dreams were like ], bringing messages from the gods. They thought that the best way to receive divine revelation was through dreaming and thus they would induce (or "incubate") dreams. Egyptians would go to sanctuaries and sleep on special "dream beds" in hope of receiving advice, comfort, or healing from the gods.<ref>1991. languages of dreaming : Anthropological approaches to the study of dreaming In other cultures. In Gackenbach J, Sheikh A, eds, Dream images: A call to mental arms. Amityville, N.Y.: Baywood.</ref> | |||
People who are blind from birth do not have visual dreams. Their dream contents are related to other senses, such as ], ], ], and ], whichever are present since birth.<ref>{{cite news |title=How do blind people dream? – The Body Odd |url=http://bodyodd.nbcnews.com/_news/2012/03/09/10602730-how-do-blind-people-dream?lite |date=March 2012 |access-date=10 May 2013 |url-status=dead |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20130124114342/http://bodyodd.nbcnews.com/_news/2012/03/09/10602730-how-do-blind-people-dream?lite |archive-date=24 January 2013}}</ref> | |||
===Classical history=== | |||
] (虎跑夢泉)]] | |||
== Neurophysiology == | |||
In Chinese history, people wrote of two vital aspects of the soul of which one is freed from the body during slumber to journey in a dream realm, while the other remained in the body,<ref name=bulkeley-71/> although this belief and dream interpretation had been questioned since early times, such as by the philosopher ] (27-97).<ref name=bulkeley-71>{{cite book|last=Bulkeley|first=Kelly|title=Dreaming in the world's religions: A comparative history|year=2008|isbn=978-0-8147-9956-7|pages=71–73}}</ref> The Indian text '']'', written between 900 and 500 BC, emphasize two meanings of dreams. The first says that dreams are merely expressions of inner desires. The second is the belief of the soul leaving the body and being guided until awakened. | |||
{{Main|Cognitive neuroscience of dreams}} | |||
{{Further|Neuroscience of sleep}} | |||
Dream study is popular with scientists exploring the ]. Some "propose to reduce aspects of dream phenomenology to neurobiology."<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Hobson |first1=J. Allan |last2=Pace-Schott |first2=Edward F. |last3=Stickgold |first3=Robert |author-link1=Allan Hobson |title=Dream science 2000: A response to commentaries on ''Dreaming and the Brain'' |journal=Behavioral and Brain Sciences |year=2000 |volume=23 |issue=6 |page=1019 |doi=10.1017/S0140525X00954025 |s2cid=144729368}}</ref> But current science cannot specify dream physiology in detail. Protocols in most nations restrict human brain research to non-invasive procedures. In the United States, invasive brain procedures with a human subject are allowed only when these are deemed necessary in surgical treatment to address medical needs of the same human subject.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Chiong |first1=Winston |last2=Leonard |first2=Matthew K. |last3=Chang |first3=Edward F. |title=Neurosurgical Patients as Human Research Subjects: Ethical Considerations in Intracranial Electrophysiology Research |journal=Neurosurgery |year=2018 |volume=83 |issue=1 |pages=29–37 |doi=10.1093/neuros/nyx361 |pmid=28973530 |url=https://academic.oup.com/neurosurgery/article-abstract/83/1/29/3988112 |pmc=5777911}}</ref> Non-invasive measures of brain activity like ] (EEG) voltage averaging or ] cannot identify small but influential neuronal populations.<ref name="hob">Hobson, J. A., Pace-Schott, E. F., & Stickgold, R. (2000). "Dreaming and the brain: Toward a cognitive neuroscience of conscious states". ''Behavioral and Brain Sciences'', 23(6), 793–842.</ref> Also, ] signals are too slow to explain how brains compute in real time.<ref>{{cite web |url=https://speakingofresearch.com/2009/07/31/the-limits-of-fmri/ |title=The limits of fMRI |last=Ringach |first=Dario L. |date=30 July 2009 |publisher=Speaking of Research |access-date=18 July 2021}}</ref> | |||
The Greeks shared their beliefs with the Egyptians on how to interpret good and bad dreams, and the idea of incubating dreams. ], the Greek god of dreams, also sent warnings and prophecies to those who slept at shrines and temples. The earliest Greek beliefs about dreams were that their gods physically visited the dreamers, where they entered through a keyhole, exiting the same way after the divine message was given. | |||
Scientists researching some brain functions can work around current restrictions by examining animal subjects. As stated by the ], "Because no adequate alternatives exist, much of this research must '''' be done on animal subjects."<ref>{{cite web |url=https://www.sfn.org/Advocacy/Policy-Positions/Policies-on-the-Use-of-Animals-and-Humans-in-Research |title=Policies on the Use of Animals and Humans in Research |publisher=Society for Neuroscience |access-date=18 July 2021}}</ref> However, since animal dreaming can be only inferred, not confirmed, animal studies yield no hard facts to illuminate the neurophysiology of dreams. Examining human subjects with brain lesions can provide clues, but the lesion method cannot discriminate between the effects of destruction and disconnection and cannot target specific neuronal groups in heterogeneous regions like the brain stem.<ref name="hob"/> | |||
] wrote the first known Greek book on dreams in the 5th century BC. In that century, other cultures influenced Greeks to develop the belief that souls left the sleeping body.<ref>O'Neil, C.W. (1976). ''Dreams, culture and the individual''. San Francisco: Chandler & Sharp.</ref> ] (469-399 BC) had a simple dream theory: during the day, the soul receives images; during the night, it produces images. Greek philosopher ] (384-322 BC) believed dreams caused ] activity. He thought dreams could analyze illness and predict diseases. ], for his part, believed that all dreams are produced by thoughts and conversations a dreamer had during the preceding days.<ref>Cicero, De Republica, VI, 10</ref> Cicero's '']'' described a lengthy dream vision, which in turn was commented on by ] in his ''Commentarii in Somnium Scipionis''. | |||
== |
== Generation == | ||
], |
]'', 1655, by ]]] | ||
In Judaism, dreams are considered part of the experience of the world that can be interpreted and from which lessons can be garnered. It is discussed in the Talmud, Tractate Berachot 55-60. | |||
Denied precision tools and obliged to depend on imaging, much dream research has succumbed to the ]. Studies detect an increase of blood flow in a specific brain region and then credit that region with a role in generating dreams. But pooling study results has led to the newer conclusion that dreaming involves large numbers of regions and pathways, which likely are different for different dream events.<ref>{{cite book |last=Uttal |first=William R. |author-link=William Uttal |title=Reliability in Cognitive Neuroscience |date=2013 |location=Cambridge, MA |publisher=The MIT Press|quote=Similarly, modern neuroscience research is increasingly showing that activation areas on the brain associated with a cognitive process are far more widely distributed than had been thought only a decade or so ago. Indeed, it now seems likely that most of the brain is active in almost any cognitive process. |page=4}}</ref> | |||
The ancient ] connected their dreams heavily with their religion, though the Hebrews were ] and believed that dreams were the voice of one God alone. Hebrews also differentiated between good dreams (from God) and bad dreams (from evil spirits). The Hebrews, like many other ancient cultures, incubated dreams in order to receive divine revelation. For example, the Hebrew prophet Samuel would "lie down and sleep in the temple at Shiloh before the Ark and receive the word of the Lord." Most of the dreams in the ] are in the ].<ref>{{cite book|url={{Google books |id=zs3gup4iFu4C |page=15 |plainurl=yes }} |title=A letter that has not been read: Dreams in the Hebrew Bible |first=Shaul |last=Bar |publisher=Hebrew Union College Press |year=2001 |accessdate=April 4, 2013}}</ref> | |||
Image creation in the brain involves significant neural activity downstream from eye intake, and it is theorized that "the visual imagery of dreams is produced by activation during sleep of the same structures that generate complex visual imagery in waking perception."<ref>{{cite journal |last=Solms |first=Mark |author-link=Mark Solms |title=Dreaming and REM sleep are controlled by different brain mechanisms |journal=Behavioral and Brain Sciences |year=2000 |volume=23 |issue=6 |pages=843–850; discussion 904–1121 |doi=10.1017/S0140525X00003988 |pmid=11515144 |s2cid=7264870}}</ref> | |||
] mostly shared their beliefs with the Hebrews and thought that dreams were of a supernatural character because the ] includes frequent stories of dreams with divine inspiration. The most famous of these dream stories was ] that stretches from ] to ]. Many ] preach that God can speak to people through their dreams. | |||
Dreams present a running narrative rather than exclusively visual imagery. Following their work with ] subjects, ] and ] postulated, without attempting to specify the neural mechanisms, a "]" that seeks to create a plausible narrative from whatever electro-chemical signals reach the brain's left hemisphere. Sleep research has determined that some brain regions fully active during waking are, during REM sleep, activated only in a partial or fragmentary way.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Braun |first1=A. R. |last2=Balkin |first2=T. J |last3=Wesensten |first3=N. J. |last4=Carson |first4=R. E. |last5=Varga |first5=M. |last6=Baldwin |first6=P. |last7=Selbie |first7=S. |last8=Belenky |first8=G. |last9=Herscovitch |first9=P. |year=1997 |title=Regional cerebral blood flow through the sleep-wake cycle |journal=Brain |volume=120 |publisher=Oxford University Press |pages=1173–1197 |doi=10.1093/brain/120.7.1173 |pmid=9236630 |doi-access=free}}</ref> Drawing on this knowledge, textbook author James W. Kalat explains, " dream represents the brain's effort to make sense of sparse and distorted information.... The cortex combines this haphazard input with whatever other activity was already occurring and does its best to synthesize a story that makes sense of the information."<ref>{{cite book |last=Kalat |first=James W. |date=2015 |title=Biological Psychology |edition=12th |location=Boston |publisher=Cengage |page=288 |isbn=978-1305105409}}</ref> Neuroscientist ] is even more blunt, calling often bizarre dream content "just the result of your interpreter trying to create a story out of random neural signaling."<ref>{{cite book |last=Viskontas |first=Indre |date=2017 |title=Brain Myths Exploded: Lessons from Neuroscience |location=Chantilly, VA |publisher=The Teaching Company |page=393}}</ref> | |||
] has researched the role of dreams in Islam.<ref>{{cite book|last=Edgar|first=Iain|title=The Dream in Islam: From Qur'anic Tradition to Jihadist Inspiration|year=2011|publisher=Berghahn Books|location=Oxford|isbn=978-0-85745-235-1|page=178|url=http://www.berghahnbooks.com/title.php?rowtag=EdgarDream}}</ref> He has argued that dreams play an important role in the history of Islam and the lives of Muslims, since dream interpretation is the only way that Muslims can receive revelations from God since the death of the last prophet, ].<ref>{{cite journal|last=Edgar|first=Iain R.|author2=Henig, David|title=Istikhara: The Guidance and practice of Islamic dream incubation through ethnographic comparison|journal=History and Anthropology|date=September 2010|volume=21|issue=3|pages=251–262|doi=10.1080/02757206.2010.496781}}</ref> | |||
== Theories on function == | |||
===In Hinduism=== | |||
{{Main|Oneirology}} | |||
In the ], part of the ] scriptures of ], a dream is one of three states that the soul experiences during its lifetime, the other two states being the waking state and the sleep state.<ref>{{cite web |url=http://www.swami-krishnananda.org/mand/mand_4.html |title=The Mandukya Upanishad, Section 4 |last=Krishnananda |first=Swami |date=16 November 1996 |access-date=26 March 2015}}</ref> | |||
{{Further|Rapid eye movement sleep}} | |||
For many humans across multiple eras and cultures, dreams are believed to have functioned as revealers of truths sourced during sleep from gods or other external entities.<ref>{{Cite book |last=Ribeiro |first=Sidarta |url=https://www.worldcat.org/oclc/1200037413 |title=The oracle of night : the history and science of dreams |date=2021 |others=Daniel Hahn, Sidarta Translation of: Ribeiro |isbn=978-1-5247-4690-2 |edition= |location=New York |oclc=1200037413}}</ref> Ancient Egyptians believed that dreams were the best way to receive divine revelation, and thus they would induce (or "incubate") dreams. They went to sanctuaries and slept on special "dream beds" in hope of receiving advice, comfort, or healing from the gods.<ref name="Krippner1"/> From a Darwinian perspective dreams would have to fulfill some kind of biological requirement, provide some benefit for natural selection to take place, or at least have no negative impact on fitness. Robert (1886),<ref>Robert, W. Der Traum als Naturnothwendigkeit erklärt. Zweite Auflage, Hamburg: Seippel, 1886.</ref> a physician from Hamburg, was the first who suggested that dreams are a need and that they have the function to erase (a) sensory impressions that were not fully worked up, and (b) ideas that were not fully developed during the day. In dreams, incomplete material is either removed (suppressed) or deepened and included into memory. ], whose dream studies focused on interpreting dreams, not explaining how or why humans dream, disputed Robert's hypothesis<ref>{{cite book |last=Freud |first=Sigmund |translator=James Strachey |editor=James Strachey |author-link=Sigmund Freud |date=1965 |title=The Interpretation of Dreams |page=188 |location=New York |publisher=Avon |quote=The view adopted by Robert that the purpose of dreams is to unburden our memory of the useless impressions of daytime is plainly no longer tenable....}}</ref> and proposed that dreams preserve sleep by representing as fulfilled those wishes that otherwise would awaken the dreamer.<ref>Rycroft, Charles. ''A Critical Dictionary of Psychoanalysis''. London: Penguin Books, 1995, p. 41.</ref> Freud wrote that dreams "serve the purpose of prolonging sleep instead of waking up. ''Dreams are the'' GUARDIANS ''of sleep and not its disturbers.''"<ref>{{cite book |last=Freud |first=Sigmund |translator=James Strachey |editor=James Strachey |author-link=Sigmund Freud |date=1965 |title=The Interpretation of Dreams |page=253 |location=New York |publisher=Avon}}</ref> | |||
===Dreams and philosophical realism=== | |||
{{Main|Dream argument}} | |||
] (1830-1833)]] | |||
Some philosophers have concluded that what we think of as the "real world" could be or is an illusion (an idea known as the ] about ]). | |||
]]] | |||
The first recorded mention of the idea was by ], and it is also discussed in ], which makes extensive use of the argument in its writings.<ref>{{cite book |title=Buddhism As Presented by the Brahmanical Systems |last=Kher |first=Chitrarekha V. |year=1992 |publisher=Sri Satguru Publications |isbn=81-7030-293-5}}</ref> It was formally introduced to Western philosophy by ] in the 17th century in his '']''. Stimulus, usually an auditory one, becomes a part of a dream, eventually then awakening the dreamer. | |||
A turning point in theorizing about dream function came in 1953, when ] published the ] and ] paper<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Aserinsky |first1=Eugene |last2=Kleitman |first2=Nathaniel |author-link=Eugene Aserinsky |title=Regularly Occurring Periods of Eye Motility, and Concomitant Phenomena, during Sleep |journal=Science |year=1953 |volume=118 |issue=3062 |pages=273–274 |doi=10.1126/science.118.3062.273 |pmid=13089671 |bibcode=1953Sci...118..273A}}</ref> establishing ] as a distinct phase of sleep and linking dreams to REM sleep.<ref>{{Citation |last=Smith |first=Robert C. |contribution=The Meaning of Dreams: A Current Warning Theory |editor-last1=Gackenbach |editor-first1=Jayne |editor-last2=Sheikh |editor-first2=Anees A. |title=Dream Images: A Call to Mental Arms |year=1991 |pages=127–146 |place=Amityville, NY |publisher=Baywood |isbn=0-89503-056-X}}</ref> Until and even after publication of the Solms 2000 paper that certified the separability of REM sleep and dream phenomena,<ref name="Solms1"/> many studies purporting to uncover the function of dreams have in fact been studying not dreams but measurable REM sleep. | |||
===Postclassical and medieval history=== | |||
Some ] tribes and ] civilizations believe that dreams are a way of visiting and having contact with their ].<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Tedlock | first1 = B | year = 1981 | title = Quiche Maya dream Interpretation | url = | journal = Ethos | volume = 9 | issue = 4| pages = 313–350 | doi=10.1525/eth.1981.9.4.02a00050}}</ref> Some ] tribes used ]s as a rite of passage, fasting and praying until an anticipated guiding dream was received, to be shared with the rest of the tribe upon their return.<ref>{{cite web |url=http://www.dreams.ca/dreams.htm |last=Webb |first=Craig |year=1995 |title=Dreams: Practical Meaning & Applications |publisher=The DREAMS Foundation}}</ref><ref>{{cite web |url=http://dreamhawk.com/dream-encyclopedia/native-american-dream-beliefs/ |accessdate=April 10, 2012 |title=Native American Dream Beliefs |publisher=Dream Encyclopedia}}</ref> | |||
Theories of dream function since the identification of REM sleep include: | |||
The ] brought a harsh interpretation of dreams. They were seen as ], and the images as ]s from the ]. Many believed that during sleep, the devil could fill the human mind with corrupting and harmful thoughts. ], founder of ], believed dreams were the work of the Devil. However, ] such as ] and ] claimed that the direction of their lives was heavily influenced by their dreams. | |||
] and ] 1977 ], which proposed "a functional role for dreaming sleep in promoting some aspect of the learning process...."<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Hobson |first1=J. Allan |last2=McCarley |first2=Robert W. |title=The Brain as a Dream State Generator: An Activation-Synthesis Hypothesis of the Dream Process |journal=The American Journal of Psychiatry |date=December 1977 |volume=134 |issue=12 |pages=1335–1348 |doi=10.1176/ajp.134.12.1335 |pmid=21570 |quote=The dream process is thus seen as having its origin in sensorimotor systems, with little or no primary ideational, volitional, or emotional content. This concept is markedly different from that of the "dream thoughts" or wishes seen by Freud as the primary stimulus for the dream.}}</ref> In 2010 a Harvard study was published showing experimental evidence that dreams were correlated with improved learning.<ref>{{cite web |last1=Benjamin |first1=Victoria |title=Study Links Dreaming to Increased Memory Performance |url=https://www.thecrimson.com/article/2010/4/27/maze-wamsley-group-navigating/#:~:text=A%20recent%20study%20published%20last,nap%2C%20according%20to%20Erin%20J. |website=The Harvard Crimson |access-date=27 January 2022}}</ref> | |||
===In art=== | |||
Dreams and dark imaginings are the theme of ]'s etching '']''. ]'s '']'' (1944) also investigates this theme through absurd juxtapositions of a nude lady, tigers leaping out of a pomegranate, and a spider-like elephant walking in the background. ]'s last painting was '']''. '']'' ("The Dream") is a 1932 painting by ]. | |||
] and Mitchison's 1983 "]" theory, which states that dreams are like the cleaning-up operations of computers when they are offline, removing (suppressing) parasitic nodes and other "junk" from the mind during sleep.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Evans |first1=C. |last2=Newman |first2=E. |year=1964 |title=Dreaming: An analogy from computers |journal=New Scientist |volume=419 |pages=577–579}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |doi=10.1038/304111a0 |last1=Crick |first1=F. |last2=Mitchison |first2=G. |year=1983 |title=The function of dream sleep |journal=Nature |volume=304 |issue=5922 |pages=111–114 |pmid=6866101 |bibcode=1983Natur.304..111C |s2cid=41500914}}</ref> | |||
===In literature=== | |||
{{further|Dream world (plot device)}} | |||
] 1995 proposal that dreams serve a "quasi-therapeutic" function, enabling the dreamer to process trauma in a safe place.<ref>{{cite journal |last=Hartmann |first=Ernest |title=Making Connections in a Safe Place: Is Dreaming Psychotherapy? |journal=Dreaming |year=1995 |volume=5 |issue=4 |pages=213–228 |doi=10.1037/h0094437}}</ref> | |||
Dream frames were frequently used in medieval allegory to justify the narrative; '']''<ref>{{cite web| url= http://public.wsu.edu/~delahoyd/chaucer/BD.html |title= The book of the duchess|publisher=Washington State University|accessdate=May 24, 2012}}</ref> and ''The Vision Concerning ]''<ref>{{cite web| url= http://www.historyguide.org/ancient/langland.html|title= William Langland's The Vision Concerning Piers Plowman|publisher=The History Guide|accessdate=May 24, 2012}}</ref> are two such ]s. Even before them, in antiquity, the same device had been used by ] and ]. | |||
] 2000 threat simulation hypothesis, whose premise is that during much of human evolution, physical and interpersonal threats were serious, giving reproductive advantage to those who survived them. Dreaming aided survival by replicating these threats and providing the dreamer with practice in dealing with them.<ref>{{cite journal |last=Revonsuo |first=A. |title=The reinterpretation of dreams: an evolutionary hypothesis of the function of dreaming |journal=Behavioral and Brain Sciences |year=2000 |volume=23 |issue=6 |pmid=11515147 |doi=10.1017/S0140525X00004015 |pages=877–901 |s2cid=145340071}}</ref> In 2015, Revonsuo proposed social simulation theory, which describes dreams as a simulation for training social skills and bonds.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Revonsuo |first1=A. |last2=Tuominen |first2=J. |year=2015 |title=Avatars in the Machine: Dreaming as a Simulation of Social Reality |journal=Open MIND |pages=1–28 |doi=10.15502/9783958570375|isbn=9783958570375 }}</ref> | |||
] | |||
] and ] 2021 defensive activation theory, which says that, given the brain's ], dreams evolved as a visual hallucinatory activity during sleep's extended periods of darkness, busying the occipital lobe and thereby protecting it from possible appropriation by other, non-vision, sense operations.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Eagleman |first1=David M. |last2=Vaughn |first2=Don A. |title=The Defensive Activation Theory: REM Sleep as a Mechanism to Prevent Takeover of the Visual Cortex |journal=Frontiers in Neuroscience |date=May 2021 |volume=15 |page=632853 |doi=10.3389/fnins.2021.632853 |pmid=34093109 |pmc=8176926 |doi-access=free}}</ref> | |||
