Misplaced Pages

Body schema: Difference between revisions

Article snapshot taken from Wikipedia with creative commons attribution-sharealike license. Give it a read and then ask your questions in the chat. We can research this topic together.
Browse history interactively← Previous editContent deleted Content addedVisualWikitext
Revision as of 20:57, 11 July 2016 editDcirovic (talk | contribs)Autopatrolled, Extended confirmed users, Pending changes reviewers, Rollbackers253,275 edits refs← Previous edit Latest revision as of 22:53, 28 December 2023 edit undoMartinPoulter (talk | contribs)Autopatrolled, Event coordinators, Extended confirmed users, Rollbackers24,129 edits reword to fix weird grammarTag: Visual edit 
(36 intermediate revisions by 23 users not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
{{Short description|Postural model that keeps track of limb position}}
'''Body schema''' is a concept used in several disciplines, including psychology, neuroscience, philosophy, sports medicine, and robotics. The neurologist Sir Henry Head originally defined it as a postural model of the body that actively organizes and modifies 'the impressions produced by incoming sensory impulses in such a way that the final sensation of position, or of locality, rises into consciousness charged with a relation to something that has happened before'.<ref name = "Head2">{{cite book|last=Head |first= H.|year= 1920|title=Studies in Neurology. Vol 2.|publisher= London: Oxford University Press}}</ref> As a postural model that keeps track of limb position, it plays an important role in control of action. It involves aspects of both central (brain processes) and peripheral (sensory, proprioceptive) systems. Thus, a body schema can be considered the collection of processes that registers the posture of one's body parts in space. The schema is updated during body movement. This is typically a non-conscious process, and is used primarily for spatial organization of action. It is therefore a pragmatic representation of the body’s spatial properties, which includes the length of ]s and limb segments, their arrangement, the configuration of the segments in space, and the shape of the body surface.<ref name = "Haggard">{{cite book|last=Haggard |first= P.|author2=D. Wolpert|publisher=Oxford University Press|isbn=0-19-852576-1|url=http://books.google.com/books?id=JICqfxg97bEC&pg=PA261|chapter=Disorders of body schema|title= High-order motor disorders: from neuroanatomy and neurobiology to clinical neurology|year= 2005|pages=261–271}}</ref><ref name = "Holmes">{{cite journal|doi=10.1007/s10339-004-0013-3|last=Holmes |first= N.|author2=C. Spence|title=The body schema and the multisensory representation(s) of peripersonal space|journal= Cognitive processing|year= 2004|volume=5|issue=2|pages=94–105|pmid=16467906|pmc=1350799}}</ref><ref name = "Macaluso">{{cite journal|doi=10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.10.010|author1=Macaluso, E. |author2=A. Maravita |lastauthoramp=yes |title= The representation of space near the body through touch and vision|journal=Neuropsychologia|year= 2010|volume= 48|issue=3|pages=782–795|pmid=19837101}}</ref><ref name = "Maravita">{{cite journal|doi=10.1016/S0960-9822(03)00449-4|last=Maravita |first=A.|author2=C. Spence|author3=J. Driver|title= Multisensory integration and the body schema: close to hand and within reach|journal= Current Biology|year= 2003|volume= 13|issue=13|pages=R531–R539|pmid=12842033}}</ref> Body schema also plays an important role in the integration and use of ] by humans.<ref name = "Berti">{{cite journal|doi=10.1162/089892900562237|last=Berti |first=A. |author2=F. Frassinetti|title= When far becomes near: Remapping of space by tool use|journal= Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience|year= 2000|volume= 12|issue=3|pages=415–420|pmid=10931768}}</ref><ref name = "Carlson">{{cite journal|author=Carlson, T.|title= Rapid Assimilation of External Objects Into the Body Schema|journal= Psychological Science|volume=21|issue=7|pages=1000–5|year= 2010|pmid=20483818|doi=10.1177/0956797610371962|display-authors=etal}}</ref><ref name = "Johnson">{{cite journal|doi=10.1016/j.tics.2003.12.002|last=Johnson-Frey |first= S.|title= The neural bases of complex tool use in humans|journal= Trends in Cognitive Sciences|year= 2004|volume= 8|issue=2|pages=71–78|pmid=15588811}}</ref><ref name = "Maravita2">{{cite journal|doi=10.1016/j.tics.2003.12.008|last=Maravita |first= A.|author2=A. Iriki|title= Tools for the body (schema)|journal= Trends in Cognitive Sciences|year= 2004|volume=8|issue=2|pages=79–86|pmid=15588812}}</ref>
'''Body schema''' is an organism's internal model of its own body, including the position of its limbs. The neurologist ] originally defined it as a postural model of the body that actively organizes and modifies 'the impressions produced by incoming sensory impulses in such a way that the final sensation of ], or of locality, rises into consciousness charged with a relation to something that has happened before'.<ref name = "Head2">{{cite book|last=Head |first= H.|year= 1920|title=Studies in Neurology. Vol 2.|publisher= London: Oxford University Press}}</ref> As a postural model that keeps track of limb position, it plays an important role in control of action.