They have also featured in ] and ] since the 19th century. One of the best-known dream worlds is ] from ]'s '']'', as well as ] from its sequel, '']''. Unlike many dream worlds, Carroll's logic is like that of actual dreams, with transitions and flexible causality. | |||
] proposes, based on artificial neural networks, that dreams prevent overfitting to past experiences; that is, they enable the dreamer to learn from novel situations.<ref>{{cite news |title=Weird dreams train us for the unexpected, says new theory |url=https://www.theguardian.com/science/2021/may/14/weird-dreams-train-us-for-the-unexpected-says-new-theory |access-date=5 January 2023 |work=the Guardian |date=14 May 2021 |language=en}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Hoel |first1=Erik |title=The overfitted brain: Dreams evolved to assist generalization |journal=Patterns |date=14 May 2021 |volume=2 |issue=5 |pages=100244 |doi=10.1016/j.patter.2021.100244 |pmid=34036289 |pmc=8134940 |language=en |issn=2666-3899|doi-access=free }}</ref> | |||
Other fictional dream worlds include the ] of ]'s '']''<ref>{{cite book|last=Phillips Lovecraft|first=Howard|title=The Dream Cycle of H.P. Lovecraft: Dreams of Terror and Death|year=1995|publisher=Ballantine Books|isbn=0-345-38421-0|url=http://www.google.com/products/catalog?hl=en&qscrl=1&nord=1&rlz=1T4ADFA_enUS369US375&ion=1&bav=on.2,or.r_gc.r_pw.r_qf.,cf.osb&biw=1366&bih=564&wrapid=tlif133790017967410&q=h.p.+lovecraft+dream+cycle&um=1&ie=UTF-8&tbm=shop&cid=7042018729009712854&sa=X&ei=k7y-T-LcGIi22gWzj-2vCg&ved=0CG0Q8wIwCA}}</ref> and '']''{{'}}s<ref>{{cite web| url= http://www.neverendingstory.com/|title= The Neverending Story - Book - Pictures - Video -Icons|publisher=Neverendingstory.com|accessdate=May 24, 2012}}</ref> world of Fantasia, which includes places like the Desert of Lost Dreams, the Sea of Possibilities and the Swamps of Sadness. Dreamworlds, shared hallucinations and other alternate realities feature in a number of works by ], such as '']'' and '']''. Similar themes were explored by ], for instance in '']''. | |||
== Religious and other cultural contexts == | |||
===In popular culture=== | |||
Dreams figure prominently in major world religions. The dream experience for early humans, according to one interpretation, gave rise to the notion of a human "]",<ref>{{cite book |last=Lévy-Bruhl |first=Lucien |author-link=Lucien Lévy-Bruhl |date=1923 |title=Primitive Mentality |translator=Lilian A. Clare |chapter=Chapter III Dreams |chapter-url=https://archive.org/details/primitivementali00levy_0/page/98/mode/2up?ref=ol&view=theater |location=New York |publisher=Macmillan |page=98 |quote=...n dreams,...man passes from the one world to the other without being aware of it. Such is in fact the ordinary idea of the dream to primitive peoples. The "soul" leaves its tenement for the time being. It frequently goes very far away; it communes with spirits or with ghosts. At the moment of awakening it returns to take its place in the body once more.}}</ref> a central element in much religious thought. ] wrote: <blockquote>But there can be no reasonable doubt that the idea of a soul must have first arisen in the mind of primitive man as a result of observation of his dreams. Ignorant as he was, he could have come to no other conclusion but that, in dreams, he left his sleeping body in one universe and went wandering off into another. It is considered that, but for that savage, the idea of such a thing as a 'soul' would never have even occurred to mankind....<ref>{{cite book |last=Dunne |first=J. W. |date=1950 |orig-date=1927 |title=An Experiment with Time |location=London |publisher=Faber |page=23}}</ref></blockquote> | |||
Modern ] often conceives of dreams, like Freud, as expressions of the dreamer's deepest fears and desires.<ref name="Van Riper 56">{{cite book |last=Van Riper |first=A. Bowdoin |title=Science in popular culture: a reference guide |publisher=] |location=Westport |year=2002 |page=56 |isbn=0-313-31822-0}}</ref> The film version of '']'' (1939) depicts a full-color dream that causes Dorothy to perceive her black-and-white reality and those with whom she shares it in a new way. In films such as '']'' (1945), '']'' (1962), '']'' (1989), and '']'' (2010), the protagonists must extract vital clues from surreal dreams.<ref name="Van Riper 57">Van Riper, op. cit., p. 57.</ref> | |||
=== Hindu === | |||
Most dreams in popular culture are, however, not symbolic, but straightforward and realistic depictions of their dreamer's fears and desires.<ref name="Van Riper 57"/> Dream scenes may be indistinguishable from those set in the dreamer's real world, a narrative device that undermines the dreamer's and the audience's sense of security<ref name="Van Riper 57"/> and allows ] protagonists, such as those of '']'' (1976), '']'' (1980) or '']'' (1981) to be suddenly attacked by dark forces while resting in seemingly safe places.<ref name="Van Riper 57"/> | |||
In the ], part of the ] scriptures of ], a dream is one of three states that the soul experiences during its lifetime, the other two states being the waking state and the sleep state.<ref>{{cite web |url=http://www.swami-krishnananda.org/mand/mand_4.html |title=The Mandukya Upanishad, Section 4 |last=Krishnananda |first=Swami |date=16 November 1996 |access-date=26 March 2015 |url-status=dead |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20150409004128/http://www.swami-krishnananda.org/mand/mand_4.html |archive-date=9 April 2015}}</ref> The earliest ], written before 300 BCE, emphasize two meanings of dreams. The first says that dreams are merely expressions of inner desires. The second is the belief of the soul leaving the body and being guided until awakened. | |||
=== Abrahamic === | |||
In ], the line between dreams and reality may be blurred even more in the service of the story.<ref name="Van Riper 57"/> Dreams may be psychically invaded or manipulated ('']'', 1984; the '']'' films, 1984–2010; '']'', 2010) or even come literally true (as in '']'', 1971). In Ursula K. Le Guin's book, '']'' (1971), the protagonist finds that his "effective" dreams can retroactively change reality. ]'s 1977 Australian film ''The Last Wave'' makes a simple and straightforward postulate about the premonitory nature of dreams (from one of his Aboriginal characters) that "... dreams are the shadow of something real". In ]'s novel ''Green Fairy'' from the ] series, the protagonist, Sol, experiences the memories of a dancer who died 100 years before through Absinthe induced dreams and after each dream something from it materializes into his reality. Such stories play to audiences' experiences with their own dreams, which feel as real to them.<ref name="Van Riper 57"/> | |||
], c. 1690. ]]] | |||
In Judaism, dreams are considered part of the experience of the world that can be interpreted and from which lessons can be garnered. It is discussed in the Talmud, Tractate Berachot 55–60. | |||
==Dynamic psychiatry== | |||
The ancient ] connected their dreams heavily with their religion, though the Hebrews were ] and believed that dreams were the voice of one God alone. Hebrews also differentiated between good dreams (from God) and bad dreams (from evil spirits). The Hebrews, like many other ancient cultures, incubated dreams in order to receive a divine revelation. For example, the Hebrew prophet ] would "lie down and sleep in the temple at ] before the Ark and receive the word of the Lord", and ] interpreted a Pharaoh's dream of seven lean cows swallowing seven fat cows as meaning the subsequent seven years would be bountiful, followed by seven years of famine. Most of the dreams in the ] are in the ].<ref>{{cite book |url={{Google books |id=zs3gup4iFu4C |page=15 |plainurl=yes}} |title=A letter that has not been read: Dreams in the Hebrew Bible |first=Shaul |last=Bar |publisher=Hebrew Union College Press |year=2001 |access-date=4 April 2013}}</ref> | |||
===Freudian view=== | |||
In the late 19th century, psychotherapist ] developed a theory that the content of dreams is driven by unconscious ]. Freud called dreams the "] to the unconscious."<ref>Freud, S. (1949)., p. 44</ref> He theorized that the content of dreams reflects the dreamer's unconscious mind and specifically that dream content is shaped by unconscious wish fulfillment. He argued that important unconscious desires often relate to early childhood memories and experiences. Freud's theory describes dreams as having both ] and ] content. Latent content relates to deep unconscious wishes or fantasies while manifest content is superficial and meaningless. Manifest content often masks or obscures latent content. | |||
] mostly shared the beliefs of the Hebrews and thought that dreams were of a supernatural character because the ] includes frequent stories of dreams with divine inspiration. The most famous of these dream stories was ] that stretches from Earth to ]. Many Christians preach that God can speak to people through their dreams. The famous glossary, the ], written in the name of ], attempted to teach Christian populations to interpret their dreams. | |||
In his early work, Freud argued that the vast majority of latent dream content is sexual in nature, but he later moved away from this categorical position. In ''Beyond the Pleasure Principle'' he considered how trauma or aggression could influence dream content. He also discussed supernatural origins in ''Dreams and Occultism'', a lecture published in ''New Introductory Lectures on Psychoanalysis''.<ref>Freud, S. ''New Introductory Lectures on Psychoanalysis'' (pp. 38–70)</ref> | |||
] has researched the role of dreams in ].<ref>{{cite book |last=Edgar |first=Iain |title=The Dream in Islam: From Qur'anic Tradition to Jihadist Inspiration |year=2011 |publisher=Berghahn Books |location=Oxford |isbn=978-0-85745-235-1 |page=178 |url=http://www.berghahnbooks.com/title.php?rowtag=EdgarDream |url-status=dead |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20110929125708/http://www.berghahnbooks.com/title.php?rowtag=EdgarDream |archive-date=29 September 2011 |access-date=9 March 2012}}</ref> He has argued that dreams play an important role in the history of Islam and the lives of Muslims, since dream interpretation is the only way that Muslims can receive revelations from God since the death of the last prophet, ].<ref name="Iain">{{cite journal |last=Edgar |first=Iain R. |author2=Henig, David |title=Istikhara: The Guidance and practice of Islamic dream incubation through ethnographic comparison |journal=History and Anthropology |date=September 2010 |volume=21 |issue=3 |pages=251–262 |doi=10.1080/02757206.2010.496781 |url=https://core.ac.uk/download/pdf/10630.pdf |url-status=dead |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20171108072540/https://core.ac.uk/download/pdf/10630.pdf |archive-date=8 November 2017 |citeseerx=10.1.1.1012.7334 |s2cid=144463607 |access-date=26 October 2017}}</ref> According to Edgar, Islam classifies three types of dreams. Firstly, there is the true dream (al-ru’ya), then the false dream, which may come from the devil (]), and finally, the meaningless everyday dream (hulm). This last dream could be brought forth by the dreamer's ego or base appetite based on what they experienced in the real world. The true dream is often indicated by Islam's ] tradition.<ref name="Iain"/> In one narration by ], the wife of the Prophet, it is said that the Prophet's dreams would come true like the ocean's waves.<ref name="Iain"/> Just as in its predecessors, the ] also recounts the story of Joseph and his unique ability to interpret dreams.<ref name="Iain"/> | |||
===Jungian and other views=== | |||
] rejected many of Freud's theories. Jung expanded on Freud's idea that dream content relates to the dreamer's unconscious desires. He described dreams as ] to the dreamer and argued that dreamers should pay attention for their own good. He came to believe that dreams present the dreamer with revelations that can uncover and help to resolve ] or religious problems and fears.<ref>Jung, 1964, p. 21</ref> | |||
In both Christianity and Islam dreams feature in conversion stories.<ref>{{Cite book |last=Bulkeley |first=Kelly |url=https://archive.org/details/big-dreams-the-science-of-dreaming-and-the-origins-of-religion |title=Big Dreams: The Science of Dreaming and the Origins of Religion |publisher=Oxford University Press |year=2016 |isbn=9780199351534}}</ref> According to ancient authors, Constantine the Great started his conversion to Christianity because he had a dream which prophesied that he would win the ] if he ]."<ref>Lactantius, ''De Mortibus Persecutorum'' 44.4–6, tr. J.L. Creed, ''Lactantius: De Mortibus Persecutorum'' (Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1984), qtd. in Lenski, "Reign of Constantine" (CC), 71.</ref><ref>Eusebius, ''Vita Constantini'' 1.27–29; Barnes, ''Constantine and Eusebius'', 43, 306; Odahl, 105–06, 319–20.</ref> | |||
Jung wrote that ]s show up repeatedly to demand attention, suggesting that the dreamer is neglecting an issue related to the dream. He believed that many of the symbols or images from these dreams return with each dream. Jung believed that memories formed throughout the day also play a role in dreaming. These memories leave impressions for the unconscious to deal with when the ego is at rest. The unconscious mind re-enacts these glimpses of the past in the form of a dream. Jung called this a ''day residue''.<ref>Jung, 1969</ref> Jung also argued that dreaming is not a purely individual concern, that all dreams are part of "one great web of psychological factors." | |||
=== Buddhist === | |||
] presented his theory of dreams as part of the holistic nature of ]. Dreams are seen as projections of parts of the self that have been ignored, rejected, or ].<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Wegner | first1 = D.M. | last2 = Wenzlaff | first2 = R.M. | last3 = Kozak | first3 = M. | year = 2004 | title = The Return of Suppressed Thoughts in Dreams | url = | journal = Psychological Science | volume = 15 | issue = 4| pages = 232–236 | doi=10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00657.x | pmid=15043639}}</ref> Jung argued that one could consider every person in the dream to represent an aspect of the dreamer, which he called the subjective approach to dreams. ] expanded this point of view to say that even inanimate objects in the dream may represent aspects of the dreamer. The dreamer may, therefore, be asked to imagine being an object in the dream and to describe it, in order to bring into awareness the characteristics of the object that correspond with the dreamer's personality. | |||
In Buddhism, ideas about dreams are similar to the classical and folk traditions in South Asia. The same dream is sometimes experienced by multiple people, as in the case of the ], before he is ]. It is described in the '']'' that several of the Buddha's relatives had premonitory dreams preceding this. Some dreams are also seen to transcend time: the Buddha-to-be has certain dreams that are the same as those of ], the '']'' states. In Buddhist literature, dreams often function as a "signpost" motif to mark certain stages in the life of the main character.<ref name="Young 2003">{{cite encyclopedia |last=Young |first=S. |year=2003 |title=Dreams |via=Indian Folklife |volume=13 |url=http://indianfolklore.org/journals/index.php/IFL/article/download/434/497 |encyclopedia=The encyclopedia of South Asian Folklore |page=7 |url-status=dead |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20180225064910/http://indianfolklore.org/journals/index.php/IFL/article/download/434/497 |archive-date=25 February 2018 |access-date=24 February 2018}}</ref> | |||
Buddhist views about dreams are expressed in the ] and the ].<ref name="Young 2003"/> | |||
==Neurobiology== | |||
{{Main|Rapid eye movement sleep}} | |||
] showing brainwaves during REM sleep]] | |||
=== Other === | |||
Accumulated observation has shown that dreams are strongly associated with REM ], during which an ] (EEG) shows brain activity that, among sleep states, is most like wakefulness. Participant-remembered dreams during ] are normally more mundane in comparison.<ref name=Dement1957>{{cite journal | last1 = Dement | first1 = W. | last2 = Kleitman | first2 = N. | year = 1957 | title = The Relation of Eye Movements during Sleep to Dream Activity | url = | journal = Journal of Experimental Psychology | volume = 53 | issue = 5| pages = 339–346 | doi = 10.1037/h0048189 | pmid=13428941}}</ref> During a typical lifespan, a person spends a total of about six years dreaming<ref name="HSWDream">{{cite book |year=2006 |title=How Dream Works |url=http://science.howstuffworks.com/dream3.htm |accessdate=May 4, 2006}}</ref> (which is about two hours each night).<ref>{{cite web |year=2006 |title=Brain Basics: Understanding Sleep |url=http://www.ninds.nih.gov/disorders/brain_basics/understanding_sleep.htm |accessdate=December 16, 2007 |publisher=]}}</ref> Most dreams only last 5 to 20 minutes.<ref name="HSWDream"/> It is unknown where in the brain dreams originate, if there is a single origin for dreams or if multiple portions of the brain are involved, or what the purpose of dreaming is for the body or mind. | |||
] (虎跑夢泉) Statue at Hupao Spring (Hupaomengquan) in Hangzhou, Zhejiang, China]] | |||
In Chinese history, people wrote of two vital aspects of the soul of which one is freed from the body during slumber to journey in a dream realm, while the other remained in the body.<ref name=bulkeley-71/> This belief and dream interpretation had been questioned since early times, such as by the philosopher ] ({{CE|27–97}}).<ref name=bulkeley-71>{{cite book |last=Bulkeley |first=Kelly |title=Dreaming in the world's religions: A comparative history |url=https://archive.org/details/dreamingworldsre00bulk |url-access=limited |year=2008 |isbn=978-0-8147-9956-7 |pages=–73|publisher=NYU Press }}</ref> | |||
During REM sleep, the release of the ]s ], ] and ] is completely suppressed.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Hobson | first1 = J.A. | year = 2009 | title = REM sleep and dreaming: towards a theory of protoconsciousness | url = | journal = Nature Reviews | volume = 10 | issue = 11| pages = 803–813 | doi=10.1038/nrn2716 | pmid=19794431}}</ref><ref>Aston-Jones G., Gonzalez M., & Doran S. (2007). "Role of the locus coeruleus-norepinephrine system in arousal and circadian regulation of the sleep-wake cycle." In G.A. Ordway, M.A. Schwartz, & A. ''Frazer Brain Norepinephrine: Neurobiology and Therapeutics''. Cambridge UP.</ref><ref>Siegel J.M. (2005). "REM Sleep." Ch. 10 in ''Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine''. 4th ed. M.H. Kryger, T. Roth, & W.C. Dement, eds. Elsevier. 120–135. Accessed July 21, 2010. </ref> | |||
The Babylonians and Assyrians divided dreams into "good," which were sent by the gods, and "bad," sent by demons.<ref>Oppenheim, L.A. (1966). ''Mantic Dreams in the Ancient Near East'' in G. E. Von Grunebaum & R. Caillois (Eds.), ''The Dream and Human Societies'' (pp. 341–350). London, England: Cambridge University Press.</ref> A surviving collection of dream omens entitled '']'' records various dream scenarios as well as ] of what will happen to the person who experiences each dream, apparently based on previous cases.<ref name="BlackGreen1992"/><ref>Nils P. Heessel : ''Divinatorische Texte I : ... oneiromantische Omina''. Harrassowitz Verlag, 2007.</ref> Some list different possible outcomes, based on occasions in which people experienced similar dreams with different results.<ref name="BlackGreen1992"/> The Greeks shared their beliefs with the Egyptians on how to interpret good and bad dreams, and the idea of incubating dreams. ], the Greek god of dreams, also sent warnings and prophecies to those who slept at shrines and temples. The earliest Greek beliefs about dreams were that their gods physically visited the dreamers, where they entered through a keyhole, exiting the same way after the divine message was given. | |||
During most dreams, the person dreaming is not aware that they are dreaming, no matter how absurd or eccentric the dream is. The reason for this may be that the ], the region of the brain responsible for logic and planning, exhibits decreased activity during dreams. This allows the dreamer to more actively interact with the dream without thinking about what might happen, since things that would normally stand out in reality blend in with the dream scenery.<ref>Trimble, M. R. (1989). ''The Prefrontal Cortex: Anatomy, Physiology and Neuropsychology of the Frontal Lobe''. ''British Journal Of Psychiatry''</ref> | |||
] wrote the first known Greek book on dreams in the 5th century BCE. In that century, other cultures influenced Greeks to develop the belief that souls left the sleeping body.<ref>O'Neil, C.W. (1976). ''Dreams, culture and the individual''. San Francisco: Chandler & Sharp.</ref> The father of modern medicine, ] ({{BCE|460–375}}), thought dreams could analyze illness and predict diseases.<ref>''On Regimen'' IV, also published sometimes as ''On Dreams''.</ref> For instance, a dream of a dim star high in the night sky indicated problems in the head region, while low in the night sky indicated bowel issues.<ref>{{Cite book |last=Hobson |first=J. A. |year=1988 |title=The Dreaming Brain |publisher=Basic Books}}</ref><ref>Steven M. Oberhelman. 1987. “The Diagnostic Dream in Ancient Medical Theory and Practice.” ''Bulletin of the History of Medicine''. 61 (1): 47-60.</ref> ] (129–216 AD) believed the same thing.<ref>Oberhelman, Steven M. 1983. “Galen, ‘On Diagnosis from Dreams’.” ''Journal of the History of Medicine and Allied Sciences''. 38 (1): 36-47.</ref> Greek philosopher ] (427–347 BCE) wrote that people harbor secret, repressed desires, such as incest, murder, adultery, and conquest, which build up during the day and run rampant during the night in dreams.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=McCurdy |first=H. G. |year=1946 |title=The history of dream theory |journal=Psychological Review |volume=53 |issue=4 |pages=225–233 |doi=10.1037/h0062107|pmid=20998507 }}</ref> Plato's student, ] (384–322 BCE), believed dreams were caused by processing incomplete ] activity during sleep, such as eyes trying to see while the sleeper's eyelids were closed.<ref>{{Cite book |last=Rycroft |first=Charles |year=1979 |title=The Innocence of Dreams |publisher=Random House}}</ref> ], for his part, believed that all dreams are produced by thoughts and conversations a dreamer had during the preceding days.<ref>Cicero, ''De Republica'', </ref> Cicero's '']'' described a lengthy dream vision, which in turn was commented on by ] in his ''Commentarii in Somnium Scipionis''. | |||