It involves aspects of both central (brain processes) and peripheral (], ]) systems. Thus, a body schema can be considered the collection of processes that registers the posture of one's body parts in space. The schema is updated during body movement. This is typically a non-conscious process, and is used primarily for spatial organization of action. It is therefore a pragmatic representation of the body’s spatial properties, which includes the length of ]s and limb segments, their arrangement, the configuration of the segments in space, and the shape of the body surface.<ref name="Haggard">{{cite book|last=Haggard |first= P.|author2=D. Wolpert|publisher=Oxford University Press|isbn=0-19-852576-1|chapter-url=https://books.google.com/books?id=JICqfxg97bEC&pg=PA261|chapter=Disorders of body schema|title= High-order motor disorders: from neuroanatomy and neurobiology to clinical neurology|year= 2005|pages=261–271}}</ref><ref name="Holmes">{{cite journal|doi=10.1007/s10339-004-0013-3|last=Holmes |first= N.|author2=C. Spence|title=The body schema and the multisensory representation(s) of peripersonal space|journal= Cognitive Processing|year= 2004|volume=5|issue=2|pages=94–105|pmid=16467906|pmc=1350799}}</ref><ref name="Macaluso">{{cite journal|doi=10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.10.010|author1=Macaluso, E. |author2=A. Maravita |name-list-style=amp |title= The representation of space near the body through touch and vision|journal=Neuropsychologia|year= 2010|volume= 48|issue=3|pages=782–795|pmid=19837101|s2cid=34334160 }}</ref><ref name="Maravita">{{cite journal|doi=10.1016/S0960-9822(03)00449-4|last=Maravita |first=A.|author2=C. Spence|author3=J. Driver|title= Multisensory integration and the body schema: close to hand and within reach|journal= Current Biology|year= 2003|volume= 13|issue=13|pages=R531–R539|pmid=12842033|doi-access=free}}</ref> Body schema also plays an important role in the integration and use of ] by humans.<ref name="Berti">{{cite journal|doi=10.1162/089892900562237|last=Berti |first=A. |author2=F. Frassinetti|title= When far becomes near: Remapping of space by tool use|journal= Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience|year= 2000|volume= 12|issue=3|pages=415–420|pmid=10931768|citeseerx=10.1.1.212.2227|s2cid=207576349 }}</ref><ref name="Carlson">{{cite journal|author=Carlson, T.|title= Rapid Assimilation of External Objects Into the Body Schema|journal= Psychological Science|volume=21|issue=7|pages=1000–5|year= 2010|pmid=20483818|doi=10.1177/0956797610371962|s2cid= 27373885|display-authors=etal}}</ref><ref name="Johnson">{{cite journal|doi=10.1016/j.tics.2003.12.002|last=Johnson-Frey |first= S.|title= The neural bases of complex tool use in humans|journal= Trends in Cognitive Sciences|year= 2004|volume= 8|issue=2|pages=71–78|pmid=15588811|s2cid=16447203 }}</ref><ref name="Maravita2">{{cite journal|doi=10.1016/j.tics.2003.12.008|last=Maravita |first= A.|author2=A. Iriki|title= Tools for the body (schema)|journal= Trends in Cognitive Sciences|year= 2004|volume=8|issue=2|pages=79–86|pmid=15588812|s2cid=9679964 }}</ref>
A clear differentiation of body schema from ] has ].

Body schema is different from ]; the distinction between them has ].


==History== ==History==
], an English neurologist who conducted pioneering work into the somatosensory system and sensory nerves, together with British neurologist ], first described the concept in 1911.<ref name = "Head">{{cite journal|doi=10.1093/brain/34.2-3.102|last=Head |first= H.|author2=G. Holmes|title=Sensory disturbances from cerebral lesions|journal= Brain|year= 1911|volume= 34|issue=2–3|pages=102}}</ref> The concept was first termed "postural schema" to describe the disordered spatial representation of patients following damage to the ] of the brain. Head and Holmes discussed two schemas (or schemata): one body schema for the registration of posture or movement and another body schema for the localization of stimulated locations on the body surface. "Body schema" became the term used for the "organized models of ourselves".<ref name = "Head"/> The term and definition first suggested by Head and Holmes has endured nearly a century of research with clarifications as more has become known about ] and the brain.<ref name="Haggard" />] ], an English neurologist who conducted pioneering work into the somatosensory system and sensory nerves, together with British neurologist ], first described the concept in 1911.<ref name = "Head">{{cite journal|doi=10.1093/brain/34.2-3.102|last=Head |first= H.|author2=G. Holmes|title=Sensory disturbances from cerebral lesions|journal= Brain|year= 1911|volume= 34|issue=2–3|pages=102|url=https://zenodo.org/record/1431638 }}</ref> The concept was first termed "postural schema" to describe the disordered spatial representation of patients following damage to the ] of the brain. Head and Holmes discussed two schemas (or schemata): one body schema for the registration of posture or movement and another body schema for the localization of stimulated locations on the body surface. "Body schema" became the term used for the "organized models of ourselves".<ref name = "Head"/> The term and definition first suggested by Head and Holmes has endured nearly a century of research with clarifications as more has become known about ] and the brain.<ref name="Haggard" />]


==Properties== ==Properties==
Neuroscientists Patrick Haggard and ] have identified seven fundamental properties of the body schema. It is spatially coded, ], ], supramodal, coherent, ] and updated with ].<ref name="Haggard" /> Neuroscientists Patrick Haggard and ] have identified seven fundamental properties of the body schema. It is spatially coded, ], ], supramodal, coherent, ], and updated with ].<ref name="Haggard" />


===Spatial encoding=== ===Spatial encoding===
Line 13: Line 16:


===Modular=== ===Modular===
The body schema is not represented wholly in a single ].<ref name="Haggard" /> Recent ] (functional magnetic resonance imaging) studies confirm earlier results. For example, the schema for feet and hands are coded by different regions of the brain, while the fingers are represented by a separate part entirely.<ref name="Chaminade">{{cite journal|doi=10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.04.026|author1=Chaminade, T. |author2=A. Meltzoff |author3=J. Decety |last-author-amp=yes |title=An fMRI study of imitation: action representation and body schema|journal=Neuropsychologia|year=2005|volume=43|issue=1|pages=115–127|pmid=15488911}}</ref> The body schema is not represented wholly in a single ].<ref name="Haggard" /> Recent ] (functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging) studies confirm earlier results. For example, the schema for feet and hands are coded by different regions of the brain, while the fingers are represented by a separate part entirely.<ref name="Chaminade">{{cite journal|doi=10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.04.026|author1=Chaminade, T. |author2=A. Meltzoff |author3=J. Decety |name-list-style=amp |title=An fMRI study of imitation: action representation and body schema|journal=Neuropsychologia|year=2005|volume=43|issue=1|pages=115–127|pmid=15488911|pmc=3648209}}</ref>


===Adaptable=== ===Adaptable===
] changes to the body schema are active and continuous. For example, gradual changes to the body schema must occur over the lifetime of an individual as he or she grows and absolute and relative sizes of body parts change over his or her life span.<ref name="Haggard" /> The development of the body schema has also been shown to occur in young ]ren. One study showed that with these children (9- 14- and 19-month-olds), older children handled spoons to optimally and comfortably grip them to feed themselves, whereas younger children tended to reach with their ] hand, regardless of the orientation of the spoon and eventual ease of use.<ref name = "Johnson2">{{cite journal|doi=10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00063-3|last=Johnson |first=S.|title= Thinking ahead: the case for motor imagery in prospective judgements of prehension|journal= Cognition|year= 2000|volume= 74|issue=1|pages=33–70|pmid=10594309}}</ref> Short term plasticity has been shown with the integration of tools into the body schema.<ref name = "Carlson"/><ref name = "Maravita2"/> The famous ], has also shown the rapid reorganization of the body schema on the timescale of seconds, showing the high level of plasticity and speed with which the body schema reorganizes.<ref name = "Lewis">{{cite journal|doi=10.1007/s11097-010-9154-2|last=Lewis |first= E. |author2=D. M. Lloyd|title= Embodied experience: A first-person investigation of the rubber hand illusion|journal= Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences|year= 2010|volume= 9|issue=3|pages=317–339}}</ref> In the Illusion, participants view a dummy hand being stroked with a paintbrush, while their own hand is stroked identically. Participants may feel that the touches on their hand are coming from the dummy hand, and even that the dummy hand is, in some way, their own hand. ] changes to the body schema are active and continuous. For example, gradual changes to the body schema must occur over the lifetime of an individual as he or she grows and absolute and relative sizes of body parts change over his or her life span.<ref name="Haggard" /> The development of the body schema has also been shown to occur in young ]ren. One study showed that with these children (9-, 14-, and 19-month-olds), older children handled spoons so as to optimally and comfortably grip them for use, whereas younger children tended to reach with their ] hand, regardless of the orientation of the spoon and eventual ease of use.<ref name = "Johnson2">{{cite journal|doi=10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00063-3|last=Johnson |first=S.|title= Thinking ahead: the case for motor imagery in prospective judgements of prehension|journal= Cognition|year= 2000|volume= 74|issue=1|pages=33–70|pmid=10594309|citeseerx=10.1.1.294.9515|s2cid=554742 }}</ref> Short-term plasticity has been shown with the integration of tools into the body schema.<ref name = "Carlson"/><ref name = "Maravita2"/> The ] has also shown the rapid reorganization of the body schema on the timescale of seconds, showing the high level of plasticity and speed with which the body schema reorganizes.<ref name = "Lewis">{{cite journal|doi=10.1007/s11097-010-9154-2|last=Lewis |first= E. |author2=D. M. Lloyd|title= Embodied experience: A first-person investigation of the rubber hand illusion|journal= Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences|year= 2010|volume= 9|issue=3|pages=317–339|s2cid=145428473 }}</ref> In the Illusion, participants view a dummy hand being stroked with a paintbrush, while their own hand is stroked identically. Participants may feel that the touches on their hand are coming from the dummy hand, and even that the dummy hand is, in some way, their own hand.