When REM sleep episodes were timed for their duration and subjects were awakened to make reports before major editing or forgetting of their dreams could take place, subjects accurately reported the length of time they had been dreaming in an REM sleep state. Some researchers have speculated that "]" effects only seem to be taking place upon reflection and do not truly occur within dreams.<ref>Barbara Bolz. "" in ''A Moment of Science''. Indiana Public Media. September 2, 2009. Accessed August 8, 2010.</ref> This close correlation of REM sleep and dream experience was the basis of the first series of reports describing the nature of dreaming: that it is a regular nightly rather than occasional phenomenon, and is correlated with high-frequency activity within each sleep period occurring at predictable intervals of approximately every 60–90 minutes in all humans throughout the lifespan. | |||
] in his '']'', writes "The visions that occur to us in dreams are, more often than not, the things we have been concerned about during the day."<ref>{{cite book |author=Herodotus |title=The Histories |year=1998 |url=https://archive.org/details/histories0000hero |url-access=registration |publisher=Oxford University Press |page=}}</ref> | |||
REM sleep episodes and the dreams that accompany them lengthen progressively through the night, with the first episode being shortest, of approximately 10–12 minutes duration, and the second and third episodes increasing to 15–20 minutes. Dreams at the end of the night may last as long as 15 minutes, although these may be experienced as several distinct episodes due to momentary arousals interrupting sleep as the night ends. Dream reports can be reported from normal subjects 50% of the time when they are awakened prior to the end of the first REM period. This rate of retrieval is increased to about 99% when awakenings are made from the last REM period of the night. The increase in the ability to recall dreams appears related to intensification across the night in the vividness of dream imagery, colors, and emotions.<ref>{{cite journal|last=Takeuchi|first=Tomoka|title=Dream mechanisms: Is REM sleep indispensable for dreaming?|journal=Sleep & Biological Rhythms|date=June 2005|volume=3|issue=2|pages=56–63|doi=10.1111/j.1479-8425.2005.00165.x}}</ref> <!--this entire paragraph needs references; someone with a copy of Dement's book might be able to help--> | |||
] is a common term within the ] creation narrative of ] for a personal, or group, ] and for what may be understood as the "timeless time" of formative creation and perpetual creating.<ref> {{webarchive |url=https://web.archive.org/web/20090711104046/http://environment.gov.au/parks/uluru/culture-history/culture/index.html |date=11 July 2009}} environment.gov.au, June 23, 2006</ref> | |||
==In other animal species== | |||
REM sleep and the ability to dream seem to be embedded in the biology of many animals in addition to humans. Scientific research suggests that all mammals experience REM.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Lesku | first1 = J. A. | last2 = Meyer | first2 = L. C. R. | last3 = Fuller | first3 = A. | last4 = Maloney | first4 = S. K. | last5 = Dell'Omo | first5 = G. | last6 = Vyssotski | first6 = A. L. | last7 = Rattenborg | first7 = N.C. | year = 2011 | title = Ostriches sleep like platypuses | url = | journal = PLOS ONE | volume = 6 | issue = 8| pages = 1–7 | doi=10.1371/journal.pone.0023203 | pmid=21887239 | pmc=3160860}}</ref> The range of REM can be seen across species: dolphins experience minimal REM, while humans are in the middle of the scale and the ] and the ] (a ]) are among the most prolific dreamers, judging from their REM patterns.<ref>{{cite news|last=Williams|first=Daniel|title=While you where sleeping|url=http://www.time.com/time/magazine/article/0,9171,1606872,00.html|publisher=Time Magazine|accessdate=October 9, 2011|date=April 5, 2007}}</ref> | |||
Some ] tribes and ] populations believe that dreams are a way of visiting and having contact with their ]s.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Tedlock |first1=B. |year=1981 |title=Quiche Maya dream Interpretation |journal=Ethos |volume=9 |issue=4 |pages=313–350 |doi=10.1525/eth.1981.9.4.02a00050 |doi-access=free}}</ref> Some ] tribes have used ]s as a rite of passage, fasting and praying until an anticipated guiding dream was received, to be shared with the rest of the tribe upon their return.<ref>{{cite web |url=http://www.dreams.ca/dreams.htm |last=Webb |first=Craig |year=1995 |title=Dreams: Practical Meaning & Applications |publisher=The DREAMS Foundation |url-status=dead |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20160305002601/http://www.dreams.ca/dreams.htm |archive-date=5 March 2016 |access-date=30 March 2008}}</ref><ref>{{cite web |url=http://dreamhawk.com/dream-encyclopedia/native-american-dream-beliefs/ |access-date=10 April 2012 |title=Native American Dream Beliefs |publisher=Dream Encyclopedia |url-status=dead |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20120415132211/http://dreamhawk.com/dream-encyclopedia/native-american-dream-beliefs/ |archive-date=15 April 2012}}</ref> | |||
Studies have observed signs of dreaming in all mammals studied, including monkeys, dogs, cats, rats, elephants, and shrews. There have also been signs of dreaming in birds and reptiles.<ref>{{cite web|title=Dream|url=http://www.britannica.com/EBchecked/topic/171188/dream|publisher=Encyclopedia Britannica|accessdate=October 26, 2011}}</ref> Sleeping and dreaming are intertwined. Scientific research results regarding the function of dreaming in animals remain disputable; however, the function of sleeping in living organisms is increasingly clear. For example, sleep deprivation experiments conducted on rats and other animals have resulted in the deterioration of physiological functioning and actual tissue damage.<ref>{{cite web|title=Sleep|url=http://www.britannica.com/EBchecked/topic/548545/sleep|publisher=Encyclopedia Britannica|accessdate=October 26, 2011}}</ref> | |||
== Interpretation == | |||
Some scientists argue that humans dream for the same reason other ] do. From a Darwinian perspective dreams would have to fulfill some kind of biological requirement, provide some benefit for natural selection to take place, or at least have no negative impact on fitness. In 2000 Antti Revonsuo, a professor at the University of Turku in Finland, claimed that centuries ago dreams would prepare humans for recognizing and avoiding danger by presenting a simulation of threatening events. The theory has therefore been called the threat-simulation theory.<ref>{{cite news|last=Williams|first=Daniel|title=While you where sleeping|url=http://www.time.com/time/magazine/article/0,9171,1606872,00.html|publisher=Time magazine|accessdate=October 26, 2011|date=April 5, 2007}}</ref> According to Tsoukalas (2012) dreaming is related to the reactive patterns elicited by encounters with predators, a fact that is still evident in the control mechanisms of REM sleep (see below).<ref name=Tsoukalas2012>{{cite journal | last1 = Tsoukalas | first1 = I | year = 2012 | title = The origin of REM sleep: A hypothesis | url = | journal = Dreaming | volume = 22 | issue = 4| pages = 253–283 | doi=10.1037/a0030790}}</ref><ref name=Vitelli2013>Vitelli, R. (2013). Exploring the Mystery of REM Sleep. ''Psychology Today'', On-line blog, March 25</ref> | |||
{{Main|Dream interpretation}} | |||
{{Further|Psychoanalysis|Precognition}} | |||
] (1836–1902).]] | |||
Beginning in the late 19th century, Austrian neurologist ], founder of ], theorized that dreams reflect the dreamer's ] and specifically that dream content is shaped by unconscious wish fulfillment. He argued that important unconscious desires often relate to early childhood memories and experiences.<ref name="Freud1"/> ] and others expanded on Freud's idea that dream content reflects the dreamer's unconscious desires. | |||
==Neurological theories== | |||
{{anchor|Veridical dream}}<!-- Target for ] --> | |||
===Activation synthesis theory=== | |||
Dream interpretation can be a result of subjective ideas and experiences. One study found that most people believe that "their dreams reveal meaningful hidden truths".<ref name=":0">{{Cite journal |title=When dreaming is believing: The (motivated) interpretation of dreams. |journal=Journal of Personality and Social Psychology |pages=249–264 |volume=96 |issue=2 |doi=10.1037/a0013264 |first1=Carey K. |last1=Morewedge |first2=Michael I. |last2=Norton |s2cid=5706448 |pmid=19159131 |year=2009 |url=http://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/4f27/7783ada0dca0d236f368600f166fc7055524.pdf |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20201114093642/http://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/4f27/7783ada0dca0d236f368600f166fc7055524.pdf |url-status=dead |archive-date=14 November 2020}}</ref> The researchers surveyed students in the United States, South Korea, and India, and found that 74% of Indians, 65% of South Koreans and 56% of Americans believed their dream content provided them with meaningful insight into their unconscious beliefs and desires. This Freudian view of dreaming was believed significantly more than theories of dreaming that attribute dream content to memory consolidation, problem-solving, or as a byproduct of unrelated brain activity. The same study found that people attribute more importance to dream content than to similar thought content that occurs while they are awake. Americans were more likely to report that they would intentionally miss their flight if they dreamt of their plane crashing than if they thought of their plane crashing the night before flying (while awake), and that they would be as likely to miss their flight if they dreamt of their plane crashing the night before their flight as if there was an actual plane crash on the route they intended to take. Participants in the study were more likely to perceive dreams to be meaningful when the content of dreams was in accordance with their beliefs and desires while awake. They were more likely to view a positive dream about a friend to be meaningful than a positive dream about someone they disliked, for example, and were more likely to view a negative dream about a person they disliked as meaningful than a negative dream about a person they liked. | |||
{{Main|Activation-synthesis hypothesis}} | |||
According to surveys, it is common for people to feel their dreams are predicting subsequent life events.<ref name="hines"/> Psychologists have explained these experiences in terms of ], namely a selective memory for accurate predictions and distorted memory so that dreams are retrospectively fitted onto life experiences.<ref name="hines">{{cite book |last=Hines |first=Terence |title=Pseudoscience and the Paranormal |publisher=Prometheus Books |year=2003 |pages=78–81 |isbn=978-1-57392-979-0}}</ref> The multi-faceted nature of dreams makes it easy to find connections between dream content and real events.<ref>{{cite book |last=Gilovich |first=Thomas |title=How We Know What Isn't So: the fallibility of human reason in everyday life |publisher=Simon & Schuster |year=1991 |pages=177–180 |isbn=978-0-02-911706-4}}</ref> The term "veridical dream" has been used to indicate dreams that reveal or contain truths not yet known to the dreamer, whether future events or secrets.<ref>{{Cite web |url=http://www.llewellyn.com/encyclopedia/term/Veridical+Dream |title=Llewellyn Worldwide – Encyclopedia: Term: Veridical Dream |website=www.llewellyn.com |access-date=16 October 2016 |url-status=dead |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20161018202429/http://www.llewellyn.com/encyclopedia/term/Veridical+Dream |archive-date=18 October 2016}}</ref> | |||
In 1976 ] and ] proposed a new theory that changed dream research, challenging the previously held ] view of dreams as unconscious wishes to be interpreted. They assume that the same structures that induce REM sleep also generate sensory information. Hobson's 1976 research suggested that the signals interpreted as dreams originate in the brainstem during REM sleep. According to Hobson and other researchers, circuits in the brainstem are activated during REM sleep. Once these circuits are activated, areas of the limbic system involved in emotions, sensations, and memories, including the amygdala and hippocampus, become active. The brain synthesizes and interprets these activities; for example, changes in the physical environment such as temperature and humidity, or physical stimuli such as ejaculation, and attempts to create meaning from these signals, result in dreaming. | |||
In one experiment, subjects were asked to write down their dreams in a diary. This prevented the selective memory effect, and the dreams no longer seemed accurate about the future.<ref>{{cite book |last=Alcock |first=James E. |title=Parapsychology: Science or Magic?: a psychological perspective |publisher=Pergamon Press |location=Oxford |year=1981 |isbn=978-0-08-025773-0}} via {{cite book |last=Hines |first=Terence |title=Pseudoscience and the Paranormal |publisher=Prometheus Books |year=2003 |pages=78–81 |isbn=978-1-57392-979-0}}</ref> Another experiment gave subjects a fake diary of a student with apparently precognitive dreams. This diary described events from the person's life, as well as some predictive dreams and some non-predictive dreams. When subjects were asked to recall the dreams they had read, they remembered more of the successful predictions than unsuccessful ones.<ref>{{cite journal |last=Madey |first=Scott |author2=Thomas Gilovich |title=Effects of Temporal Focus on the Recall of Expectancy-Consistent and Expectancy-Inconsistent Information |journal=Journal of Personality and Social Psychology |volume=65 |issue=3 |year=1993 |doi=10.1037/0022-3514.65.3.458 |pages=458–468 |pmid=8410650}} via {{cite book |last=Kida |first=Thomas |title=Don't Believe Everything You Think: The 6 Basic Mistakes We Make in Thinking |year=2006 |publisher=Prometheus Books |isbn=978-1-59102-408-8 |url=https://archive.org/details/dontbelieveevery00kida}}</ref> | |||
However, research by ] suggests that dreams are generated in the ], and that REM sleep and dreaming are not directly related.<ref>{{cite book |last=Solms |first=M. |year=2000 |title=Dreaming and REM sleep are controlled by different brain mechanisms |publisher= Behavioral and Brain Sciences |edition=23(6) |pages=793–1121}}</ref> While working in the neurosurgery department at hospitals in ] and ], Solms had access to patients with various brain injuries. He began to question patients about their dreams and confirmed that patients with damage to the ] stopped dreaming; this finding was in line with Hobson's 1977 theory. However, Solms did not encounter cases of loss of dreaming with patients having brainstem damage. This observation forced him to question Hobson's prevailing theory, which marked the brainstem as the source of the signals interpreted as dreams. | |||
== Images and literature == | |||
===Continual-activation theory=== | |||
{{Main|Dream art}} | |||
Combining Hobson's activation synthesis hypothesis with Solms' findings, the continual-activation theory of dreaming presented by Jie Zhang proposes that dreaming is a result of brain activation and synthesis; at the same time, dreaming and REM sleep are controlled by different brain mechanisms. Zhang hypothesizes that the function of sleep is to process, encode, and transfer the data from the temporary memory store to the ] store. During ] sleep the conscious-related memory (declarative memory) is processed, and during REM sleep the unconscious related memory (procedural memory) is processed.{{Citation needed|date=October 2014}} | |||
{{Further|Dream world (plot device)}} | |||
Graphic artists, writers and filmmakers all have found dreams to offer a rich vein for creative expression. In the West, artists' depictions of dreams in Renaissance and Baroque art often were related to Biblical narrative. Especially preferred by visual artists were the ] dream in Genesis and ] in the ]. | |||
Zhang assumes that during REM sleep the unconscious part of a brain is busy processing the procedural memory; meanwhile, the level of activation in the conscious part of the brain descends to a very low level as the inputs from the sensory systems are basically disconnected. This triggers the "continual-activation" mechanism to generate a data stream from the memory stores to flow through the conscious part of the brain. Zhang suggests that this pulse-like brain activation is the inducer of each dream. He proposes that, with the involvement of the brain associative thinking system, dreaming is, thereafter, self-maintained with the dreamer's own thinking until the next pulse of memory insertion. This explains why dreams have both characteristics of continuity (within a dream) and sudden changes (between two dreams).<ref>{{cite book |last=Zhang |first=Jie |year=2004 |title=Memory process and the function of sleep |publisher=Journal of Theoretics |edition=6-6 |url=http://www.journaloftheoretics.com/Articles/6-6/Zhang.pdf |accessdate=March 13, 2006}}</ref><ref>{{cite book |last=Zhang |first=Jie |year=2005 |title=Continual-activation theory of dreaming, Dynamical Psychology |url=http://www.goertzel.org/dynapsyc/2005/ZhangDreams.htm |accessdate=March 13, 2006}}</ref> | |||
<gallery> | |||
===Defensive immobilization: the precursor of dreams=== | |||
File:Nicolas Dipre. Le songe de Jacob. c.1500 Avignon, Petit Palais..jpg|Nicolas Dipre. Le songe de Jacob. {{circa|1500}} Avignon, Petit Palais. | |||
According to Tsoukalas (2012) REM sleep is an evolutionary transformation of a well-known defensive mechanism, the ] reflex. This reflex, also known as animal hypnosis or death feigning, functions as the last line of defense against an attacking predator and consists of the total immobilization of the animal: the animal appears dead (cf. "playing possum"). Tsoukalas claims that the neurophysiology and phenomenology of this reaction shows striking similarities to REM sleep, a fact that suggests a deep evolutionary kinship. For example, both reactions exhibit brainstem control, paralysis, sympathetic activation, and thermoregulatory changes. Tsoukalas claims that this theory integrates many earlier findings into a unified framework.<ref name=Tsoukalas2012/><ref name=Vitelli2013/> | |||
File:El sueño de Jacob, by José de Ribera, from Prado in Google Earth.jpg|José de Ribera (1591–1652). El sueño de Jacob, from Prado in Google Earth | |||
File:Loggia_di_raffaello_08.jpg|Raphael. Jacob's Dream (1518) | |||
File:Rembrandt Dream of Joseph.jpg|Rembrandt. Dream of Joseph (1645) | |||
File:Mengs, Traum des hl. Joseph.jpg|Anton Raphael Mengs. Traum des Hl. Joseph (1773 or 1774) | |||
</gallery> | |||
Many later graphic artists have depicted dreams, including Japanese ] artist ] (1760–1849) and Western European painters ] (1844–1910), ] (1881–1973), and ] (1904–1989). | |||
===Dreams as excitations of long-term memory=== | |||
Eugen Tarnow suggests that dreams are ever-present excitations of ], even during waking life. The strangeness of dreams is due to the format of long-term memory, reminiscent of ] & Rasmussen's findings that electrical excitations of the ] give rise to experiences similar to dreams. During waking life an executive function interprets long-term memory consistent with reality checking. Tarnow's theory is a reworking of Freud's theory of dreams in which Freud's unconscious is replaced with the long-term memory system and Freud's "Dream Work" describes the structure of long-term memory.<ref>{{cite book |last=Tarnow |first=Eugen |year=2003 |title=How Dreams And Memory May Be Related |publisher=NEURO-PSYCHOANALYSIS |edition=5(2)}}</ref> | |||
In literature, dream frames were frequently used in medieval allegory to justify the narrative; '']''<ref>{{cite web |url=http://public.wsu.edu/~delahoyd/chaucer/BD.html |title=The book of the duchess|publisher=Washington State University |access-date=24 May 2012 |url-status=dead |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20121114035012/http://public.wsu.edu/~delahoyd/chaucer/BD.html |archive-date=14 November 2012}}</ref> and ''The Vision Concerning ]''<ref>{{cite web |url=http://www.historyguide.org/ancient/langland.html |title=William Langland's The Vision Concerning Piers Plowman |publisher=The History Guide |access-date=24 May 2012 |url-status=dead |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20120606164224/http://www.historyguide.org/ancient/langland.html |archive-date=6 June 2012}}</ref> are two such ]s. Even before them, in antiquity, the same device had been used by ] and ]. | |||
===Dreams for strengthening of semantic memories=== | |||
]]] | |||
] (1820–1914), illustration in ''Alice's Adventures in Wonderland'', 1866 edition]] | |||
A 2001 study showed evidence that illogical locations, characters, and dream flow may help the brain strengthen the linking and consolidation of ].<ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.webmd.com/mental-health/features/the-health-benefits-of-dreams |title=The Health Benefits of Dreams |publisher=Webmd.com |date=February 25, 2009 |accessdate=August 11, 2012}}</ref> These conditions may occur because, during REM sleep, the flow of information between the ] and ] is reduced.<ref>{{cite journal |author=R. Stickgold, J.A. Hobson, R. Fosse, M. Fosse1 |date=November 2001 |title= Sleep, Learning, and Dreams: Off-line Memory Reprocessing |journal=Science |pmid=11691983 |volume=294 |issue=5544 |pages=1052–1057 |doi=10.1126/science.1063530}}</ref> | |||
Dreams have also featured in ] and ] since the 19th century. One of the best-known dream worlds is ] from ]'s '']'', as well as ] from its sequel, '']''. Unlike many dream worlds, Carroll's logic is like that of actual dreams, with transitions and flexible causality. | |||
Increasing levels of the ] hormone ] late in sleep (often during REM sleep) causes this decreased communication. One stage of ] is the linking of distant but related memories. Payne and Nadal hypothesize these memories are then consolidated into a smooth narrative, similar to a process that happens when memories are created under stress.<ref>{{cite journal |doi=10.1101/lm.77104 |author=Jessica D. Payne and Lynn Nadel1 |year=2004 |title=Sleep, dreams, and memory consolidation: The role of the stress hormone cortisol |journal=Learning & Memory |volume=11 |issue=6 |pages=671–678 |issn=1072-0502 |pmid=15576884 |url= http://www.learnmem.org/cgi/content/full/11/6/671 |pmc=534}}</ref> | |||
Robert (1886),<ref>Robert, W. Der Traum als Naturnothwendigkeit erklärt. Zweite Auflage, Hamburg: Seippel, 1886.</ref> a physician from Hamburg, was the first who suggested that dreams are a need and that they have the function to erase (a) sensory impressions that were not fully worked up, and (b) ideas that were not fully developed during the day. By the dream work, incomplete material is either removed (suppressed) or deepened and included into memory. Robert's ideas were cited repeatedly by Freud in his ''Die Traumdeutung''. ] (1911) viewed that sleep serves to sweep away unnecessary memories and connections from the day. | |||