===Supramodal=== ===Supramodal===
Line 25: Line 28:


===Interpersonal=== ===Interpersonal===
It is thought that an individual’s body schema is used to represent both one’s own body and the bodies of others. ] are thought to play a role in the interpersonal characteristics of body schema. Interpersonal projection of one’s body schema plays an important role in successfully imitating motions such as hand gestures, especially while maintaining the handedness and location of the gesture, but not necessarily copying the exact motion itself.<ref name = "Chaminade"/> It is thought that an individual's body schema is used to represent both one's own body and the bodies of others. ] are thought to play a role in the interpersonal characteristics of body schema. Interpersonal projection of one's body schema plays an important role in successfully imitating motions such as hand gestures, especially while maintaining the handedness and location of the gesture, but not necessarily copying the exact motion itself.<ref name = "Chaminade"/>


===Updated with movement=== ===Updated with movement===
A working body schema must be able to interactively track the movements and positions of body parts in space.<ref name="Haggard" /> Neurons in the premotor cortex may contribute to this function. A class of neuron in the premotor cortex is multisensory. Each of these multisensory neurons responds to tactile stimuli and also to visual stimuli. The neuron has a tactile receptive field (responsive region on the body surface) typically on the face, arms, or hands. The same neuron also responds to visual stimuli in the space near the tactile receptive field. For example, if a neuron’s tactile receptive field covers the arm, the same neuron will respond to visual stimuli in the space near the arm. As shown by ] and colleagues, the visual receptive field will update with arm movement, translating through space as the arm moves.<ref name=Graziano&etal2007>{{cite journal |author1=Graziano, M.S.A. |author2=Hu, X.T. |author3=Gross, C.G |last-author-amp=yes | year=1997 | title=Coding the locations of objects in the dark | journal=Science | volume=277 | pages=239–241 | doi=10.1126/science.277.5323.239 | PMID=9211852 | issue=5323}}</ref><ref name=Graziano&etal&1994>{{cite journal | authors=], Yap, G.S. and Gross, C.G | year=1994 | title=Coding of visual space by premotor neurons | journal=Science | volume=266 | pages=1054–1057|doi=10.1126/science.7973661 | pmid=7973661}}</ref> Similar body-part-centered neuronal receptive fields relate to the face. These neurons apparently monitor the location of body parts and the location of nearby objects with respect to body parts. Similar neuronal properties may also important for the ability to incorporate external objects into the body schema, such as in tool use. A working body schema must be able to interactively track the movements and positions of body parts in space.<ref name="Haggard" /> Neurons in the premotor cortex may contribute to this function. A class of neuron in the premotor cortex is ]. Each of these multisensory neurons responds to tactile stimuli and also to visual stimuli. The neuron has a tactile receptive field (responsive region on the body surface) typically on the face, arms, or hands. The same neuron also responds to visual stimuli in the space near the tactile receptive field. For example, if a neuron's tactile receptive field covers the arm, the same neuron will respond to visual stimuli in the space near the arm. As shown by ] and colleagues, the visual receptive field will update with arm movement, translating through space as the arm moves.<ref name="Graziano&etal2007">{{cite journal |author1=Graziano, M.S.A. |author2=Hu, X.T. |author3=Gross, C.G |name-list-style=amp | year=1997 | title=Coding the locations of objects in the dark | journal=Science | volume=277 | pages=239–241 | doi=10.1126/science.277.5323.239 | pmid=9211852 | issue=5323}}</ref><ref name="Graziano&etal&1994">{{cite journal | author=Graziano, M.S.A. | author-link=Michael Graziano | author2=Yap, G.S. | author3=Gross, C.G. | name-list-style=amp | year=1994 | title=Coding of visual space by premotor neurons | journal=Science | volume=266 | issue=5187 | pages=1054–1057|doi=10.1126/science.7973661 | pmid=7973661| bibcode=1994Sci...266.1054G | citeseerx=10.1.1.211.7236 }}</ref> Similar body-part-centered neuronal receptive fields relate to the face. These neurons apparently monitor the location of body parts and the location of nearby objects with respect to body parts. Similar neuronal properties may also be important for the ability to incorporate external objects into the body schema, such as in tool use.

==Extended body schema==
The idea of the extended body schema is that, aside from the proprioceptive, visual, and sensory components that contribute to making a mental conception of one's body, the same processes that contribute to a body schema are also able to incorporate external objects into the mental conception of one's body.<ref>Aglioti et al., 1996. Disownership of left hand and objects related to it in a patient with right brain damage. NeuroReport. 8:293-296.</ref> Part philosophical and part neuroscience, this concept builds upon the ideas of plasticity and adaptation to attempt to answer the question of where the body schema ends.

There is debate as to whether this concept truly exists, with one side arguing that the body schema does not extend past the body and the other side believing otherwise.<ref>Reed, C. L., & Farah, M. J. (1995). The psychological reality of the body schema: A test with normal participants. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(2), 334-343.</ref><ref>Yamamoto and Kitazawa. 2011. Reversal of subjective temporal order due to arm crossing. Nature Neuroscience 4: 759-765</ref>

===Supporting arguments===
The perspective shared by those who agree with the theory of the extended body schema follow reasoning in line with such that supports theories on ].

In some studies, attempts at understanding tool assimilation are used to argue for the existence of the extended body schema. In an experiment involving the use and interaction with wool objects, subjects were tested on their ability to perceive afterimages of wool objects in varying contexts. Subjects accustomed their eyes to a dark room and then were shown a brief (1 millisecond) flash of light, intending to produce an afterimage effect of their arms which they held out in front of them during the experiment. Moving an arm afterwards would make the afterimage "fade" or disappear as it moved, thus indicating that the feature (the arm) was being tracked and integrated into the person's body schema. To test integration of the meaningless wool objects, subjects experienced four different contexts.

# Subjects held the wool objects in each hand and one hand (the active hand) would move, still holding the object (the active object).
# Using the active hand, the active wool object would be dropped once an afterimage was perceived.
# Using the active hand, one would grab the active wool object once an afterimage was perceived.
# The subjects were to hold onto a mechanical device which held the wool object. Once an afterimage was perceived, a subject's active hand would cause the mechanical device to drop the wool object.

In all situations but the fourth, the subjects experienced the same "fading" effect as they did with their arm alone. This would thus indicate that the wool objects had been integrated into their body schema and contributes support towards the idea of the body's using proprioceptive and visual elements to create an extended body schema. The mechanical device acted as an intermediate between the subject and the active object, and the subjects' failure to detect an afterimage in that context indicates that this concept of extension is limited to being sensitive to only what the body is directly in contact with.<ref name = "Carlson"/>

===Dissenting arguments===

The alternate perspective is that the body is the limit of any sort of body schema.

An example of this division is found in a study and discussion on personal and extrapersonal attention, where personal relates to the body's sense of itself (the body schema) and extrapersonal relates to all external of such. Some research supports the claim that these two categories are purely distinct and do not intermingle, contrary to what the extended body schema theory describes. Evidence for such is primarily found in subjects with unilateral neglect, such as in the case of E.D.S., who was a middle-aged man with right hemisphere brain damage. When he was tested for hemispatial neglect using traditional measures such as sentence reading and cancellation tests, E.D.S. showed few signs and upon later examination showed no signs whatsoever, leading doctors to believe he was normal. However, he constantly had issues with physical therapy because he would claim to not be able to see his left leg; upon further examination, E.D.S. was known to have a particular type of hemispatial neglect that only affected the perception of his body. The motor function of the left side of his body was negatively affected though not totally compromised, yet when attempting tasks such as shaving, he would invariably not shave the left side of his face. This led some researchers to believe that there is a distinction between personal and extrapersonal neglect, which would thus reflect a similar distinction with body schema itself.<ref>Guariglia and Antonucci. 1992. Personal and extrapersonal space: A case of neglect dissociation. Neuropsychologia, 30:1001-1009</ref>


==Associated disorders== ==Associated disorders==


===Deafferentation=== ===Deafferentation===
The most direct of related ], ] occurs when sensory input from the body is reduced or absent, without affecting ], or motor, neurons. The most famous case of this disorder is "IW", who lost all sensory input from below the neck, resulting in temporary paralysis. He was forced to learn to control his movement all over again using only his conscious body image and visual feedback. As a result, when constant visual input is lost during an activity, such as walking, it becomes impossible for him to complete the task, which may result in falling, or simply stopping. IW requires constant attention to tasks to be able to complete them accurately, demonstrating how automatic and ] the process of integrating touch and proprioception into the body schema actually is.<ref name = "Gallagher">{{cite journal|last=Gallagher |first= S.|author2=J. Cole|title= Body schema and body image in a deafferented subject|journal= Journal of Mind and Behavior|year= 1995|volume= 16|issue=4|pages=369–390|url=http://www.umaine.edu/jmb/archives/volume16/16_4_1995autumn.html#abstract2 | accessdate=16 June 2012}}</ref> The most direct of related ], ] occurs when sensory input from the body is reduced or absent, without affecting ]. The most famous case of this disorder is "IW", who lost all sensory input from below the neck, resulting in temporary paralysis. He was forced to learn to control his movement all over again using only his conscious body image and visual feedback. As a result, when constant visual input is lost during an activity, such as walking, it becomes impossible for him to complete the task, which may result in falling, or simply stopping. IW requires constant attention to tasks to be able to complete them accurately, demonstrating how automatic and ] the process of integrating touch and proprioception into the body schema actually is.<ref name = "Gallagher">{{cite journal|last= Gallagher|first= S.|author2= J. Cole|title= Body schema and body image in a deafferented subject|journal= Journal of Mind and Behavior|year= 1995|volume= 16|issue= 4|pages= 369–390|url= http://www.umaine.edu/jmb/archives/volume16/16_4_1995autumn.html#abstract2|accessdate= 16 June 2012|archive-url= https://web.archive.org/web/20080526000553/http://www.umaine.edu/jmb/archives/volume16/16_4_1995autumn.html#abstract2|archive-date= 26 May 2008|url-status= dead}}</ref>


===Autotopagnosia=== ===Autotopagnosia===
] typically occurs after left parietal lesions. Patients with this disorder make errors which result from confusion between adjacent body parts. For example, a patient may point to their knee when asked to point to their hip. Because the disorder involves the body schema, localization errors may be made both on the patient’s own body and that of others. The spatial unity of the body within the body schema has been damaged such that it has incorrectly been segmented in relation to its other modular parts.<ref name = "Sirigu">{{cite journal|doi=10.1093/brain/114.1.629|author=Sirigu, A.|title= Multiple representations contribute to body knowledge processing: Evidence from a case of autotopagnosia|journal= Brain|year= 1991|volume=114|issue=1|pages=629|display-authors=etal}}</ref> ] typically occurs after left parietal lesions. Patients with this disorder make errors which result from confusion between adjacent body parts. For example, a patient may point to their knee when asked to point to their hip. Because the disorder involves the body schema, localization errors may be made both on the patient’s own body and that of others. The spatial unity of the body within the body schema has been damaged such that it has incorrectly been segmented in relation to its other modular parts.<ref name = "Sirigu">{{cite journal|doi=10.1093/brain/114.1.629|pmid=2004260|author=Sirigu, A.|title= Multiple representations contribute to body knowledge processing: Evidence from a case of autotopagnosia|journal= Brain|year= 1991|volume=114|issue=1|pages=629–42|display-authors=etal|url=https://zenodo.org/record/1234297}}</ref>