Other fictional dream worlds include the ] of ]'s '']''<ref>{{cite book |last=Lovecraft |first=Howard Phillips |title=The Dream Cycle of H. P. Lovecraft: Dreams of Terror and Death |year=1995 |publisher=Ballantine Books |isbn=978-0-345-38421-8 |url=http://www.google.com/products/catalog?qscrl=1&nord=1&rlz=1T4ADFA_enUS369US375&ion=1&bav=on.2,or.r_gc.r_pw.r_qf.,cf.osb&biw=1366&bih=564&wrapid=tlif133790017967410&q=h.p.+lovecraft+dream+cycle&um=1&tbm=shop&cid=7042018729009712854 |url-status=live |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20130903092316/http://www.google.com/products/catalog?qscrl=1&nord=1&rlz=1T4ADFA_enUS369US375&ion=1&bav=on.2,or.r_gc.r_pw.r_qf.,cf.osb&biw=1366&bih=564&wrapid=tlif133790017967410&q=h.p.+lovecraft+dream+cycle&um=1&tbm=shop&cid=7042018729009712854 |archive-date=3 September 2013}}</ref> and '']''{{'}}s<ref>{{cite web |url=http://www.neverendingstory.com/ |title=The Neverending Story – Book – Pictures – Video – Icons |access-date=24 May 2012 |url-status=dead |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20120601055446/http://www.neverendingstory.com/ |archive-date=1 June 2012}}</ref> world of Fantastica, which includes places like the Desert of Lost Dreams, the Sea of Possibilities and the Swamps of Sadness. Dreamworlds, shared hallucinations and other alternate realities feature in a number of works by ], such as '']'' and '']''. Similar themes were explored by ], for instance in '']''. | |||
This was revised in 1983 by Crick and Mitchison's "]" theory, which states that dreams are like the cleaning-up operations of computers when they are off-line, removing (suppressing) parasitic nodes and other "junk" from the mind during sleep.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Evans | first1 = C. | last2 = Newman | first2 = E. | year = 1964 | title = Dreaming: An analogy from computers | url = | journal = New Scientist | volume = 419 | issue = | pages = 577–579 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | doi = 10.1038/304111a0 | last1 = Crick | first1 = F. | last2 = Mitchison | first2 = G. | year = 1983 | title = The function of dream sleep | url = | journal = Nature | volume = 304 | issue = 5922| pages = 111–114 | pmid = 6866101 }}</ref> However, the opposite view that dreaming has an information handling, memory-consolidating function (Hennevin and Leconte, 1971) is also common. | |||
Modern ] often conceives of dreams, as did Freud, as expressions of the dreamer's deepest fears and desires.<ref name="Van Riper 56">{{cite book |last=Van Riper |first=A. Bowdoin |title=Science in popular culture: a reference guide |publisher=] |location=Westport |year=2002 |pages=56–57 |isbn=978-0-313-31822-1}}</ref> In speculative fiction, the line between dreams and reality may be blurred even more in service to the story.<ref name="Van Riper 57">Van Riper, op. cit., p. 57.</ref> Dreams may be psychically invaded or manipulated ('']'', 1984; the '']'' films, 1984–2010; '']'', 2010) or even come literally true (as in '']'', 1971).<ref name="Van Riper 56"/> | |||
==Psychological theories== | |||
== Lucidity == | |||
===Dreams for testing and selecting mental schemas=== | |||
{{Main|Lucid dream}} | |||
Coutts<ref>{{cite journal | doi = 10.2466/pr0.102.2.561-574 | last1 = Coutts | first1 = R | year = 2008 | title = Dreams as modifiers and tests of mental schemas: an emotional selection hypothesis | url = | journal = Psychological Reports | volume = 102 | issue = 2| pages = 561–574 | pmid = 18567225 }}</ref> describes dreams as playing a central role in a two-phase sleep process that improves the mind's ability to meet human needs during wakefulness. During the accommodation phase, mental schemas self-modify by incorporating dream themes. During the ] phase, dreams test prior schema accommodations. Those that appear adaptive are retained, while those that appear maladaptive are culled. The cycle maps to the sleep cycle, repeating several times during a typical night's sleep. ] suggested that dreams are often emotional preparations for solving problems, intoxicating an individual away from common sense toward private logic. The residual dream feelings may either reinforce or inhibit contemplated action. | |||
Lucid dreaming is the conscious perception of one's state while dreaming. In this state the dreamer may often have some degree of control over their own actions within the dream or even the characters and the environment of the dream. Dream control has been reported to improve with practiced deliberate lucid dreaming, but the ability to control aspects of the dream is not necessary for a dream to qualify as "lucid"—a lucid dream is any dream during which the dreamer knows they are dreaming.<ref> {{webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20070313100513/http://www.psychwww.com/asc/ld/faq.html |date=13 March 2007}} by ] at Psych Web.</ref> The occurrence of lucid dreaming has been scientifically verified.<ref name=Watanabe2003>{{cite journal |author=Watanabe, T. |year=2003 |title=Lucid Dreaming: Its Experimental Proof and Psychological Conditions |journal=J Int Soc Life Inf Sci |volume=21 |issue=1 |issn=1341-9226}}</ref> | |||
===Evolutionary psychology theories of dreams=== | |||
Numerous theories state that dreaming is a random by-product of REM sleep physiology and that it does not serve any natural purpose.<ref>{{cite journal|last=Revonsuo|first=A.|title=The reinterpretation of dreams: an evolutionary hypothesis of the function of dreaming|journal=Behavioral and Brain Sciences|year=2000|volume=23|issue=6|url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov.proxyau.wrlc.org/pubmed/11515147|doi=10.1017/S0140525X00004015|pages=877–901}}</ref> Flanagan claims that "dreams are evolutionary ]" and they have no adaptive function. "Dreaming came along as a free ride on a system designed to think and to sleep."<ref>{{cite book|last=Blackmore|first=Susan|title=Consciousness an introduction|year=2004|publisher=Oxford University Press|location=New York, NY|isbn=978-0-19-515343-9|page=342}}</ref> Hobson, for different reasons, also considers dreams epiphenomena. He believes that the substance of dreams have no significant influence on waking actions, and most people go about their daily lives perfectly well without remembering their dreams.<ref>{{cite book|last=Blackmore|first=Susan|title=Consciousness an introduction|year=2004|publisher=Oxford University Press|location=New York, NY|isbn=978-0-19-515343-9|pages=342–343}}</ref> | |||
"]" is a term sometimes used for those who lucidly dream. | |||
Hobson proposed the activation-synthesis theory, which states that "there is a randomness of dream imagery and the randomness synthesizes dream-generated images to fit the patterns of internally generated stimulations".<ref>{{cite web|last=Tubo|first=J|title=The evolution of dreaming|url=http://webpub.allegheny.edu/employee/r/rmumme/FS101/ResearchPapers/JackieTubo.html}}</ref> This theory is based on the physiology of REM sleep, and Hobson believes dreams are the outcome of the forebrain reacting to random activity beginning at the brainstem. The activation-synthesis theory hypothesizes that the peculiar nature of dreams is attributed to certain parts of the brain trying to piece together a story out of what is essentially bizarre information.<ref>{{cite journal|last=Franklin|first=M|author2=Zyphur, M|title=The role of dreams in the evolution of the human mind|journal=Evolutionary Psychology|year=2005|volume=3|pages=59–78|url=http://www.epjournal.net/filestore/ep035978.pdf}}</ref> | |||
In 1975, psychologist Keith Hearne successfully recorded a communication from a dreamer experiencing a lucid dream. On April 12, 1975, after agreeing to move his eyes left and right upon becoming lucid, the subject and Hearne's co-author on the resulting article, Alan Worsley, successfully carried out this task.<ref name="Lucid dream communication">{{cite web |url=http://www.keithhearne.com/wp-content/uploads/2010/06/RECHTSCHAFFEN.pdf |title=The University of Chicago Department of Psychiatry |date=5 September 1975 |access-date=21 October 2013 |url-status=dead |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20120426081653/http://www.keithhearne.com/wp-content/uploads/2010/06/RECHTSCHAFFEN.pdf |archive-date=26 April 2012}}</ref> Years later, psychophysiologist ] conducted similar work including: | |||
However, evolutionary psychologists believe dreams serve some adaptive function for survival. ] describes dreaming as simply "thinking in different biochemical state" and believes people continue to work on all the same problems—personal and objective—in that state.<ref>{{cite book|url={{Google books|id=ojvXAAAAMAAJ|plainurl=yes}}|last=Barrett|first=Deirdre|chapter=An Evolutionary Theory of Dreams and Problem-Solving |editor-last=Barrett|editor-first=D. L.|editor2-last=McNamara |editor2-first=P. |title=The New Science of Dreaming, Volume III: Cultural and Theoretical Perspectives on Dreaming |location=New York, NY |publisher=Praeger/Greenwood |year=2007 |accessdate=April 4, 2013}}</ref> Her research finds that anything—math, musical composition, business dilemmas—may get solved during dreaming.<ref>{{cite book |url={{Google books |id=v-SzPAAACAAJ |plainurl=yes }} |last=Barrett |first=Deirdre |title=The Committee of Sleep: How Artists, Scientists, and Athletes Use their Dreams for Creative Problem Solving—and How You Can Too |location=New York |publisher=Crown Books/Random House |year=2001 }}{{dead link|date=April 2013}}</ref><ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.asdreams.org/journal/articles/barrett3-2.htm |title=Barrett, Deirdre. The 'Committee of Sleep': A Study of Dream Incubation for Problem Solving. Dreaming: Journal of the Association for the Study of Dreams, 1993, 3, pp. 115–123 |publisher=Asdreams.org |accessdate=April 4, 2013}}</ref> In a related theory, which ] terms "Oneiric Darwinism," dreams are seen as creating new ideas through the generation of random thought mutations. Some of these may be rejected by the mind as useless, while others may be seen as valuable and retained.<ref>Blechner, M. (2001) ''The Dream Frontier''. Hillsdale, NJ: The Analytic Press.</ref> | |||
* Using eye signals to map the subjective sense of time in dreams. | |||
* Comparing the electrical activity of the brain while singing awake and while dreaming. | |||
* Studies comparing in-dream sex, arousal, and orgasm.<ref>{{cite book|doi=10.1037/14258-006 |chapter=Lucid dreaming: Paradoxes of dreaming consciousness |title=Varieties of anomalous experience: Examining the scientific evidence |edition=2nd |date=2014 |last1=Laberge |first1=Stephen |pages=145–173 |isbn=978-1-4338-1529-4 |s2cid=152082735 }} | |||
</ref> | |||
Communication between two dreamers has also been documented. The processes involved included ] monitoring, ocular signaling, incorporation of reality in the form of red light stimuli and a coordinating website. The website tracked when both dreamers were dreaming and sent the stimulus to one of the dreamers where it was incorporated into the dream. This dreamer, upon becoming lucid, signaled with eye movements; this was detected by the website whereupon the stimulus was sent to the second dreamer, invoking incorporation into that dreamer's dream.<ref>{{Cite web |url=https://www.forbes.com/sites/parmyolson/2012/11/08/saying-hi-through-a-dream-how-the-internet-could-make-sleeping-more-social/#2eb99101590c |title=Saying 'Hi' Through A Dream: How The Internet Could Make Sleeping More Social |last=Olson |first=Parmy |website=Forbes |access-date=24 April 2016 |url-status=live |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20160531010710/http://www.forbes.com/sites/parmyolson/2012/11/08/saying-hi-through-a-dream-how-the-internet-could-make-sleeping-more-social/#2eb99101590c |archive-date=31 May 2016}}</ref> | |||
== Recollection == | |||
Finnish psychologist Antti Revonsuo posits that dreams have evolved for "threat simulation" exclusively. According to the Threat Simulation Theory he proposes, during much of human evolution physical and interpersonal threats were serious, giving reproductive advantage to those who survived them. Therefore, dreaming evolved to replicate these threats and continually practice dealing with them. In support of this theory, Revonsuo shows that contemporary dreams comprise much more threatening events than people meet in daily non-dream life, and the dreamer usually engages appropriately with them.<ref name="Blackmore 2004 342–343">{{cite book|last=Blackmore|first=Susan|title=Consciousness an Introduction|year=2004|publisher=Oxford University Press|location=New York, NY|isbn=978-0-19-515343-9|pages=342–343}}</ref> It is suggested by this theory that dreams serve the purpose of allowing for the rehearsal of threatening scenarios in order to better prepare an individual for real-life threats. | |||
{{Further|Dream diary}} | |||
] and Franz Riepenhausen.]] | |||
The recollection of dreams is extremely unreliable, though it is a skill that can be trained. Dreams can usually be recalled if a person is awakened while dreaming.<ref name="npr"/> Women tend to have more frequent dream recall than men.<ref name="npr">{{cite web |url=https://www.npr.org/templates/story/story.php?storyId=15778923 |title=The Science Behind Dreams and Nightmares |website=NPR.org |access-date=4 April 2013 |url-status=live |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20130822180530/http://www.npr.org/templates/story/story.php?storyId=15778923 |archive-date=22 August 2013}}</ref> Dreams that are difficult to recall may be characterized by relatively little ], and factors such as ], ], and interference play a role in dream recall. Often, a dream may be recalled upon viewing or hearing a random trigger or stimulus. The ] proposes that dream content that is salient, that is, novel, intense, or unusual, is more easily remembered. There is considerable evidence that vivid, intense, or unusual dream content is more frequently recalled.<ref name=Watson>{{cite journal |doi=10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00114-9 |title=To dream, perchance to remember: Individual differences in dream recall |year=2003 |last1=Watson |first1=David |journal=Personality and Individual Differences |volume=34 |issue=7 |pages=1271–1286}}</ref> A ] can be used to assist dream recall, for personal interest or ] purposes. | |||
According to Tsoukalas (2012) the biology of dreaming is related to the reactive patterns elicited by predatorial encounters (especially the tonic immobility reflex), a fact that lends support to evolutionary theories claiming that dreams specialize in threat avoidance and/or emotional processing.<ref name="Tsoukalas2012"/> | |||
Adults report remembering around two dreams per week, on average.<ref>{{cite news |title=Why Do Some People Always Remember Their Dreams, While Others Almost Never Do? |url=https://www.discovermagazine.com/mind/why-do-some-people-always-remember-their-dreams-while-others-almost-never |access-date=10 February 2021 |work=Discover Magazine |date=2019}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Herlin |first1=Bastien |last2=Leu-Semenescu |first2=Smaranda |last3=Chaumereuil |first3=Charlotte |last4=Arnulf |first4=Isabelle |title=Evidence that non-dreamers do dream: a REM sleep behaviour disorder model |journal=Journal of Sleep Research |date=December 2015 |volume=24 |issue=6 |pages=602–609 |doi=10.1111/jsr.12323 |pmid=26307463 |quote=Adults report, on average, 1–2.8 dream recalls per week in a dream questionnaire and 2.38 dream recalls per week when a home dream diary is completed |doi-access=free}}</ref> Unless a dream is particularly vivid and if one wakes during or immediately after it, the content of the dream is typically not remembered.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Hobson |first1=J.A. |last2=McCarly |first2=R.W. |s2cid=10396934 |year=1977 |title=The brain as a dream-state generator: An activation-synthesis hypothesis of the dream process |journal=American Journal of Psychiatry |volume=134 |issue=12 |pages=1335–1348 |pmid=21570 |doi=10.1176/ajp.134.12.1335 |url=https://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/f1af/886bfac2ee058ddaf1a6fb61dabe08e19b08.pdf |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20170709221130/https://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/f1af/886bfac2ee058ddaf1a6fb61dabe08e19b08.pdf |url-status=dead |archive-date=9 July 2017}}</ref> | |||
English writer Anders Johansson argues that dreams show desire as well as fear; and relates dreams to four areas essential for humans to survive and thrive: physical danger, social status, health and sexual reproduction. He argues that we dream of danger and humiliation and bad health in order to encourage us to fear and avoid these things; but also dream of higher social status and desirable members of the opposite sex in order to encourage our pursuit of them – thus making dreams a total evolutionary system.<ref>Johansson, A. (2014) ''Bête Noire''. Salisbury, UK: AH Publishing.</ref> | |||
In line with the salience hypothesis, there is considerable evidence that people who have more vivid, intense or unusual dreams show better recall. There is evidence that continuity of consciousness is related to recall. Specifically, people who have vivid and unusual experiences during the day tend to have more memorable dream content and hence better dream recall. People who score high on measures of personality traits associated with creativity, imagination, and fantasy, such as ], ]ing, ], ], and ], tend to show more frequent dream recall.<ref name= Watson/> There is also evidence for continuity between the bizarre aspects of dreaming and waking experience. That is, people who report more bizarre experiences during the day, such as people high in ] (psychosis proneness), have more frequent dream recall and also report more frequent ]s.<ref name= Watson/> | |||
===Other hypotheses=== | |||
There are many other hypotheses about the function of dreams, including:<ref name="cartwrightcontent">{{cite encyclopedia |year=1993 |title=Functions of Dreams |encyclopedia=Encyclopedia of Sleep and Dreaming |last=Cartwright |first=Rosalind D}}</ref> | |||
* Dreams allow the repressed parts of the mind to be satisfied through ] while keeping the conscious mind from thoughts that would suddenly cause one to awaken from shock.<ref>{{cite book |last= Vedfelt |first=Ole |title=The Dimensions of Dreams |publisher=Fromm |year=1999 |isbn= 0-88064-230-0}}</ref> | |||
* ]<ref>Ferenczi, S. (1913) To whom does one relate one's dreams? In: ''Further Contributions to the Theory and Technique of Psycho-Analysis.'' New York: Brunner/Mazel, 349.</ref> proposed that the dream, when told, may communicate something that is not being said outright. | |||
* Dreams regulate mood.<ref>Kramer, M. (1993) The selective mood regulatory function of dreaming: An update and revision. In: ''The Function of Dreaming''. Ed., A. Moffitt, M. Kramer, & R. Hoffmann. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.</ref> | |||
* Hartmann<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Hartmann | first1 = E. | year = 1995 | title = Making connections in a safe place: Is dreaming psychotherapy? | url = | journal = Dreaming | volume = 5 | issue = 4| pages = 213–228 | doi = 10.1037/h0094437 }}</ref> says dreams may function like psychotherapy, by "making connections in a safe place" and allowing the dreamer to integrate thoughts that may be dissociated during waking life. | |||
* LaBerge and DeGracia<ref>LaBerge, S. & DeGracia, D.J. (2000). Varieties of lucid dreaming experience. In R.G. Kunzendorf & B. Wallace (Eds.), Individual Differences in Conscious Experience (pp. 269-307). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.</ref> have suggested that dreams may function, in part, to recombine unconscious elements within consciousness on a temporary basis by a process they term "mental recombination", in analogy with genetic recombination of DNA. From a bio-computational viewpoint, mental recombination may contribute to maintaining an optimal information processing flexibility in brain information networks. | |||
* ] in his '']'', writes "The visions that occur to us in dreams are, more often than not, the things we have been concerned about during the day."<ref>{{cite book|author=Herodotus|title=The Histories|publisher=Oxford University Press|page=414}}</ref> | |||
=== Dream-recording machine === | |||
==Content== | |||
Recording or reconstructing dreams may one day assist with dream recall.<ref name="Underwood-2013-Science-Dream-Machine">{{cite journal |last1=Underwood |first1=Emily |title=How to Build a Dream-Reading Machine |journal=Science |date=5 April 2013 |volume=340 |issue=6128 |pages=21 |doi=10.1126/science.340.6128.21 |pmid=23559230 |url=https://www.science.org/doi/10.1126/science.340.6128.21 |access-date=15 May 2023 |language=en |issn=0036-8075}}</ref><ref>{{Cite web |last=Hawks |first=Charlotte |date=April 5, 2018 |title=How close are we to video-recording our dreams? |url=https://edition.cnn.com/2018/04/04/health/recording-our-dreams/index.html |website=CNN}}</ref> Using the permitted non-invasive technologies, functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and ] (EMG), researchers have been able to identify basic dream imagery,<ref>{{Cite news |url=https://www.bbc.com/news/science-environment-22031074 |title=Scientists 'read dreams' using brain scans |website=BBC News |access-date=24 April 2016 |url-status=live |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20160427185103/http://www.bbc.com/news/science-environment-22031074 |archive-date=27 April 2016 |date=4 April 2013 |last1=Morelle |first1=Rebecca |author-link=Rebecca Morelle}}</ref> ] activity<ref>{{Cite book |url=https://books.google.com/books?id=esv8GMVwHwEC |title=The Psychophysiology of Thinking: Studies of Covert Processes |last=Mcguigan |first=F. |year=2012 |publisher=Elsevier |isbn=978-0-323-14700-2}}</ref> and dream motor behavior (such as walking and hand movements).<ref>{{Cite book |url=https://books.google.com/books?id=n6YXNrpA8ZYC |title=States of Consciousness: Experimental Insights into Meditation, Waking, Sleep and Dreams |last1=Cvetkovic |first1=Dean |last2=Cosic |first2=Irena |year=2011 |publisher=Springer Science & Business Media |isbn=978-3-642-18047-7}}</ref><ref>{{Cite web |url=https://huffingtonpost.com/dreamscloud/can-we-turn-our-dreams-in_b_9152612.html |title=Can We Turn Our Dreams Into Watchable Movies? |last=Oldis |first=Daniel |website=The Huffington Post |access-date=2016-08-20 |url-status=live |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20160804163822/http://www.huffingtonpost.com/dreamscloud/can-we-turn-our-dreams-in_b_9152612.html |archive-date=4 August 2016 |date=4 February 2016}}</ref> | |||
From the 1940s to 1985, ] collected more than 50,000 dream reports at ]. In 1966 Hall and Van De Castle published ''The Content Analysis of Dreams'', in which they outlined a coding system to study 1,000 dream reports from college students.<ref name="hallcontent">Hall, C., & Van de Castle, R. (1966). The Content Analysis of Dreams. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. </ref> Results indicated that participants from varying parts of the world demonstrated similarity in their dream content. Hall's complete dream reports were made publicly available in the mid-1990s by Hall's protégé ]. | |||