===Phantom limb=== ===Phantom limb===
]s are a phenomenon which occurs following ] of a limb from an individual. In 90–98% of cases, amputees report feeling all or part of the limb or body part still there, taking up space.<ref name = "Ramachandran">{{cite journal|doi=10.1093/brain/121.9.1603|last=Ramachandran |first=V.S.|author2=W. Hirstein|title= The perception of phantom limbs. The D. O. Hebb lecture|journal= Brain|year= 1998|volume= 121|issue=9|pages=1603–30|pmid=9762952}}</ref> The amputee may perceive a limb under full control, or ]. A common side effect of phantom limbs is phantom limb pain. The neurophysiological mechanisms by which phantom limbs occur is still under debate.<ref name = "Giummarra">{{cite journal|doi=10.1016/j.brainresrev.2007.01.009|author=Giummarra, M.|title= Central mechanisms in phantom limb perception: the past, present and future|journal= Brain Research Reviews|year= 2007|volume= 54|issue=1|pages=219–232|pmid=17500095|display-authors=etal}}</ref> A common theory posits that the ], since deafferented due to amputation, typically remap to adjacent cortical regions within the brain. This can cause amputees to report feeling their missing limb being touched when a seemingly unrelated part of the body is stimulated (such as if the face is touched, but the amputee also feels their missing arm being stroked in a specific location). Another facet of phantom limbs is that the ] (motor feedback) responsible for reporting on position to the body schema does not attenuate quickly. Thus the missing body part may be attributed by the amputee to still be in a fixed or movable position.<ref name="Haggard" /> ]s are a phenomenon which occurs following ] of a limb from an individual. In 90–98% of cases, amputees report feeling all or part of the limb or body part still there, taking up space.<ref name = "Ramachandran">{{cite journal|doi=10.1093/brain/121.9.1603|last=Ramachandran |first=V.S.|author2=W. Hirstein|title= The perception of phantom limbs. The D. O. Hebb lecture|journal= Brain|year= 1998|volume= 121|issue=9|pages=1603–30|pmid=9762952|doi-access=free}}</ref> The amputee may perceive a limb under full control, or ]. A common side effect of phantom limbs is phantom limb pain. The neurophysiological mechanisms by which phantom limbs occur is still under debate.<ref name = "Giummarra">{{cite journal|doi=10.1016/j.brainresrev.2007.01.009|author=Giummarra, M.|title= Central mechanisms in phantom limb perception: the past, present and future|journal= Brain Research Reviews|year= 2007|volume= 54|issue=1|pages=219–232|pmid=17500095|s2cid=42209339 |display-authors=etal}}</ref> A common theory posits that the ], since deafferented due to amputation, typically remap to adjacent cortical regions within the brain. This can cause amputees to report feeling their missing limb being touched when a seemingly unrelated part of the body is stimulated (such as if the face is touched, but the amputee also feels their missing arm being stroked in a specific location). Another facet of phantom limbs is that the ] (motor feedback) responsible for reporting on position to the body schema does not attenuate quickly. Thus the missing body part may be attributed by the amputee to still be in a fixed or movable position.<ref name="Haggard" />

===Others===
], ], ], ], ] and ] all involve (or in some cases involve) aspects of impaired body schema. Hemispatial neglect is not uncommon because ]s sometimes cause it.


==Tool use== ==Tool use==
Line 48: Line 77:


==Confusion with body image== ==Confusion with body image==
Historically, body schema and ] were generally lumped together, used interchangeably, or ill-defined. In science and elsewhere, the two terms are still commonly misattributed or confused. Efforts have been made to distinguish the two and define them in clear and differentiable ways.<ref name = "Gallagherbook">{{cite book|last=Gallagher |first=S.|title= How the body shapes the mind|year= 2006|isbn=0-19-920416-0|publisher= Oxford University Press, USA}}</ref> A body image consists of perceptions, attitudes, and beliefs concerning one's body. In contrast, body schema consists of sensory-motor capacities that control movement and posture. Historically, body schema and ] were generally lumped together, used interchangeably, or ill-defined. In science and elsewhere, the two terms are still commonly misattributed or confused. Efforts have been made to distinguish the two and define them in clear and differentiable ways.<ref name = "Gallagherbook">{{cite book|last=Gallagher |first=S.|title= How the body shapes the mind|year= 2006|isbn=0-19-920416-0|publisher= Oxford University Press, USA}}</ref> A body image consists of perceptions, attitudes, and beliefs concerning one's body. In contrast, body schema consists of sensory-motor capacities that control movement and posture.