== |
== Miscellany == | ||
=== Illusion of reality === | |||
The visual nature of dreams is generally highly phantasmagoric; that is, different locations and objects continuously blend into each other. The visuals (including locations, characters/people, objects/artifacts) are generally reflective of a person's memories and experiences, but conversation can take on highly exaggerated and bizarre forms. | |||
{{Main|Dream argument}} | |||
Some philosophers have proposed that what we think of as the "real world" could be or is an illusion (an idea known as the ] about ]). The first recorded mention of the idea was in the 4th century BCE by ], and in Eastern philosophy, the problem has been named the "]." | |||
People who are blind from birth do not have visual dreams. Their dream contents are related to other senses like ], ], ] and ], whichever are present since birth.<ref>{{cite news|title=How do blind people dream? - The Body Odd |url=http://bodyodd.nbcnews.com/_news/2012/03/09/10602730-how-do-blind-people-dream?lite |date=March 2012 |accessdate=May 10, 2013}}</ref> | |||
<blockquote>He who dreams of drinking wine may weep when morning comes; he who dreams of weeping may in the morning go off to hunt. While he is dreaming he does not know it is a dream, and in his dream he may even try to interpret a dream. Only after he wakes does he know it was a dream. And someday there will be a great awakening when we know that this is all a great dream. Yet the stupid believe they are awake, busily and brightly assuming they understand things, calling this man ruler, that one herdsman—how dense! Confucius and you are both dreaming! And when I say you are dreaming, I am dreaming, too. Words like these will be labeled the Supreme Swindle. Yet, after ten thousand generations, a great sage may appear who will know their meaning, and it will still be as though he appeared with astonishing speed.<ref>莊子, 齊物論, 12. ''Zhuàngzi'', "Discussion on making all things equal," 12. ''from'' Zhuàngzi, Burton Watson trans., ''Chuang Tzu'' (New York: Columbia University Press, 1996), 43. {{ISBN|978-0-231-10595-8}} </ref></blockquote> | |||
The idea also is discussed in Hindu and Buddhist writings.<ref>{{cite book |title=Buddhism As Presented by the Brahmanical Systems |last=Kher |first=Chitrarekha V. |year=1992 |publisher=Sri Satguru Publications |isbn=978-81-7030-293-3}}</ref> It was formally introduced to Western philosophy by ] in the 17th century in his '']''. | |||
===Emotions=== | |||
In the Hall study, the most common emotion experienced in dreams was ]. Other emotions included ], ], ], ], and ]. Negative emotions were much more common than positive ones.<ref name="hallcontent"/> | |||
=== Absent-minded transgression === | |||
===Sexual themes=== | |||
Dreams of absent-minded transgression (DAMT) are dreams wherein the dreamer absent-mindedly performs an action that he or she has been trying to stop (one classic example is of a quitting smoker having dreams of lighting a cigarette). Subjects who have had DAMT have reported waking with intense feelings of ]. One study found a positive association between having these dreams and successfully stopping the behavior.<ref>{{cite journal |vauthors=Hajek P, Belcher M |title=Dream of absent-minded transgression: an empirical study of a cognitive withdrawal symptom |journal=J Abnorm Psychol |volume=100 |issue=4 |pages=487–491 |year=1991 |pmid=1757662 |doi=10.1037/0021-843X.100.4.487}}</ref> | |||
The Hall data analysis shows that sexual dreams occur no more than 10% of the time and are more prevalent in young to mid-teens.<ref name="hallcontent"/> Another study showed that 8% of both men and women's dreams have sexual content.<ref>Zadra, A., , ''Sleep'' Volume 30, Abstract Supplement, 2007 A376.</ref> In some cases, sexual dreams may result in ]s or ]s. These are colloquially known as ''wet dreams''.<ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.measuredhs.com/pubs/pdf/FR157/04Chapter04.pdf |title=Badan Pusat Statistik "Indonesia Young Adult Reproductive Health Survey 2002–2004" p. 27 |format=PDF |accessdate=April 4, 2013}}</ref> | |||
=== |
===Non-REM dreams=== | ||
Hypnogogic and hypnopompic dreams, dreamlike states shortly after falling asleep and shortly before awakening, and dreams during stage 2 of ], also occur, but are shorter than REM-dreams.<ref>{{Cite web|url=https://www.psychologytoday.com/us/blog/dreaming-in-the-digital-age/201712/the-science-dreaming-9-key-points|title=The Science of Dreaming: 9 Key Points | Psychology Today|website=www.psychologytoday.com}}</ref><ref></ref> | |||
A small minority of people say that they dream only in black and white.<ref>{{cite journal |author1=Michael Schredl |author2=Petra Ciric |author3=Simon Götz |author4=Lutz Wittmann |date=November 2004 |title=Typical Dreams: Stability and Gender Differences |journal=The Journal of Psychology |volume=138 |issue=6 |doi=10.3200/JRLP.138.6.485-494 |pmid=15612605 |pages=485–94}}</ref>{{failed verification|date=April 2015}} A 2008 study by a researcher at the ] found that people who were only exposed to black and white television and film in childhood reported dreaming in black and white about 25% of the time.<ref>{{cite news |first=Richard |last=Alleyne |date=October 17, 2008 |title=Black and white TV generation have monochrome dreams |newspaper=The Daily Telegraph| location=London |url=http://www.telegraph.co.uk/science/science-news/3353504/Black-and-white-TV-generation-have-monochrome-dreams.html}}</ref> | |||
=== Daydreams === | |||
====Relationship with medical conditions==== | |||
{{Main|Daydream}} | |||
There is evidence that certain medical conditions (normally only neurological conditions) can impact dreams. For instance, some people with ] have never reported entirely black-and-white dreaming, and often have a difficult time imagining the idea of dreaming in only black and white.<ref>{{cite book |title=Synaesthesia: The Strangest Thing |last=Harrison |first=John E. |year=2001 |publisher=Oxford University Press |isbn=0-19-263245-0}}</ref> | |||
]]] | |||
A daydream is a visionary ], especially one of happy, pleasant thoughts, hopes or ambitions, imagined as coming to pass, and experienced while awake.<ref name="Klinger">Klinger, Eric (October 1987). '']''.</ref> There are many different types of daydreams, and there is no consistent definition amongst psychologists.<ref name="Klinger"/> The general public also uses the term for a broad variety of experiences. Research by Harvard psychologist ] has found that people who experience vivid dreamlike ]s reserve the word for these, whereas many other people refer to milder imagery, realistic future planning, review of memories or just "spacing out"—i.e. one's mind going relatively blank—when they talk about "daydreaming".<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Barrett |first1=D.L. |year=1979 |title=The Hypnotic Dream: Its Content in Comparison to Nocturnal Dreams and Waking Fantasy |journal=Journal of Abnormal Psychology |volume=88 |issue=5 |pages=584–591 |doi=10.1037/0021-843x.88.5.584}}</ref><ref>Barrett, D.L. "Fantasizers and Dissociaters: Two types of High Hypnotizables, Two Imagery Styles". in R. Kusendorf, N. Spanos, & B. Wallace (Eds.) ''Hypnosis and Imagination''. New York: Baywood, 1996. and, Barrett, D.L. "Dissociaters, Fantasizers, and their Relation to Hypnotizability" in Barrett, D.L. (Ed.) ''Hypnosis and Hypnotherapy'', (2 vol.): Vol. 1: ''History, theory and general research'', Vol. 2: ''Psychotherapy research and applications'', New York: Praeger/Greenwood, 2010.</ref> | |||
==Interpretations== | |||
{{Main|Dream interpretation}} | |||
While daydreaming has long been derided as a lazy, non-productive pastime, it is now commonly acknowledged that daydreaming can be constructive in some contexts.<ref>{{cite news |first=John |last=Tierney |author-link=John Tierney (journalist) |title=Discovering the Virtues of a Wandering Mind |url=https://www.nytimes.com/2010/06/29/science/29tier.html |newspaper=] |date=28 June 2010 |url-status=live |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20170421104306/http://www.nytimes.com/2010/06/29/science/29tier.html |archive-date=21 April 2017}}</ref> There are numerous examples of people in creative or artistic careers, such as composers, novelists and filmmakers, developing new ideas through daydreaming. Similarly, research scientists, mathematicians and physicists have developed new ideas by daydreaming about their subject areas. | |||
=== Common interpretations of dreams === | |||
Dream interpretation can be a result of subjective ideas and experiences. A recent paper by Carey Morewedge and Michael Norton (2009) in the Journal of Personality and Social Psychology <ref name=":0" /> found that most people believe that "their dreams reveal meaningful hidden truths". In one study conducted in the United States, South Korea and India, they found that 74% of Indians, 65% of South Koreans and 56% of Americans believed their dream content provided them with meaningful insight into their unconscious beliefs and desires. This Freudian view of dreaming was endorsed significantly more than theories of dreaming than attribute dream content to memory consolidation, problem solving, or random brain activity. | |||
=== |
=== Hallucination === | ||
{{Main|Hallucination}} | |||
In the paper, Morewedge and Norton (2009) also found that people attribute more importance to dream content than to similar thought content that occurs while they are awake. In one study, Americans were more likely to report that they would miss their flight if they dreamt of their plane crashing than if they thought of their plane crashing the night before flying (while awake), and that they would be as likely to miss their flight if they dreamt of their plane crashing the night before their flight as if there was an actual plane crash on the route they intended to take.<ref name=":0" /> Not all dream content was considered equally important. Participants in their studies were more likely to perceive dreams to be meaningful when the content of dreams was in accordance with their beliefs and desires while awake. People were more likely to view a positive dream about a friend to be meaningful than a positive dream about someone they disliked, for example, and were more likely to view a negative dream about a person they disliked as meaningful than a negative dream about a person they liked. | |||
A hallucination, in the broadest sense of the word, is a ] in the absence of a ]. In a stricter sense, hallucinations are perceptions in a conscious and awake state, in the absence of external stimuli, and have qualities of real perception, in that they are vivid, substantial, and located in external objective space. The latter definition distinguishes hallucinations from the related phenomena of dreaming, which does not involve wakefulness. | |||
=== Other === | |||
Therapy for recurring ] (often associated with ]) can include imagining alternative scenarios that could begin at each step of the dream.<ref name="npr"/> | |||
=== Nightmare === | |||
==Other associated phenomena== | |||
{{Main|Nightmare}} | |||
] | |||
A nightmare is an unpleasant dream that can cause a strong ]al response from the mind, typically ] or ], but also ], ] and great ]. The dream may contain situations of danger, discomfort, psychological or physical terror. Sufferers usually awaken in a state of distress and may be unable to return to ] for a prolonged period of time.<ref>American Psychiatric Association (2000), Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th ed, TR, p. 631</ref> | |||
===Incorporation of reality=== | |||
During the night, many external stimuli may bombard the senses, but the brain often interprets the stimulus and makes it a part of a dream to ensure continued sleep.<ref>{{cite encyclopedia |year=1993 |title=Characteristics of Dreams |encyclopedia=Encyclopedia of Sleep and Dreaming |last=Antrobus |first=John}}</ref> Dream incorporation is a phenomenon whereby an actual sensation, such as environmental sounds, is incorporated into dreams, such as hearing a phone ringing in a dream while it is ringing in reality or dreaming of ] while ] the bed. The mind can, however, awaken an individual if they are in danger or if trained to respond to certain sounds, such as a baby crying. | |||
=== Night terror === | |||
The term "dream incorporation" is also used in research examining the degree to which preceding daytime events become elements of dreams. Recent studies suggest that events in the day immediately preceding, and those about a week before, have the most influence.<ref name="day-residue">{{cite web |author=Geneviève Alain, MPs |author2=Tore A. Nielsen, PhD |author3=Russell Powell, PhD |author4=Don Kuiken, PhD |url=http://www.asdreams.org/2003/abstracts/genevieve_alain.htm |title=Replication of the Day-residue and Dream-lag Effect |date= July 2003 |work=20th Annual International Conference of the Association for the Study of Dreams}}</ref> | |||
{{Main|Night terror}} | |||
A night terror, also known as a sleep terror or ''pavor nocturnus'', is a ] ] that predominantly affects children, causing feelings of terror or dread. Night terrors should not be confused with ]s, which are bad dreams that cause the feeling of horror or fear.<ref>{{cite book |last1=Hockenbury |first1=Don H. |last2=Hockenbury |first2=Sandra E. |title=Discovering psychology |date=2010 |publisher=Worth Publishers |location=New York |isbn=978-1-4292-1650-0 |edition=5th |page=157}}</ref> | |||
===Apparent precognition of real events=== | |||
{{Main|Precognition}} | |||
=== Déjà vu === | |||
According to surveys, it is common for people to feel their dreams are predicting subsequent life events.<ref name="hines"/> Psychologists have explained these experiences in terms of ], namely a selective memory for accurate predictions and distorted memory so that dreams are retrospectively fitted onto life experiences.<ref name="hines">{{cite book |last=Hines |first= Terence |title=Pseudoscience and the Paranormal |publisher=Prometheus Books |year=2003 |pages=78–81 |isbn=978-1-57392-979-0}}</ref> The multi-faceted nature of dreams makes it easy to find connections between dream content and real events.<ref>{{cite book |last=Gilovich |first=Thomas |title=How We Know What Isn't So: the fallibility of human reason in everyday life |publisher=Simon & Schuster |year=1991 |pages= 177–180 |isbn=978-0-02-911706-4}}</ref> | |||
In one experiment, subjects were asked to write down their dreams in a diary. This prevented the selective memory effect, and the dreams no longer seemed accurate about the future.<ref>{{cite book |last=Alcock |first=James E. |title=Parapsychology: Science or Magic?: a psychological perspective |publisher=Pergamon Press |location=Oxford |year=1981 |isbn=0-08-025773-9}} via {{cite book |last=Hines |first=Terence |title= Pseudoscience and the Paranormal |publisher=Prometheus Books |year=2003 |pages=78–81 |isbn=978-1-57392-979-0}}</ref> Another experiment gave subjects a fake diary of a student with apparently precognitive dreams. This diary described events from the person's life, as well as some predictive dreams and some non-predictive dreams. When subjects were asked to recall the dreams they had read, they remembered more of the successful predictions than unsuccessful ones.<ref>{{cite journal |last=Madey |first=Scott |author2=Thomas Gilovich |title=Effects of Temporal Focus on the Recall of Expectancy-Consistent and Expectancy-Inconsistent Information |journal=Journal of Personality and Social Psychology |volume=65 |issue=3 |year=1993 | doi = 10.1037/0022-3514.65.3.458 |pages=458–468 |pmid=8410650}} via {{cite book |last=Kida |first=Thomas |title=Don't Believe Everything You Think: The 6 Basic Mistakes We Make in Thinking |year=2006 |publisher=Prometheus Books |isbn=978-1-59102-408-8}}</ref> | |||
===Lucid dreaming=== | |||
{{Main|Lucid dreaming}} | |||
Lucid dreaming is the conscious perception of one's state while dreaming. In this state the dreamer may often have some degree of control over their own actions within the dream or even the characters and the environment of the dream. Dream control has been reported to improve with practiced deliberate lucid dreaming, but the ability to control aspects of the dream is not necessary for a dream to qualify as "lucid" — a lucid dream is any dream during which the dreamer knows they are dreaming.<ref> by ] at Psych Web.</ref> The occurrence of lucid dreaming has been scientifically verified.<ref name= Watanabe2003>{{cite journal |author=Watanabe, T. |year=2003 |title=Lucid Dreaming: Its Experimental Proof and Psychological Conditions |journal=J Int Soc Life Inf Sci |volume=21 |issue=1 |issn=1341-9226}}</ref> | |||
'']'' is a term sometimes used for those who lucidly dream. | |||
====Communication through lucid dreaming==== | |||
In 1975, psychologist Keith Hearne successfully recorded a communication from a dreamer experiencing a lucid dream. On April 12, 1975, after agreeing to move his eyes left and right upon becoming lucid, the subject and Hearne's co-author on the resulting article, Alan Worsley, successfully carried out this task.<ref name="Lucid dream communication">, Lucid Dream Communication</ref> | |||
Years later, psychophysiologist Stephen LaBerge conducted similar work including: | |||
*Using eye signals to map the subjective sense of time in dreams | |||
*Comparing the electrical activity of the brain while singing awake and while dreaming. | |||
*Studies comparing in-dream sex, arousal, and orgasm<ref>LaBerge, S. (2014). Lucid dreaming: Paradoxes of dreaming consciousness. In E. Cardeña, S. Lynn, S. Krippner (Eds.) , Varieties of anomalous experience: Examining the scientific evidence (2nd ed.) (pp. 145-173). Washington, DC US: American Psychological Association. {{DOI|10.1037/14258-006}}</ref> | |||
Communication between two dreamers has also been documented. The processes involved included ] monitoring, ocular signaling, incorporation of reality in the form of red light stimuli and a coordinating website. The website tracked when both dreamers were dreaming and sent the stimulus to one of the dreamers where it was incorporated into the dream. This dreamer, upon becoming lucid, signaled with eye movements; this was detected by the website whereupon the stimulus was sent to the second dreamer, invoking incorporation into this dream.<ref>{{Cite web|url=http://www.forbes.com/sites/parmyolson/2012/11/08/saying-hi-through-a-dream-how-the-internet-could-make-sleeping-more-social/#2eb99101590c|title=Saying 'Hi' Through A Dream: How The Internet Could Make Sleeping More Social|last=Olson|first=Parmy|website=Forbes|access-date=2016-04-24}}</ref> | |||
===Dreams of absent-minded transgression=== | |||
Dreams of absent-minded transgression (DAMT) are dreams wherein the dreamer absentmindedly performs an action that he or she has been trying to stop (one classic example is of a quitting smoker having dreams of lighting a cigarette). Subjects who have had DAMT have reported waking with intense feelings of ]. One study found a positive association between having these dreams and successfully stopping the behavior.<ref>{{cite journal |vauthors=Hajek P, Belcher M |title=Dream of absent-minded transgression: an empirical study of a cognitive withdrawal symptom |journal=J Abnorm Psychol |volume=100 |issue=4 |pages= 487–91 |year=1991 |pmid=1757662 |doi=10.1037/0021-843X.100.4.487}}</ref> | |||
===Recalling dreams=== | |||
The recall of dreams is extremely unreliable, though it is a skill that can be trained. Dreams can usually be recalled if a person is awakened while dreaming.<ref name="npr"/> Women tend to have more frequent dream recall than men.<ref name="npr">{{cite web|url=http://www.npr.org/templates/story/story.php?storyId=15778923 |title=The Science Behind Dreams and Nightmares |publisher=Npr.org |accessdate=April 4, 2013}}</ref> Dreams that are difficult to recall may be characterized by relatively little ], and factors such as ], ], and interference play a role in dream recall. Often, a dream may be recalled upon viewing or hearing a random trigger or stimulus. The ''salience hypothesis'' proposes that dream content that is salient, that is, novel, intense, or unusual, is more easily remembered. There is considerable evidence that vivid, intense, or unusual dream content is more frequently recalled.<ref name=Watson>{{cite journal|doi=10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00114-9|title=To dream, perchance to remember: Individual differences in dream recall|year=2003|last1=Watson|first1=David|journal=Personality and Individual Differences|volume=34|issue=7|pages=1271–1286}}</ref> A ] can be used to assist dream recall, for personal interest or ] purposes. | |||
For some people, sensations from the previous night's dreams are sometimes spontaneously experienced in falling asleep. However they are usually too slight and fleeting to allow dream recall. At least 95% of all dreams are not remembered. Certain brain chemicals necessary for converting short-term memories into long-term ones are suppressed during REM sleep. Unless a dream is particularly vivid and if one wakes during or immediately after it, the content of the dream is not remembered.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Hobson | first1 = J.A. | last2 = McCarly | first2 = R.W. | year = 1977 | title = The brain as a dream-state generator: An activation-synthesis hypothesis of the dream process | url = | journal = American Journal of Psychiatry | volume = 134 | issue = 12| pages = 1335–1348 | pmid = 21570 | doi=10.1176/ajp.134.12.1335}}</ref> | |||
Recording or reconstructing dreams may one day assist with dream recall or replace the need for recall. Using technologies such as ] (fMRI) and ] (EMG), researchers have been able to record basic dream imagery,<ref>{{Cite web|url=http://www.bbc.com/news/science-environment-22031074|title=Scientists 'read dreams' using brain scans - BBC News|website=BBC News|language=en-GB|access-date=2016-04-24}}</ref> dream speech activity<ref>{{Cite book|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=esv8GMVwHwEC|title=The Psychophysiology of Thinking: Studies of Covert Processes|last=Mcguigan|first=F.|date=2012-12-02|publisher=Elsevier|isbn=9780323147002|language=en}}</ref> and dream motor behavior (such as walking and hand movements).<ref>{{Cite book|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=n6YXNrpA8ZYC|title=States of Consciousness: Experimental Insights into Meditation, Waking, Sleep and Dreams|last=Cvetkovic|first=Dean|last2=Cosic|first2=Irena|date=2011-06-22|publisher=Springer Science & Business Media|isbn=9783642180477|language=en}}</ref> The ultimate goal of these combined technologies is to create a movie from a dream.<ref>{{Cite web|url=http://www.huffingtonpost.com/dreamscloud/can-we-turn-our-dreams-in_b_9152612.html|title=Can We Turn Our Dreams Into Watchable Movies?|date=2016-02-04|website=The Huffington Post|access-date=2016-04-24}}</ref> | |||
====Individual differences==== | |||
In line with the salience hypothesis, there is considerable evidence that people who have more vivid, intense or unusual dreams show better recall. There is evidence that continuity of consciousness is related to recall. Specifically, people who have vivid and unusual experiences during the day tend to have more memorable dream content and hence better dream recall. People who score high on measures of personality traits associated with creativity, imagination, and fantasy, such as ], ], ], ], and ], tend to show more frequent dream recall.<ref name= Watson/> There is also evidence for continuity between the bizarre aspects of dreaming and waking experience. That is, people who report more bizarre experiences during the day, such as people high in ] (psychosis proneness) have more frequent dream recall and also report more frequent ].<ref name= Watson/> | |||
===Déjà vu=== | |||
{{Main|Déjà vu}} | {{Main|Déjà vu}} | ||
One theory of déjà vu attributes the feeling of having previously seen or experienced something to having dreamed about a similar situation or place, and forgetting about it until one seems to be mysteriously reminded of the situation or the place while awake.<ref>{{cite book |title=The Dream Directory: The Comprehensive Guide to Analysis and Interpretation |last=Lohff |first=David C. |year=2004 |publisher=Running Press |isbn=0-7624-1962- |
One theory of déjà vu attributes the feeling of having previously seen or experienced something to having dreamed about a similar situation or place, and forgetting about it until one seems to be mysteriously reminded of the situation or the place while awake.<ref>{{cite book |title=The Dream Directory: The Comprehensive Guide to Analysis and Interpretation |last=Lohff |first=David C. |year=2004 |publisher=Running Press |isbn=978-0-7624-1962-3 |url=https://archive.org/details/davidclohffsdrea0000lohf}}</ref> | ||
===Daydreaming=== | |||
{{Main|Daydreaming}} | |||
A daydream is a visionary ], especially one of happy, pleasant thoughts, hopes or ambitions, imagined as coming to pass, and experienced while awake.<ref name="Klinger">Klinger, Eric (October 1987). '']''.</ref> There are many different types of daydreams, and there is no consistent definition amongst ].<ref name="Klinger"/> The general public also uses the term for a broad variety of experiences. Research by Harvard psychologist ] has found that people who experience vivid dream-like ]s reserve the word for these, whereas many other people refer to milder imagery, realistic future planning, review of past memories or just "spacing out"—i.e. one's mind going relatively blank—when they talk about "daydreaming."<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Barrett | first1 = D. L. | year = 1979 | title = The Hypnotic Dream: Its Content in Comparison to Nocturnal Dreams and Waking Fantasy | url = | journal = Journal of Abnormal Psychology | volume = 88 | issue = | pages = 584–591 | doi=10.1037/0021-843x.88.5.584}}</ref><ref>Barrett, D. L. Fantasizers and Dissociaters: Two types of High Hypnotizables, Two Imagery Styles. in R. Kusendorf, N. Spanos, & B. Wallace (Eds.) Hypnosis and Imagination, NY: Baywood, 1996; & Barrett, D. L. Dissociaters, Fantasizers, and their Relation to Hypnotizability in Barrett, D. L. (Ed.) Hypnosis and Hypnotherapy, (2 vol.): Vol. 1: History, theory and general research, Vol. 2: Psychotherapy research and applications, NY, NY: Praeger/Greenwood, 2010.</ref> | |||
While daydreaming has long been derided as a lazy, non-productive pastime, it is now commonly acknowledged that daydreaming can be constructive in some contexts.<ref>{{cite news |first=John |last=Tierney |authorlink=John Tierney (journalist) |title=Discovering the Virtues of a Wandering Mind |url=http://www.nytimes.com/2010/06/29/science/29tier.html?src=me&ref=general |newspaper=] |date=June 28, 2010}}</ref> There are numerous examples of people in ] or artistic careers, such as ], ]s and ]s, developing new ideas through daydreaming. Similarly, research ]s, ]s and ]s have developed new ideas by daydreaming about their subject areas. | |||
===Hallucination=== | |||
{{Main|Hallucination}} | |||
A hallucination, in the broadest sense of the word, is a ] in the absence of a ]. In a stricter sense, hallucinations are perceptions in a conscious and awake state, in the absence of external stimuli, and have qualities of real perception, in that they are vivid, substantial, and located in external objective space. The latter definition distinguishes hallucinations from the related phenomena of dreaming, which does not involve wakefulness. | |||
===Nightmare=== | |||
{{Main|Nightmare}} | |||
A nightmare is an unpleasant dream that can cause a strong negative emotional response from the mind, typically ] and/or ], but also ], ] and great ]. The dream may contain situations of danger, discomfort, psychological or physical terror. Sufferers usually awaken in a state of distress and may be unable to return to ] for a prolonged period of time.<ref>American Psychiatric Association (2000), Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th ed, TR, p. 631</ref> | |||
===Night terror=== | |||
{{Main|Night terror}} | |||
=== Melatonin === | |||
A night terror, also known as a sleep terror or ''pavor nocturnus'', is a ] ] that predominantly affects children, causing feelings of terror or dread. Night terrors should not be confused with ]s, which are bad dreams that cause the feeling of horror or fear. | |||
{{Main|Melatonin}} | |||
Melatonin is a natural hormone secreted by the brain's ], inducing nocturnal behaviors in animals and sleep in humans during nighttime. Chemically isolated in 1958, melatonin has been marketed as a ] since the 1990s and is currently sold in the United States as an over-the-counter product requiring no prescription. Anecdotal reports and formal research studies over the past few decades have established a link between melatonin supplementation and more vivid dreams.<ref>{{cite journal |title=Effects of melatonin on dream bizarreness among male and female college students.|last=Kahan |first=Tracey L. |year=2000 |journal=Sleep and Hypnosis |volume=2 |issue=2 |pages=74–83|url=https://scholarcommons.scu.edu/psych/102/}}</ref> | |||
==See also== | |||
{{Div col|cols=2}} | |||
== See also == | |||
* ] | |||
{{Portal|Psychology}} | |||
* ] | |||
<!-- Please keep entries in alphabetical order & add a short description ] --> | |||
* ] | |||
{{div col|colwidth=12em}} | |||
* ] | * ] | ||
* ] | |||
* ] | |||
* ] | * ] | ||
* ] | * ] | ||
* ] | * ] | ||
* ] | |||
* ] | * ] | ||
* ] | |||
* ] | * ] | ||
* ] | * ] | ||
* '']'' | |||
* ] | * ] | ||
* ], a Sumerian dream |
* ], a Sumerian dream demoness | ||
* ] | * ] | ||
* ] | |||
* ] | * ] | ||
* ] | * ] | ||
* ] | * ] | ||
* ] | |||
* ] | * ] | ||
* ] | * ] | ||
* ] | * ] | ||
* ] | * ] | ||
{{ |
{{div col end}} | ||
<!-- please keep entries in alphabetical order --> | |||
==References== | == References == | ||
{{Reflist |
{{Reflist}} | ||
==Further reading== | == Further reading == | ||
* '']'' – journal published by the ] | |||
* {{cite book |author=Freud, Sigmund |authorlink=Sigmund Freud |title=The interpretation of dreams |publisher=Modern Library |location=New York |year=1994 |isbn=0-679-60121-X}} | |||
* {{cite book |author=Jung, Carl | |
* {{cite book |author=Jung, Carl |author-link=Carl Jung |title=The Practice of Psychotherapy. ''"The Practical Use of Dream-analysis"'' |location=New York |year=1934 |pages=139– |isbn=978-0-7100-1645-4 |publisher= Routledge & Kegan Paul }} | ||
* {{cite book |author=Jung, Carl |
* {{cite book |author=Jung, Carl |title=Dreams (Routledge Classics) |publisher=Routledge |location=New York |year=2002 |isbn=978-0-415-26740-3}} | ||
* Harris, William V. |
* Harris, William V. (2009) ''Dreams and Еxperience in Classical Antiquity''. Cambridge, MA & London: Harvard University Press. {{isbn|978-0-674-03297-2}} | ||
==External links== | == External links == | ||
{{Commons category| |
{{Commons category|Dreams}} | ||
{{wikiquote}} | {{wikiquote}} | ||
* {{In Our Time|Dreams|p004y23x|Dreams}} | * {{In Our Time|Dreams|p004y23x|Dreams}} | ||
* - research on the cultural and literary history of the dream | |||
* - an online sleep research database documenting the physiological effects of dreams through biofeedback. | |||
* | * | ||
* | * | ||
* {{Cite news |first=Jay |last=Dixit |title=Dreams: Night School |url=https://www.psychologytoday.com/us/articles/200711/dreams-night-school |magazine=] |date=November 2007 |access-date=December 1, 2018}} | |||
*{{DMOZ|Science/Social_Sciences/Psychology/Dreams/}} | |||
* A long-running USENET forum wherein readers post and analyze dreams | |||
* {{Cite journal|url=http://www.psychologytoday.com/articles/index.php?term=20071029-000003 |first=Jay |last=Dixit |title=Dreams: Night School |journal=Psychology Today |year=2007 |deadurl=yes |archiveurl=https://web.archive.org/web/20090112220142/http://www.psychologytoday.com:80/articles/index.php?term=20071029-000003 |archivedate=January 12, 2009 }} | |||
* – online sleep research database documenting physiological effects of dreams through biofeedback | |||
* A long-running USENET forum wherein readers post and analyze each other's dreams. | |||
{{Dreaming}} | {{Dreaming}} | ||
{{SleepSeries2}} | {{SleepSeries2}} | ||
{{Authority control}} | {{Authority control}} | ||
] | ] | ||
] | ] | ||
] | |||
] | ] |
Latest revision as of 21:31, 9 January 2025
Event occurring in the mind while sleeping For other uses, see Dream (disambiguation) and Dreams (disambiguation).
A dream is a succession of images, ideas, emotions, and sensations that usually occur involuntarily in the mind during certain stages of sleep. Humans spend about two hours dreaming per night, and each dream lasts around 5–20 minutes, although the dreamer may perceive the dream as being much longer than this.
The content and function of dreams have been topics of scientific, philosophical and religious interest throughout recorded history. Dream interpretation, practiced by the Babylonians in the third millennium BCE and even earlier by the ancient Sumerians, figures prominently in religious texts in several traditions, and has played a lead role in psychotherapy. The scientific study of dreams is called oneirology. Most modern dream study focuses on the neurophysiology of dreams and on proposing and testing hypotheses regarding dream function. It is not known where in the brain dreams originate, if there is a single origin for dreams or if multiple regions of the brain are involved, or what the purpose of dreaming is for the body or mind.
The human dream experience and what to make of it has undergone sizable shifts over the course of history. Long ago, according to writings from Mesopotamia and Ancient Egypt, dreams dictated post-dream behaviors to an extent that was sharply reduced in later millennia. These ancient writings about dreams highlight visitation dreams, where a dream figure, usually a deity or a prominent forebear, commands the dreamer to take specific actions, and which may predict future events. Framing the dream experience varies across cultures as well as through time.
Dreaming and sleep are intertwined. Dreams occur mainly in the rapid-eye movement (REM) stage of sleep—when brain activity is high and resembles that of being awake. Because REM sleep is detectable in many species, and because research suggests that all mammals experience REM, linking dreams to REM sleep has led to conjectures that animals dream. However, humans dream during non-REM sleep, also, and not all REM awakenings elicit dream reports. To be studied, a dream must first be reduced to a verbal report, which is an account of the subject's memory of the dream, not the subject's dream experience itself. So, dreaming by non-humans is currently unprovable, as is dreaming by human fetuses and pre-verbal infants.
Subjective experience and content
Further information: OneiromancyPreserved writings from early Mediterranean civilizations indicate a relatively abrupt change in subjective dream experience between Bronze Age antiquity and the beginnings of the classical era.
In visitation dreams reported in ancient writings, dreamers were largely passive in their dreams, and visual content served primarily to frame authoritative auditory messaging. Gudea, the king of the Sumerian city-state of Lagash (reigned c. 2144–2124 BCE), rebuilt the temple of Ningirsu as the result of a dream in which he was told to do so. After antiquity, the passive hearing of visitation dreams largely gave way to visualized narratives in which the dreamer becomes a character who actively participates.
From the 1940s to 1985, Calvin S. Hall collected more than 50,000 dream reports at Western Reserve University. In 1966, Hall and Robert Van de Castle published The Content Analysis of Dreams, in which they outlined a coding system to study 1,000 dream reports from college students. Results indicated that participants from varying parts of the world demonstrated similarity in their dream content. The only residue of antiquity's authoritative dream figure in the Hall and Van de Castle listing of dream characters is the inclusion of God in the category of prominent persons. Hall's complete dream reports were made publicly available in the mid-1990s by his protégé William Domhoff. More recent studies of dream reports, while providing more detail, continue to cite the Hall study favorably.
In the Hall study, the most common emotion experienced in dreams was anxiety. Other emotions included abandonment, anger, fear, joy, and happiness. Negative emotions were much more common than positive ones. The Hall data analysis showed that sexual dreams occur no more than 10% of the time and are more prevalent in young to mid-teens. Another study showed that 8% of both men's and women's dreams have sexual content. In some cases, sexual dreams may result in orgasms or nocturnal emissions. These are colloquially known as "wet dreams".
The visual nature of dreams is generally highly phantasmagoric; that is, different locations and objects continuously blend into each other. The visuals (including locations, people, and objects) are generally reflective of a person's memories and experiences, but conversation can take on highly exaggerated and bizarre forms. Some dreams may even tell elaborate stories wherein the dreamer enters entirely new, complex worlds and awakes with ideas, thoughts and feelings never experienced prior to the dream.
People who are blind from birth do not have visual dreams. Their dream contents are related to other senses, such as hearing, touch, smell, and taste, whichever are present since birth.
Neurophysiology
Main article: Cognitive neuroscience of dreams Further information: Neuroscience of sleepDream study is popular with scientists exploring the mind–brain problem. Some "propose to reduce aspects of dream phenomenology to neurobiology." But current science cannot specify dream physiology in detail. Protocols in most nations restrict human brain research to non-invasive procedures. In the United States, invasive brain procedures with a human subject are allowed only when these are deemed necessary in surgical treatment to address medical needs of the same human subject. Non-invasive measures of brain activity like electroencephalogram (EEG) voltage averaging or cerebral blood flow cannot identify small but influential neuronal populations. Also, fMRI signals are too slow to explain how brains compute in real time.
Scientists researching some brain functions can work around current restrictions by examining animal subjects. As stated by the Society for Neuroscience, "Because no adequate alternatives exist, much of this research must be done on animal subjects." However, since animal dreaming can be only inferred, not confirmed, animal studies yield no hard facts to illuminate the neurophysiology of dreams. Examining human subjects with brain lesions can provide clues, but the lesion method cannot discriminate between the effects of destruction and disconnection and cannot target specific neuronal groups in heterogeneous regions like the brain stem.
Generation
Denied precision tools and obliged to depend on imaging, much dream research has succumbed to the law of the instrument. Studies detect an increase of blood flow in a specific brain region and then credit that region with a role in generating dreams. But pooling study results has led to the newer conclusion that dreaming involves large numbers of regions and pathways, which likely are different for different dream events.
Image creation in the brain involves significant neural activity downstream from eye intake, and it is theorized that "the visual imagery of dreams is produced by activation during sleep of the same structures that generate complex visual imagery in waking perception."
Dreams present a running narrative rather than exclusively visual imagery. Following their work with split-brain subjects, Gazzaniga and LeDoux postulated, without attempting to specify the neural mechanisms, a "left-brain interpreter" that seeks to create a plausible narrative from whatever electro-chemical signals reach the brain's left hemisphere. Sleep research has determined that some brain regions fully active during waking are, during REM sleep, activated only in a partial or fragmentary way. Drawing on this knowledge, textbook author James W. Kalat explains, " dream represents the brain's effort to make sense of sparse and distorted information.... The cortex combines this haphazard input with whatever other activity was already occurring and does its best to synthesize a story that makes sense of the information." Neuroscientist Indre Viskontas is even more blunt, calling often bizarre dream content "just the result of your interpreter trying to create a story out of random neural signaling."
Theories on function
Main article: Oneirology Further information: Rapid eye movement sleepFor many humans across multiple eras and cultures, dreams are believed to have functioned as revealers of truths sourced during sleep from gods or other external entities. Ancient Egyptians believed that dreams were the best way to receive divine revelation, and thus they would induce (or "incubate") dreams. They went to sanctuaries and slept on special "dream beds" in hope of receiving advice, comfort, or healing from the gods. From a Darwinian perspective dreams would have to fulfill some kind of biological requirement, provide some benefit for natural selection to take place, or at least have no negative impact on fitness. Robert (1886), a physician from Hamburg, was the first who suggested that dreams are a need and that they have the function to erase (a) sensory impressions that were not fully worked up, and (b) ideas that were not fully developed during the day. In dreams, incomplete material is either removed (suppressed) or deepened and included into memory. Freud, whose dream studies focused on interpreting dreams, not explaining how or why humans dream, disputed Robert's hypothesis and proposed that dreams preserve sleep by representing as fulfilled those wishes that otherwise would awaken the dreamer. Freud wrote that dreams "serve the purpose of prolonging sleep instead of waking up. Dreams are the GUARDIANS of sleep and not its disturbers."
A turning point in theorizing about dream function came in 1953, when Science published the Aserinsky and Kleitman paper establishing REM sleep as a distinct phase of sleep and linking dreams to REM sleep. Until and even after publication of the Solms 2000 paper that certified the separability of REM sleep and dream phenomena, many studies purporting to uncover the function of dreams have in fact been studying not dreams but measurable REM sleep.
Theories of dream function since the identification of REM sleep include:
Hobson's and McCarley's 1977 activation-synthesis hypothesis, which proposed "a functional role for dreaming sleep in promoting some aspect of the learning process...." In 2010 a Harvard study was published showing experimental evidence that dreams were correlated with improved learning.
Crick's and Mitchison's 1983 "reverse learning" theory, which states that dreams are like the cleaning-up operations of computers when they are offline, removing (suppressing) parasitic nodes and other "junk" from the mind during sleep.
Hartmann's 1995 proposal that dreams serve a "quasi-therapeutic" function, enabling the dreamer to process trauma in a safe place.
Revonsuo's 2000 threat simulation hypothesis, whose premise is that during much of human evolution, physical and interpersonal threats were serious, giving reproductive advantage to those who survived them. Dreaming aided survival by replicating these threats and providing the dreamer with practice in dealing with them. In 2015, Revonsuo proposed social simulation theory, which describes dreams as a simulation for training social skills and bonds.
Eagleman's and Vaughn's 2021 defensive activation theory, which says that, given the brain's neuroplasticity, dreams evolved as a visual hallucinatory activity during sleep's extended periods of darkness, busying the occipital lobe and thereby protecting it from possible appropriation by other, non-vision, sense operations.
Erik Hoel proposes, based on artificial neural networks, that dreams prevent overfitting to past experiences; that is, they enable the dreamer to learn from novel situations.
Religious and other cultural contexts
Dreams figure prominently in major world religions. The dream experience for early humans, according to one interpretation, gave rise to the notion of a human "soul", a central element in much religious thought. J. W. Dunne wrote:
But there can be no reasonable doubt that the idea of a soul must have first arisen in the mind of primitive man as a result of observation of his dreams. Ignorant as he was, he could have come to no other conclusion but that, in dreams, he left his sleeping body in one universe and went wandering off into another. It is considered that, but for that savage, the idea of such a thing as a 'soul' would never have even occurred to mankind....
Hindu
In the Mandukya Upanishad, part of the Veda scriptures of Indian Hinduism, a dream is one of three states that the soul experiences during its lifetime, the other two states being the waking state and the sleep state. The earliest Upanishads, written before 300 BCE, emphasize two meanings of dreams. The first says that dreams are merely expressions of inner desires. The second is the belief of the soul leaving the body and being guided until awakened.
Abrahamic
In Judaism, dreams are considered part of the experience of the world that can be interpreted and from which lessons can be garnered. It is discussed in the Talmud, Tractate Berachot 55–60.