Body image may involve a person’s conscious perception of his or her own physical appearance. It is how individuals see themselves when picturing themselves in their mind, or when perceiving themselves in a mirror. Body image differs from body schema as perception differs from movement. Both may be involved in action, especially when learning new movements. Body image may involve a person’s conscious perception of his or her own physical appearance. It is how individuals see themselves when picturing themselves in their mind, or when perceiving themselves in a mirror. Body image differs from body schema as perception differs from movement. Both may be involved in action, especially when learning new movements.
Line 54: Line 83:
==See also== ==See also==
* ] * ]
* ]
* ]
* ]
* ] * ]
* ]


==References== ==References==

Latest revision as of 22:53, 28 December 2023

Postural model that keeps track of limb position

Body schema is an organism's internal model of its own body, including the position of its limbs. The neurologist Sir Henry Head originally defined it as a postural model of the body that actively organizes and modifies 'the impressions produced by incoming sensory impulses in such a way that the final sensation of body position, or of locality, rises into consciousness charged with a relation to something that has happened before'. As a postural model that keeps track of limb position, it plays an important role in control of action.

It involves aspects of both central (brain processes) and peripheral (sensory, proprioceptive) systems. Thus, a body schema can be considered the collection of processes that registers the posture of one's body parts in space. The schema is updated during body movement. This is typically a non-conscious process, and is used primarily for spatial organization of action. It is therefore a pragmatic representation of the body’s spatial properties, which includes the length of limbs and limb segments, their arrangement, the configuration of the segments in space, and the shape of the body surface. Body schema also plays an important role in the integration and use of tools by humans.

Body schema is different from body image; the distinction between them has developed over time.

History

Henry Head, an English neurologist who conducted pioneering work into the somatosensory system and sensory nerves, together with British neurologist Gordon Morgan Holmes, first described the concept in 1911. The concept was first termed "postural schema" to describe the disordered spatial representation of patients following damage to the parietal lobe of the brain. Head and Holmes discussed two schemas (or schemata): one body schema for the registration of posture or movement and another body schema for the localization of stimulated locations on the body surface. "Body schema" became the term used for the "organized models of ourselves". The term and definition first suggested by Head and Holmes has endured nearly a century of research with clarifications as more has become known about neuroscience and the brain.

A portrait of Henry Head, the pioneering English neurologist who first defined and used the term "body schema".

Properties

Neuroscientists Patrick Haggard and Daniel Wolpert have identified seven fundamental properties of the body schema. It is spatially coded, modular, adaptable, supramodal, coherent, interpersonal, and updated with movement.

Spatial encoding

The body schema represents both position and configuration of the body as a 3-dimensional object in space. A combination of sensory information, primarily tactile and visual, contributes to the representation of the limbs in space. This integration allows for stimuli to be localized in external space with respect to the body. An example by Haggard and Wolpert shows the combination of tactile sensation of the hand with information about the joint angles of the arm, which allow for rapid movements of said arm to swat a fly.

Modular

The body schema is not represented wholly in a single region of the brain. Recent fMRI (functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging) studies confirm earlier results. For example, the schema for feet and hands are coded by different regions of the brain, while the fingers are represented by a separate part entirely.

Adaptable

Plastic changes to the body schema are active and continuous. For example, gradual changes to the body schema must occur over the lifetime of an individual as he or she grows and absolute and relative sizes of body parts change over his or her life span. The development of the body schema has also been shown to occur in young children. One study showed that with these children (9-, 14-, and 19-month-olds), older children handled spoons so as to optimally and comfortably grip them for use, whereas younger children tended to reach with their dominant hand, regardless of the orientation of the spoon and eventual ease of use. Short-term plasticity has been shown with the integration of tools into the body schema. The rubber hand illusion has also shown the rapid reorganization of the body schema on the timescale of seconds, showing the high level of plasticity and speed with which the body schema reorganizes. In the Illusion, participants view a dummy hand being stroked with a paintbrush, while their own hand is stroked identically. Participants may feel that the touches on their hand are coming from the dummy hand, and even that the dummy hand is, in some way, their own hand.

Supramodal

By its nature, body schema integrates proprioceptive, (the sense of the relative position of neighbouring parts of one's body), and tactile information to maintain a three-dimensional body representation. However, other sensory information, particularly visual, can be in the same representation of the body. This simultaneous participation means there are combined representations within the body schema, which suggests the involvement of a process to translate primary information (e.g. visual, tactile, etc.) into a single sensory modality or an abstract, amodal form.

Coherent

The body schema, to function properly, must be able to maintain coherent organization continuously. To do so, it must be able to resolve any differences between sensory inputs. Resolving these inter-sensory inconsistencies can result in interesting sensations, such as those experienced during the Rubber Hand Illusion.

Interpersonal

It is thought that an individual's body schema is used to represent both one's own body and the bodies of others. Mirror neurons are thought to play a role in the interpersonal characteristics of body schema. Interpersonal projection of one's body schema plays an important role in successfully imitating motions such as hand gestures, especially while maintaining the handedness and location of the gesture, but not necessarily copying the exact motion itself.

Updated with movement

A working body schema must be able to interactively track the movements and positions of body parts in space. Neurons in the premotor cortex may contribute to this function. A class of neuron in the premotor cortex is multisensory. Each of these multisensory neurons responds to tactile stimuli and also to visual stimuli. The neuron has a tactile receptive field (responsive region on the body surface) typically on the face, arms, or hands. The same neuron also responds to visual stimuli in the space near the tactile receptive field. For example, if a neuron's tactile receptive field covers the arm, the same neuron will respond to visual stimuli in the space near the arm. As shown by Graziano and colleagues, the visual receptive field will update with arm movement, translating through space as the arm moves. Similar body-part-centered neuronal receptive fields relate to the face. These neurons apparently monitor the location of body parts and the location of nearby objects with respect to body parts. Similar neuronal properties may also be important for the ability to incorporate external objects into the body schema, such as in tool use.