The ancient Hebrews connected their dreams heavily with their religion, though the Hebrews were monotheistic and believed that dreams were the voice of one God alone. Hebrews also differentiated between good dreams (from God) and bad dreams (from evil spirits). The Hebrews, like many other ancient cultures, incubated dreams in order to receive a divine revelation. For example, the Hebrew prophet Samuel would "lie down and sleep in the temple at Shiloh before the Ark and receive the word of the Lord", and Joseph interpreted a Pharaoh's dream of seven lean cows swallowing seven fat cows as meaning the subsequent seven years would be bountiful, followed by seven years of famine. Most of the dreams in the Bible are in the Book of Genesis.
Christians mostly shared the beliefs of the Hebrews and thought that dreams were of a supernatural character because the Old Testament includes frequent stories of dreams with divine inspiration. The most famous of these dream stories was Jacob's dream of a ladder that stretches from Earth to Heaven. Many Christians preach that God can speak to people through their dreams. The famous glossary, the Somniale Danielis, written in the name of Daniel, attempted to teach Christian populations to interpret their dreams.
Iain R. Edgar has researched the role of dreams in Islam. He has argued that dreams play an important role in the history of Islam and the lives of Muslims, since dream interpretation is the only way that Muslims can receive revelations from God since the death of the last prophet, Muhammad. According to Edgar, Islam classifies three types of dreams. Firstly, there is the true dream (al-ru’ya), then the false dream, which may come from the devil (shaytan), and finally, the meaningless everyday dream (hulm). This last dream could be brought forth by the dreamer's ego or base appetite based on what they experienced in the real world. The true dream is often indicated by Islam's hadith tradition. In one narration by Aisha, the wife of the Prophet, it is said that the Prophet's dreams would come true like the ocean's waves. Just as in its predecessors, the Quran also recounts the story of Joseph and his unique ability to interpret dreams.
In both Christianity and Islam dreams feature in conversion stories. According to ancient authors, Constantine the Great started his conversion to Christianity because he had a dream which prophesied that he would win the battle of the Milvian Bridge if he adopted the Chi-Rho as his battle standard."
Buddhist
In Buddhism, ideas about dreams are similar to the classical and folk traditions in South Asia. The same dream is sometimes experienced by multiple people, as in the case of the Buddha-to-be, before he is leaving his home. It is described in the Mahāvastu that several of the Buddha's relatives had premonitory dreams preceding this. Some dreams are also seen to transcend time: the Buddha-to-be has certain dreams that are the same as those of previous Buddhas, the Lalitavistara states. In Buddhist literature, dreams often function as a "signpost" motif to mark certain stages in the life of the main character.
Buddhist views about dreams are expressed in the Pāli Commentaries and the Milinda Pañhā.
Other
In Chinese history, people wrote of two vital aspects of the soul of which one is freed from the body during slumber to journey in a dream realm, while the other remained in the body. This belief and dream interpretation had been questioned since early times, such as by the philosopher Wang Chong (27–97 CE).
The Babylonians and Assyrians divided dreams into "good," which were sent by the gods, and "bad," sent by demons. A surviving collection of dream omens entitled Iškar Zaqīqu records various dream scenarios as well as prognostications of what will happen to the person who experiences each dream, apparently based on previous cases. Some list different possible outcomes, based on occasions in which people experienced similar dreams with different results. The Greeks shared their beliefs with the Egyptians on how to interpret good and bad dreams, and the idea of incubating dreams. Morpheus, the Greek god of dreams, also sent warnings and prophecies to those who slept at shrines and temples. The earliest Greek beliefs about dreams were that their gods physically visited the dreamers, where they entered through a keyhole, exiting the same way after the divine message was given.
Antiphon wrote the first known Greek book on dreams in the 5th century BCE. In that century, other cultures influenced Greeks to develop the belief that souls left the sleeping body. The father of modern medicine, Hippocrates (460–375 BCE), thought dreams could analyze illness and predict diseases. For instance, a dream of a dim star high in the night sky indicated problems in the head region, while low in the night sky indicated bowel issues. Galen (129–216 AD) believed the same thing. Greek philosopher Plato (427–347 BCE) wrote that people harbor secret, repressed desires, such as incest, murder, adultery, and conquest, which build up during the day and run rampant during the night in dreams. Plato's student, Aristotle (384–322 BCE), believed dreams were caused by processing incomplete physiological activity during sleep, such as eyes trying to see while the sleeper's eyelids were closed. Marcus Tullius Cicero, for his part, believed that all dreams are produced by thoughts and conversations a dreamer had during the preceding days. Cicero's Somnium Scipionis described a lengthy dream vision, which in turn was commented on by Macrobius in his Commentarii in Somnium Scipionis.
Herodotus in his The Histories, writes "The visions that occur to us in dreams are, more often than not, the things we have been concerned about during the day."
The Dreaming is a common term within the animist creation narrative of indigenous Australians for a personal, or group, creation and for what may be understood as the "timeless time" of formative creation and perpetual creating.
Some Indigenous American tribes and Mexican populations believe that dreams are a way of visiting and having contact with their ancestors. Some Native American tribes have used vision quests as a rite of passage, fasting and praying until an anticipated guiding dream was received, to be shared with the rest of the tribe upon their return.
Interpretation
Main article: Dream interpretation Further information: Psychoanalysis and PrecognitionBeginning in the late 19th century, Austrian neurologist Sigmund Freud, founder of psychoanalysis, theorized that dreams reflect the dreamer's unconscious mind and specifically that dream content is shaped by unconscious wish fulfillment. He argued that important unconscious desires often relate to early childhood memories and experiences. Carl Jung and others expanded on Freud's idea that dream content reflects the dreamer's unconscious desires.
Dream interpretation can be a result of subjective ideas and experiences. One study found that most people believe that "their dreams reveal meaningful hidden truths". The researchers surveyed students in the United States, South Korea, and India, and found that 74% of Indians, 65% of South Koreans and 56% of Americans believed their dream content provided them with meaningful insight into their unconscious beliefs and desires. This Freudian view of dreaming was believed significantly more than theories of dreaming that attribute dream content to memory consolidation, problem-solving, or as a byproduct of unrelated brain activity. The same study found that people attribute more importance to dream content than to similar thought content that occurs while they are awake. Americans were more likely to report that they would intentionally miss their flight if they dreamt of their plane crashing than if they thought of their plane crashing the night before flying (while awake), and that they would be as likely to miss their flight if they dreamt of their plane crashing the night before their flight as if there was an actual plane crash on the route they intended to take. Participants in the study were more likely to perceive dreams to be meaningful when the content of dreams was in accordance with their beliefs and desires while awake. They were more likely to view a positive dream about a friend to be meaningful than a positive dream about someone they disliked, for example, and were more likely to view a negative dream about a person they disliked as meaningful than a negative dream about a person they liked.
According to surveys, it is common for people to feel their dreams are predicting subsequent life events. Psychologists have explained these experiences in terms of memory biases, namely a selective memory for accurate predictions and distorted memory so that dreams are retrospectively fitted onto life experiences. The multi-faceted nature of dreams makes it easy to find connections between dream content and real events. The term "veridical dream" has been used to indicate dreams that reveal or contain truths not yet known to the dreamer, whether future events or secrets.
In one experiment, subjects were asked to write down their dreams in a diary. This prevented the selective memory effect, and the dreams no longer seemed accurate about the future. Another experiment gave subjects a fake diary of a student with apparently precognitive dreams. This diary described events from the person's life, as well as some predictive dreams and some non-predictive dreams. When subjects were asked to recall the dreams they had read, they remembered more of the successful predictions than unsuccessful ones.
Images and literature
Main article: Dream art Further information: Dream world (plot device)Graphic artists, writers and filmmakers all have found dreams to offer a rich vein for creative expression. In the West, artists' depictions of dreams in Renaissance and Baroque art often were related to Biblical narrative. Especially preferred by visual artists were the Jacob's Ladder dream in Genesis and St. Joseph's dreams in the Gospel according to Matthew.
- Nicolas Dipre. Le songe de Jacob. c. 1500 Avignon, Petit Palais.
- José de Ribera (1591–1652). El sueño de Jacob, from Prado in Google Earth
- Raphael. Jacob's Dream (1518)
- Rembrandt. Dream of Joseph (1645)
- Anton Raphael Mengs. Traum des Hl. Joseph (1773 or 1774)
Many later graphic artists have depicted dreams, including Japanese woodblock artist Hokusai (1760–1849) and Western European painters Rousseau (1844–1910), Picasso (1881–1973), and Dalí (1904–1989).
In literature, dream frames were frequently used in medieval allegory to justify the narrative; The Book of the Duchess and The Vision Concerning Piers Plowman are two such dream visions. Even before them, in antiquity, the same device had been used by Cicero and Lucian of Samosata.
Dreams have also featured in fantasy and speculative fiction since the 19th century. One of the best-known dream worlds is Wonderland from Lewis Carroll's Alice's Adventures in Wonderland, as well as Looking-Glass Land from its sequel, Through the Looking-Glass. Unlike many dream worlds, Carroll's logic is like that of actual dreams, with transitions and flexible causality.
Other fictional dream worlds include the Dreamlands of H. P. Lovecraft's Dream Cycle and The Neverending Story's world of Fantastica, which includes places like the Desert of Lost Dreams, the Sea of Possibilities and the Swamps of Sadness. Dreamworlds, shared hallucinations and other alternate realities feature in a number of works by Philip K. Dick, such as The Three Stigmata of Palmer Eldritch and Ubik. Similar themes were explored by Jorge Luis Borges, for instance in The Circular Ruins.
Modern popular culture often conceives of dreams, as did Freud, as expressions of the dreamer's deepest fears and desires. In speculative fiction, the line between dreams and reality may be blurred even more in service to the story. Dreams may be psychically invaded or manipulated (Dreamscape, 1984; the Nightmare on Elm Street films, 1984–2010; Inception, 2010) or even come literally true (as in The Lathe of Heaven, 1971).
Lucidity
Main article: Lucid dreamLucid dreaming is the conscious perception of one's state while dreaming. In this state the dreamer may often have some degree of control over their own actions within the dream or even the characters and the environment of the dream. Dream control has been reported to improve with practiced deliberate lucid dreaming, but the ability to control aspects of the dream is not necessary for a dream to qualify as "lucid"—a lucid dream is any dream during which the dreamer knows they are dreaming. The occurrence of lucid dreaming has been scientifically verified.
"Oneironaut" is a term sometimes used for those who lucidly dream.
In 1975, psychologist Keith Hearne successfully recorded a communication from a dreamer experiencing a lucid dream. On April 12, 1975, after agreeing to move his eyes left and right upon becoming lucid, the subject and Hearne's co-author on the resulting article, Alan Worsley, successfully carried out this task. Years later, psychophysiologist Stephen LaBerge conducted similar work including:
- Using eye signals to map the subjective sense of time in dreams.
- Comparing the electrical activity of the brain while singing awake and while dreaming.
- Studies comparing in-dream sex, arousal, and orgasm.
Communication between two dreamers has also been documented. The processes involved included EEG monitoring, ocular signaling, incorporation of reality in the form of red light stimuli and a coordinating website. The website tracked when both dreamers were dreaming and sent the stimulus to one of the dreamers where it was incorporated into the dream. This dreamer, upon becoming lucid, signaled with eye movements; this was detected by the website whereupon the stimulus was sent to the second dreamer, invoking incorporation into that dreamer's dream.
Recollection
Further information: Dream diaryThe recollection of dreams is extremely unreliable, though it is a skill that can be trained. Dreams can usually be recalled if a person is awakened while dreaming. Women tend to have more frequent dream recall than men. Dreams that are difficult to recall may be characterized by relatively little affect, and factors such as salience, arousal, and interference play a role in dream recall. Often, a dream may be recalled upon viewing or hearing a random trigger or stimulus. The salience hypothesis proposes that dream content that is salient, that is, novel, intense, or unusual, is more easily remembered. There is considerable evidence that vivid, intense, or unusual dream content is more frequently recalled. A dream journal can be used to assist dream recall, for personal interest or psychotherapy purposes.
Adults report remembering around two dreams per week, on average. Unless a dream is particularly vivid and if one wakes during or immediately after it, the content of the dream is typically not remembered.
In line with the salience hypothesis, there is considerable evidence that people who have more vivid, intense or unusual dreams show better recall. There is evidence that continuity of consciousness is related to recall. Specifically, people who have vivid and unusual experiences during the day tend to have more memorable dream content and hence better dream recall. People who score high on measures of personality traits associated with creativity, imagination, and fantasy, such as openness to experience, daydreaming, fantasy proneness, absorption, and hypnotic susceptibility, tend to show more frequent dream recall. There is also evidence for continuity between the bizarre aspects of dreaming and waking experience. That is, people who report more bizarre experiences during the day, such as people high in schizotypy (psychosis proneness), have more frequent dream recall and also report more frequent nightmares.
Dream-recording machine
Recording or reconstructing dreams may one day assist with dream recall. Using the permitted non-invasive technologies, functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and electromyography (EMG), researchers have been able to identify basic dream imagery, dream speech activity and dream motor behavior (such as walking and hand movements).
Miscellany
Illusion of reality
Main article: Dream argumentSome philosophers have proposed that what we think of as the "real world" could be or is an illusion (an idea known as the skeptical hypothesis about ontology). The first recorded mention of the idea was in the 4th century BCE by Zhuangzi, and in Eastern philosophy, the problem has been named the "Zhuangzi Paradox."
He who dreams of drinking wine may weep when morning comes; he who dreams of weeping may in the morning go off to hunt. While he is dreaming he does not know it is a dream, and in his dream he may even try to interpret a dream. Only after he wakes does he know it was a dream. And someday there will be a great awakening when we know that this is all a great dream. Yet the stupid believe they are awake, busily and brightly assuming they understand things, calling this man ruler, that one herdsman—how dense! Confucius and you are both dreaming! And when I say you are dreaming, I am dreaming, too. Words like these will be labeled the Supreme Swindle. Yet, after ten thousand generations, a great sage may appear who will know their meaning, and it will still be as though he appeared with astonishing speed.
The idea also is discussed in Hindu and Buddhist writings. It was formally introduced to Western philosophy by Descartes in the 17th century in his Meditations on First Philosophy.
Absent-minded transgression
Dreams of absent-minded transgression (DAMT) are dreams wherein the dreamer absent-mindedly performs an action that he or she has been trying to stop (one classic example is of a quitting smoker having dreams of lighting a cigarette). Subjects who have had DAMT have reported waking with intense feelings of guilt. One study found a positive association between having these dreams and successfully stopping the behavior.
Non-REM dreams
Hypnogogic and hypnopompic dreams, dreamlike states shortly after falling asleep and shortly before awakening, and dreams during stage 2 of NREM-sleep, also occur, but are shorter than REM-dreams.
Daydreams
Main article: DaydreamA daydream is a visionary fantasy, especially one of happy, pleasant thoughts, hopes or ambitions, imagined as coming to pass, and experienced while awake. There are many different types of daydreams, and there is no consistent definition amongst psychologists. The general public also uses the term for a broad variety of experiences. Research by Harvard psychologist Deirdre Barrett has found that people who experience vivid dreamlike mental images reserve the word for these, whereas many other people refer to milder imagery, realistic future planning, review of memories or just "spacing out"—i.e. one's mind going relatively blank—when they talk about "daydreaming".
While daydreaming has long been derided as a lazy, non-productive pastime, it is now commonly acknowledged that daydreaming can be constructive in some contexts. There are numerous examples of people in creative or artistic careers, such as composers, novelists and filmmakers, developing new ideas through daydreaming. Similarly, research scientists, mathematicians and physicists have developed new ideas by daydreaming about their subject areas.
Hallucination
Main article: HallucinationA hallucination, in the broadest sense of the word, is a perception in the absence of a stimulus. In a stricter sense, hallucinations are perceptions in a conscious and awake state, in the absence of external stimuli, and have qualities of real perception, in that they are vivid, substantial, and located in external objective space. The latter definition distinguishes hallucinations from the related phenomena of dreaming, which does not involve wakefulness.
Nightmare
Main article: NightmareA nightmare is an unpleasant dream that can cause a strong negative emotional response from the mind, typically fear or horror, but also despair, anxiety and great sadness. The dream may contain situations of danger, discomfort, psychological or physical terror. Sufferers usually awaken in a state of distress and may be unable to return to sleep for a prolonged period of time.
Night terror
Main article: Night terrorA night terror, also known as a sleep terror or pavor nocturnus, is a parasomnia disorder that predominantly affects children, causing feelings of terror or dread. Night terrors should not be confused with nightmares, which are bad dreams that cause the feeling of horror or fear.
Déjà vu
Main article: Déjà vuOne theory of déjà vu attributes the feeling of having previously seen or experienced something to having dreamed about a similar situation or place, and forgetting about it until one seems to be mysteriously reminded of the situation or the place while awake.
Melatonin
Main article: MelatoninMelatonin is a natural hormone secreted by the brain's pineal gland, inducing nocturnal behaviors in animals and sleep in humans during nighttime. Chemically isolated in 1958, melatonin has been marketed as a sleep aid since the 1990s and is currently sold in the United States as an over-the-counter product requiring no prescription. Anecdotal reports and formal research studies over the past few decades have established a link between melatonin supplementation and more vivid dreams.
See also
- Dream dictionary
- Dream incubation
- Dream of Macsen Wledig
- Dream pop
- Dream sequence
- Dream yoga
- Dreamcatcher
- Dreams in analytical psychology
- Dreamwork
- False awakening
- Hatsuyume
- Incubus
- Lilith, a Sumerian dream demoness
- List of dream diaries
- Mare (folklore)
- Mabinogion
- Recurring dream
- Sleep in animals
- Sleep paralysis
- Spirit spouse
- Succubus
- Works based on dreams
References
- "Dream". The American Heritage Dictionary of the English Language, Fourth Edition. 2000. Retrieved 7 May 2009.
- "Brain Basics: Understanding Sleep". National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke. 2006. Archived from the original on 11 October 2007. Retrieved 16 December 2007.
- Lee Ann Obringer (2006). How Dream Works. Archived from the original on 18 April 2006. Retrieved 4 May 2006.
- Krippner, Stanley; Bogzaran, Fariba; Carvalho, Andre Percia de (2002). Extraordinary Dreams and How To Work with Them. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press. p. 9. ISBN 0-7914-5257-3.
Clay tablets have been found, dating to about 2500 B.C.E., that contain interpretive material for Babylonian and Assyrian dreamers.
- Seligman, K (1948). Magic, Supernaturalism and Religion. New York: Random House.
- ^ Black, Jeremy; Green, Anthony (1992). Gods, Demons and Symbols of Ancient Mesopotamia: An Illustrated Dictionary. Austin: University of Texas Press. pp. 71–72, 89–90. ISBN 0714117056.
- ^ Freud, Sigmund (1965). James Strachey (ed.). The Interpretation of Dreams. Translated by James Strachey. New York: Avon.
- Schredl, Michael; Bohusch, Claudia; Kahl, Johanna; Mader, Andrea; Somesan, Alexandra (2000). "The Use of Dreams in Psychotherapy". The Journal of Psychotherapy Practice and Research. 9 (2): 81–87.
- Kavanau, J.L. (2000). "Sleep, memory maintenance, and mental disorders". Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences. 12 (2): 199–208. doi:10.1176/jnp.12.2.199. ISSN 0895-0172. PMID 11001598.
- ^ Dodds, E. R. (1951). The Greeks and the Irrational. Berkeley: University of California Press. p. 105.
The Greeks never spoke as we do of having a dream, but always of seeing a dream....
- Packer, Sharon (2002). Dreams in Myth, Medicine, and Movies. Westport, CT: Praeger Publishers. p. 85. ISBN 0-275-97243-7.
…any more ancient cultures think that dreams are imposed by a force that resides outside the individual.
- Macrobius (1952) . Commentary on the Dream of Scipio. Translated by W. H. Stahl. New York: Columbia University Press. p. 90.
We call a dream oracular in which a parent, or a pious or revered man, or a priest, or even a god clearly reveals what will or will not transpire, and what action to take or to avoid.
- Dodds (1951), referring to the type of dream described by Macrobius: "This last type is not, I think, at all common in our own dream-experience. But there is considerable evidence that dreams of this sort were familiar in antiquity." (p. 107).
- ^ Krippner, Stanley; Bogzaran, Fariba; Carvalho, André Percia de (2002). Extraordinary Dreams and How To Work with Them. Albany: State University of New York Press. p. 10. ISBN 0-7914-5257-3.
The Egyptian papyrus of Deral-Madineh was written about 1300 B.C.E. and gives instructions on how to obtain a dream message from a god.
- Lesku, J. A.; Meyer, L. C. R.; Fuller, A.; Maloney, S. K.; Dell'Omo, G.; Vyssotski, A. L.; Rattenborg, N. C. (2011). "Ostriches sleep like platypuses". PLOS ONE. 6 (8): 1–7. Bibcode:2011PLoSO...623203L. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0023203. PMC 3160860. PMID 21887239.
- ^ Solms, Mark (2000). "Dreaming and REM sleep are controlled by different brain mechanisms". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 23 (6): 843–850. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00003988. PMID 11515144. S2CID 7264870.
Dreaming and REM sleep are incompletely correlated. Between 5 and 30% of REM awakenings do not elicit dream reports; and at least 5–10% of NREM awakenings do elicit dream reports that are indistinguishable from REM....
- Bulkeley, Kelly (2008). Dreaming in the world's religions: A comparative history. NYU Press. p. 14. ISBN 978-0-8147-9956-7.
Do animals dream? We currently have no means of proving it one way or the other, just as we have no way to determine whether human fetuses and newborns are genuinely dreaming before they develop the ability to speak and relate their experiences.
- Damasio, Antonio (2010). Self Comes to Mind. New York: Pantheon Books. p. 289. ISBN 978-0-307-37875-0.
…I sympathize with Julian Jaynes's claim that something of great import may have happened to the human mind during the relatively brief interval of time between the events narrated in the Iliad and those that make up the Odyssey.
- Nielsen, Tore A. (1991), "Reality Dreams and Their Effects on Spiritual Belief: A Revision of Animism Theory", in Gackenbach, Jayne; Sheikh, Anees A. (eds.), Dream Images: A Call to Mental Arms, Amityville, NY: Baywood, pp. 233–264, ISBN 0-89503-056-X
- Atwan, Robert (1981). "The Interpretation of Dreams, The Origin of Consciousness, and the Birth of Tragedy". Research Communication in Psychology, Psychiatry and Behavior. 6 (2): 163–182.
- ^ Hall, C., & Van de Castle, R. (1966). The Content Analysis of Dreams. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. Content Analysis Explained Archived 12 April 2007 at the Wayback Machine
- Schneider, Adam; Domhoff, G. William. "The Classification and Coding of Characters". University of California at Santa Cruz. Retrieved 17 July 2021.
- Schredl, Michael; Ciric, Petra; Götz, Simon; Wittmann, Lutz (November 2004). "Typical Dreams: Stability and Gender Differences". The Journal of Psychology. 138 (6): 485–494. doi:10.3200/JRLP.138.6.485-494. PMID 15612605. S2CID 13554573.
- Zadra, A., "1093: Sex dreams: what do men and women dream about?" Archived 27 September 2007 at the Wayback Machine, Sleep Volume 30, Abstract Supplement, 2007 A376.
- "Badan Pusat Statistik "Indonesia Young Adult Reproductive Health Survey 2002–2004" p. 27" (PDF). Archived (PDF) from the original on 9 December 2012. Retrieved 4 April 2013.
- "How do blind people dream? – The Body Odd". March 2012. Archived from the original on 24 January 2013. Retrieved 10 May 2013.
- Hobson, J. Allan; Pace-Schott, Edward F.; Stickgold, Robert (2000). "Dream science 2000: A response to commentaries on Dreaming and the Brain". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 23 (6): 1019. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00954025. S2CID 144729368.
- Chiong, Winston; Leonard, Matthew K.; Chang, Edward F. (2018). "Neurosurgical Patients as Human Research Subjects: Ethical Considerations in Intracranial Electrophysiology Research". Neurosurgery. 83 (1): 29–37. doi:10.1093/neuros/nyx361. PMC 5777911. PMID 28973530.
- ^ Hobson, J. A., Pace-Schott, E. F., & Stickgold, R. (2000). "Dreaming and the brain: Toward a cognitive neuroscience of conscious states". Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23(6), 793–842.
- Ringach, Dario L. (30 July 2009). "The limits of fMRI". Speaking of Research. Retrieved 18 July 2021.
- "Policies on the Use of Animals and Humans in Research". Society for Neuroscience. Retrieved 18 July 2021.
- Uttal, William R. (2013). Reliability in Cognitive Neuroscience. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. p. 4.
Similarly, modern neuroscience research is increasingly showing that activation areas on the brain associated with a cognitive process are far more widely distributed than had been thought only a decade or so ago. Indeed, it now seems likely that most of the brain is active in almost any cognitive process.
- Solms, Mark (2000). "Dreaming and REM sleep are controlled by different brain mechanisms". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 23 (6): 843–850, discussion 904–1121. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00003988. PMID 11515144. S2CID 7264870.
- Braun, A. R.; Balkin, T. J; Wesensten, N. J.; Carson, R. E.; Varga, M.; Baldwin, P.; Selbie, S.; Belenky, G.; Herscovitch, P. (1997). "Regional cerebral blood flow through the sleep-wake cycle". Brain. 120. Oxford University Press: 1173–1197. doi:10.1093/brain/120.7.1173. PMID 9236630.
- Kalat, James W. (2015). Biological Psychology (12th ed.). Boston: Cengage. p. 288. ISBN 978-1305105409.
- Viskontas, Indre (2017). Brain Myths Exploded: Lessons from Neuroscience. Chantilly, VA: The Teaching Company. p. 393.
- Ribeiro, Sidarta (2021). The oracle of night : the history and science of dreams. Daniel Hahn, Sidarta Translation of: Ribeiro. New York. ISBN 978-1-5247-4690-2. OCLC 1200037413.
{{cite book}}
: CS1 maint: location missing publisher (link) - Robert, W. Der Traum als Naturnothwendigkeit erklärt. Zweite Auflage, Hamburg: Seippel, 1886.
- Freud, Sigmund (1965). James Strachey (ed.). The Interpretation of Dreams. Translated by James Strachey. New York: Avon. p. 188.
The view adopted by Robert that the purpose of dreams is to unburden our memory of the useless impressions of daytime is plainly no longer tenable....
- Rycroft, Charles. A Critical Dictionary of Psychoanalysis. London: Penguin Books, 1995, p. 41.
- Freud, Sigmund (1965). James Strachey (ed.). The Interpretation of Dreams. Translated by James Strachey. New York: Avon. p. 253.
- Aserinsky, Eugene; Kleitman, Nathaniel (1953). "Regularly Occurring Periods of Eye Motility, and Concomitant Phenomena, during Sleep". Science. 118 (3062): 273–274. Bibcode:1953Sci...118..273A. doi:10.1126/science.118.3062.273. PMID 13089671.
- Smith, Robert C. (1991), "The Meaning of Dreams: A Current Warning Theory", in Gackenbach, Jayne; Sheikh, Anees A. (eds.), Dream Images: A Call to Mental Arms, Amityville, NY: Baywood, pp. 127–146, ISBN 0-89503-056-X
- Hobson, J. Allan; McCarley, Robert W. (December 1977). "The Brain as a Dream State Generator: An Activation-Synthesis Hypothesis of the Dream Process". The American Journal of Psychiatry. 134 (12): 1335–1348. doi:10.1176/ajp.134.12.1335. PMID 21570.
The dream process is thus seen as having its origin in sensorimotor systems, with little or no primary ideational, volitional, or emotional content. This concept is markedly different from that of the "dream thoughts" or wishes seen by Freud as the primary stimulus for the dream.
- Benjamin, Victoria. "Study Links Dreaming to Increased Memory Performance". The Harvard Crimson. Retrieved 27 January 2022.
- Evans, C.; Newman, E. (1964). "Dreaming: An analogy from computers". New Scientist. 419: 577–579.
- Crick, F.; Mitchison, G. (1983). "The function of dream sleep". Nature. 304 (5922): 111–114. Bibcode:1983Natur.304..111C. doi:10.1038/304111a0. PMID 6866101. S2CID 41500914.
- Hartmann, Ernest (1995). "Making Connections in a Safe Place: Is Dreaming Psychotherapy?". Dreaming. 5 (4): 213–228. doi:10.1037/h0094437.
- Revonsuo, A. (2000). "The reinterpretation of dreams: an evolutionary hypothesis of the function of dreaming". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 23 (6): 877–901. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00004015. PMID 11515147. S2CID 145340071.
- Revonsuo, A.; Tuominen, J. (2015). "Avatars in the Machine: Dreaming as a Simulation of Social Reality". Open MIND: 1–28. doi:10.15502/9783958570375. ISBN 9783958570375.
- Eagleman, David M.; Vaughn, Don A. (May 2021). "The Defensive Activation Theory: REM Sleep as a Mechanism to Prevent Takeover of the Visual Cortex". Frontiers in Neuroscience. 15: 632853. doi:10.3389/fnins.2021.632853. PMC 8176926. PMID 34093109.
- "Weird dreams train us for the unexpected, says new theory". the Guardian. 14 May 2021. Retrieved 5 January 2023.
- Hoel, Erik (14 May 2021). "The overfitted brain: Dreams evolved to assist generalization". Patterns. 2 (5): 100244. doi:10.1016/j.patter.2021.100244. ISSN 2666-3899. PMC 8134940. PMID 34036289.
- Lévy-Bruhl, Lucien (1923). "Chapter III Dreams". Primitive Mentality. Translated by Lilian A. Clare. New York: Macmillan. p. 98.
...n dreams,...man passes from the one world to the other without being aware of it. Such is in fact the ordinary idea of the dream to primitive peoples. The "soul" leaves its tenement for the time being. It frequently goes very far away; it communes with spirits or with ghosts. At the moment of awakening it returns to take its place in the body once more.
- Dunne, J. W. (1950) . An Experiment with Time. London: Faber. p. 23.
- Krishnananda, Swami (16 November 1996). "The Mandukya Upanishad, Section 4". Archived from the original on 9 April 2015. Retrieved 26 March 2015.
- Bar, Shaul (2001). A letter that has not been read: Dreams in the Hebrew Bible. Hebrew Union College Press. Retrieved 4 April 2013.
- Edgar, Iain (2011). The Dream in Islam: From Qur'anic Tradition to Jihadist Inspiration. Oxford: Berghahn Books. p. 178. ISBN 978-0-85745-235-1. Archived from the original on 29 September 2011. Retrieved 9 March 2012.
- ^ Edgar, Iain R.; Henig, David (September 2010). "Istikhara: The Guidance and practice of Islamic dream incubation through ethnographic comparison" (PDF). History and Anthropology. 21 (3): 251–262. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.1012.7334. doi:10.1080/02757206.2010.496781. S2CID 144463607. Archived from the original (PDF) on 8 November 2017. Retrieved 26 October 2017.
- Bulkeley, Kelly (2016). Big Dreams: The Science of Dreaming and the Origins of Religion. Oxford University Press. ISBN 9780199351534.
- Lactantius, De Mortibus Persecutorum 44.4–6, tr. J.L. Creed, Lactantius: De Mortibus Persecutorum (Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1984), qtd. in Lenski, "Reign of Constantine" (CC), 71.
- Eusebius, Vita Constantini 1.27–29; Barnes, Constantine and Eusebius, 43, 306; Odahl, 105–06, 319–20.
- ^ Young, S. (2003). "Dreams". The encyclopedia of South Asian Folklore. Vol. 13. p. 7. Archived from the original on 25 February 2018. Retrieved 24 February 2018 – via Indian Folklife.
- ^ Bulkeley, Kelly (2008). Dreaming in the world's religions: A comparative history. NYU Press. pp. 71–73. ISBN 978-0-8147-9956-7.
- Oppenheim, L.A. (1966). Mantic Dreams in the Ancient Near East in G. E. Von Grunebaum & R. Caillois (Eds.), The Dream and Human Societies (pp. 341–350). London, England: Cambridge University Press.
- Nils P. Heessel : Divinatorische Texte I : ... oneiromantische Omina. Harrassowitz Verlag, 2007.
- O'Neil, C.W. (1976). Dreams, culture and the individual. San Francisco: Chandler & Sharp.
- On Regimen IV, also published sometimes as On Dreams.
- Hobson, J. A. (1988). The Dreaming Brain. Basic Books.
- Steven M. Oberhelman. 1987. “The Diagnostic Dream in Ancient Medical Theory and Practice.” Bulletin of the History of Medicine. 61 (1): 47-60.
- Oberhelman, Steven M. 1983. “Galen, ‘On Diagnosis from Dreams’.” Journal of the History of Medicine and Allied Sciences. 38 (1): 36-47.
- McCurdy, H. G. (1946). "The history of dream theory". Psychological Review. 53 (4): 225–233. doi:10.1037/h0062107. PMID 20998507.
- Rycroft, Charles (1979). The Innocence of Dreams. Random House.
- Cicero, De Republica, 6.10
- Herodotus (1998). The Histories. Oxford University Press. p. 414.
- Uluru – Kata Tjuta National Park: Tjukurpa – Anangu culture Archived 11 July 2009 at the Wayback Machine environment.gov.au, June 23, 2006
- Tedlock, B. (1981). "Quiche Maya dream Interpretation". Ethos. 9 (4): 313–350. doi:10.1525/eth.1981.9.4.02a00050.
- Webb, Craig (1995). "Dreams: Practical Meaning & Applications". The DREAMS Foundation. Archived from the original on 5 March 2016. Retrieved 30 March 2008.
- "Native American Dream Beliefs". Dream Encyclopedia. Archived from the original on 15 April 2012. Retrieved 10 April 2012.
- Morewedge, Carey K.; Norton, Michael I. (2009). "When dreaming is believing: The (motivated) interpretation of dreams" (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 96 (2): 249–264. doi:10.1037/a0013264. PMID 19159131. S2CID 5706448. Archived from the original (PDF) on 14 November 2020.
- ^ Hines, Terence (2003). Pseudoscience and the Paranormal. Prometheus Books. pp. 78–81. ISBN 978-1-57392-979-0.
- Gilovich, Thomas (1991). How We Know What Isn't So: the fallibility of human reason in everyday life. Simon & Schuster. pp. 177–180. ISBN 978-0-02-911706-4.
- "Llewellyn Worldwide – Encyclopedia: Term: Veridical Dream". www.llewellyn.com. Archived from the original on 18 October 2016. Retrieved 16 October 2016.
- Alcock, James E. (1981). Parapsychology: Science or Magic?: a psychological perspective. Oxford: Pergamon Press. ISBN 978-0-08-025773-0. via Hines, Terence (2003). Pseudoscience and the Paranormal. Prometheus Books. pp. 78–81. ISBN 978-1-57392-979-0.
- Madey, Scott; Thomas Gilovich (1993). "Effects of Temporal Focus on the Recall of Expectancy-Consistent and Expectancy-Inconsistent Information". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 65 (3): 458–468. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.65.3.458. PMID 8410650. via Kida, Thomas (2006). Don't Believe Everything You Think: The 6 Basic Mistakes We Make in Thinking. Prometheus Books. ISBN 978-1-59102-408-8.
- "The book of the duchess". Washington State University. Archived from the original on 14 November 2012. Retrieved 24 May 2012.
- "William Langland's The Vision Concerning Piers Plowman". The History Guide. Archived from the original on 6 June 2012. Retrieved 24 May 2012.
- Lovecraft, Howard Phillips (1995). The Dream Cycle of H. P. Lovecraft: Dreams of Terror and Death. Ballantine Books. ISBN 978-0-345-38421-8. Archived from the original on 3 September 2013.
- "The Neverending Story – Book – Pictures – Video – Icons". Archived from the original on 1 June 2012. Retrieved 24 May 2012.
- ^ Van Riper, A. Bowdoin (2002). Science in popular culture: a reference guide. Westport: Greenwood Press. pp. 56–57. ISBN 978-0-313-31822-1.
- Van Riper, op. cit., p. 57.
- Lucid dreaming FAQ Archived 13 March 2007 at the Wayback Machine by The Lucidity Institute at Psych Web.
- Watanabe, T. (2003). "Lucid Dreaming: Its Experimental Proof and Psychological Conditions". J Int Soc Life Inf Sci. 21 (1). ISSN 1341-9226.
- "The University of Chicago Department of Psychiatry" (PDF). 5 September 1975. Archived from the original (PDF) on 26 April 2012. Retrieved 21 October 2013.
- Laberge, Stephen (2014). "Lucid dreaming: Paradoxes of dreaming consciousness". Varieties of anomalous experience: Examining the scientific evidence (2nd ed.). pp. 145–173. doi:10.1037/14258-006. ISBN 978-1-4338-1529-4. S2CID 152082735.
- Olson, Parmy. "Saying 'Hi' Through A Dream: How The Internet Could Make Sleeping More Social". Forbes. Archived from the original on 31 May 2016. Retrieved 24 April 2016.
- ^ "The Science Behind Dreams and Nightmares". NPR.org. Archived from the original on 22 August 2013. Retrieved 4 April 2013.
- ^ Watson, David (2003). "To dream, perchance to remember: Individual differences in dream recall". Personality and Individual Differences. 34 (7): 1271–1286. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00114-9.
- "Why Do Some People Always Remember Their Dreams, While Others Almost Never Do?". Discover Magazine. 2019. Retrieved 10 February 2021.
- Herlin, Bastien; Leu-Semenescu, Smaranda; Chaumereuil, Charlotte; Arnulf, Isabelle (December 2015). "Evidence that non-dreamers do dream: a REM sleep behaviour disorder model". Journal of Sleep Research. 24 (6): 602–609. doi:10.1111/jsr.12323. PMID 26307463.
Adults report, on average, 1–2.8 dream recalls per week in a dream questionnaire and 2.38 dream recalls per week when a home dream diary is completed
- Hobson, J.A.; McCarly, R.W. (1977). "The brain as a dream-state generator: An activation-synthesis hypothesis of the dream process" (PDF). American Journal of Psychiatry. 134 (12): 1335–1348. doi:10.1176/ajp.134.12.1335. PMID 21570. S2CID 10396934. Archived from the original (PDF) on 9 July 2017.
- Underwood, Emily (5 April 2013). "How to Build a Dream-Reading Machine". Science. 340 (6128): 21. doi:10.1126/science.340.6128.21. ISSN 0036-8075. PMID 23559230. Retrieved 15 May 2023.
- Hawks, Charlotte (April 5, 2018). "How close are we to video-recording our dreams?". CNN.
- Morelle, Rebecca (4 April 2013). "Scientists 'read dreams' using brain scans". BBC News. Archived from the original on 27 April 2016. Retrieved 24 April 2016.
- Mcguigan, F. (2012). The Psychophysiology of Thinking: Studies of Covert Processes. Elsevier. ISBN 978-0-323-14700-2.
- Cvetkovic, Dean; Cosic, Irena (2011). States of Consciousness: Experimental Insights into Meditation, Waking, Sleep and Dreams. Springer Science & Business Media. ISBN 978-3-642-18047-7.
- Oldis, Daniel (4 February 2016). "Can We Turn Our Dreams Into Watchable Movies?". The Huffington Post. Archived from the original on 4 August 2016. Retrieved 2016-08-20.
- 莊子, 齊物論, 12. Zhuàngzi, "Discussion on making all things equal," 12. from Zhuàngzi, Burton Watson trans., Chuang Tzu (New York: Columbia University Press, 1996), 43. ISBN 978-0-231-10595-8
- Kher, Chitrarekha V. (1992). Buddhism As Presented by the Brahmanical Systems. Sri Satguru Publications. ISBN 978-81-7030-293-3.
- Hajek P, Belcher M (1991). "Dream of absent-minded transgression: an empirical study of a cognitive withdrawal symptom". J Abnorm Psychol. 100 (4): 487–491. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.100.4.487. PMID 1757662.
- "The Science of Dreaming: 9 Key Points | Psychology Today". www.psychologytoday.com.
- Dormio: A Targeted Dream Incubation Device – PMC – NCBI
- ^ Klinger, Eric (October 1987). Psychology Today.
- Barrett, D.L. (1979). "The Hypnotic Dream: Its Content in Comparison to Nocturnal Dreams and Waking Fantasy". Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 88 (5): 584–591. doi:10.1037/0021-843x.88.5.584.
- Barrett, D.L. "Fantasizers and Dissociaters: Two types of High Hypnotizables, Two Imagery Styles". in R. Kusendorf, N. Spanos, & B. Wallace (Eds.) Hypnosis and Imagination. New York: Baywood, 1996. and, Barrett, D.L. "Dissociaters, Fantasizers, and their Relation to Hypnotizability" in Barrett, D.L. (Ed.) Hypnosis and Hypnotherapy, (2 vol.): Vol. 1: History, theory and general research, Vol. 2: Psychotherapy research and applications, New York: Praeger/Greenwood, 2010.
- Tierney, John (28 June 2010). "Discovering the Virtues of a Wandering Mind". The New York Times. Archived from the original on 21 April 2017.
- American Psychiatric Association (2000), Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th ed, TR, p. 631
- Hockenbury, Don H.; Hockenbury, Sandra E. (2010). Discovering psychology (5th ed.). New York: Worth Publishers. p. 157. ISBN 978-1-4292-1650-0.
- Lohff, David C. (2004). The Dream Directory: The Comprehensive Guide to Analysis and Interpretation. Running Press. ISBN 978-0-7624-1962-3.
- Kahan, Tracey L. (2000). "Effects of melatonin on dream bizarreness among male and female college students". Sleep and Hypnosis. 2 (2): 74–83.
Further reading
- Dreaming – journal published by the American Psychological Association
- Jung, Carl (1934). The Practice of Psychotherapy. "The Practical Use of Dream-analysis". New York: Routledge & Kegan Paul. pp. 139–. ISBN 978-0-7100-1645-4.
- Jung, Carl (2002). Dreams (Routledge Classics). New York: Routledge. ISBN 978-0-415-26740-3.
- Harris, William V. (2009) Dreams and Еxperience in Classical Antiquity. Cambridge, MA & London: Harvard University Press. ISBN 978-0-674-03297-2
External links
- Dreams on In Our Time at the BBC
- Archive for Research in Archetypal Symbolism website
- The International Association for the Study of Dreams
- Dixit, Jay (November 2007). "Dreams: Night School". Psychology Today. Retrieved December 1, 2018.
- alt.dreams A long-running USENET forum wherein readers post and analyze dreams
- LSDBase – online sleep research database documenting physiological effects of dreams through biofeedback
Dreams and oneirology | |||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Phenomena |
| ||||||||||
Therapy and interpretation |
| ||||||||||
Ancient books |