Extended body schema

The idea of the extended body schema is that, aside from the proprioceptive, visual, and sensory components that contribute to making a mental conception of one's body, the same processes that contribute to a body schema are also able to incorporate external objects into the mental conception of one's body. Part philosophical and part neuroscience, this concept builds upon the ideas of plasticity and adaptation to attempt to answer the question of where the body schema ends.

There is debate as to whether this concept truly exists, with one side arguing that the body schema does not extend past the body and the other side believing otherwise.

Supporting arguments

The perspective shared by those who agree with the theory of the extended body schema follow reasoning in line with such that supports theories on tool use.

In some studies, attempts at understanding tool assimilation are used to argue for the existence of the extended body schema. In an experiment involving the use and interaction with wool objects, subjects were tested on their ability to perceive afterimages of wool objects in varying contexts. Subjects accustomed their eyes to a dark room and then were shown a brief (1 millisecond) flash of light, intending to produce an afterimage effect of their arms which they held out in front of them during the experiment. Moving an arm afterwards would make the afterimage "fade" or disappear as it moved, thus indicating that the feature (the arm) was being tracked and integrated into the person's body schema. To test integration of the meaningless wool objects, subjects experienced four different contexts.

  1. Subjects held the wool objects in each hand and one hand (the active hand) would move, still holding the object (the active object).
  2. Using the active hand, the active wool object would be dropped once an afterimage was perceived.
  3. Using the active hand, one would grab the active wool object once an afterimage was perceived.
  4. The subjects were to hold onto a mechanical device which held the wool object. Once an afterimage was perceived, a subject's active hand would cause the mechanical device to drop the wool object.

In all situations but the fourth, the subjects experienced the same "fading" effect as they did with their arm alone. This would thus indicate that the wool objects had been integrated into their body schema and contributes support towards the idea of the body's using proprioceptive and visual elements to create an extended body schema. The mechanical device acted as an intermediate between the subject and the active object, and the subjects' failure to detect an afterimage in that context indicates that this concept of extension is limited to being sensitive to only what the body is directly in contact with.

Dissenting arguments

The alternate perspective is that the body is the limit of any sort of body schema.

An example of this division is found in a study and discussion on personal and extrapersonal attention, where personal relates to the body's sense of itself (the body schema) and extrapersonal relates to all external of such. Some research supports the claim that these two categories are purely distinct and do not intermingle, contrary to what the extended body schema theory describes. Evidence for such is primarily found in subjects with unilateral neglect, such as in the case of E.D.S., who was a middle-aged man with right hemisphere brain damage. When he was tested for hemispatial neglect using traditional measures such as sentence reading and cancellation tests, E.D.S. showed few signs and upon later examination showed no signs whatsoever, leading doctors to believe he was normal. However, he constantly had issues with physical therapy because he would claim to not be able to see his left leg; upon further examination, E.D.S. was known to have a particular type of hemispatial neglect that only affected the perception of his body. The motor function of the left side of his body was negatively affected though not totally compromised, yet when attempting tasks such as shaving, he would invariably not shave the left side of his face. This led some researchers to believe that there is a distinction between personal and extrapersonal neglect, which would thus reflect a similar distinction with body schema itself.

Associated disorders

Deafferentation

The most direct of related disorders, deafferentation occurs when sensory input from the body is reduced or absent, without affecting motor neurons. The most famous case of this disorder is "IW", who lost all sensory input from below the neck, resulting in temporary paralysis. He was forced to learn to control his movement all over again using only his conscious body image and visual feedback. As a result, when constant visual input is lost during an activity, such as walking, it becomes impossible for him to complete the task, which may result in falling, or simply stopping. IW requires constant attention to tasks to be able to complete them accurately, demonstrating how automatic and subconscious the process of integrating touch and proprioception into the body schema actually is.

Autotopagnosia

Autotopagnosia typically occurs after left parietal lesions. Patients with this disorder make errors which result from confusion between adjacent body parts. For example, a patient may point to their knee when asked to point to their hip. Because the disorder involves the body schema, localization errors may be made both on the patient’s own body and that of others. The spatial unity of the body within the body schema has been damaged such that it has incorrectly been segmented in relation to its other modular parts.

Phantom limb

Phantom limbs are a phenomenon which occurs following amputation of a limb from an individual. In 90–98% of cases, amputees report feeling all or part of the limb or body part still there, taking up space. The amputee may perceive a limb under full control, or paralyzed. A common side effect of phantom limbs is phantom limb pain. The neurophysiological mechanisms by which phantom limbs occur is still under debate. A common theory posits that the afferent neurons, since deafferented due to amputation, typically remap to adjacent cortical regions within the brain. This can cause amputees to report feeling their missing limb being touched when a seemingly unrelated part of the body is stimulated (such as if the face is touched, but the amputee also feels their missing arm being stroked in a specific location). Another facet of phantom limbs is that the efferent copy (motor feedback) responsible for reporting on position to the body schema does not attenuate quickly. Thus the missing body part may be attributed by the amputee to still be in a fixed or movable position.

Others

Asomatognosia, somatoparaphrenia, anosognosia, anosodiaphoria, allochiria and hemispatial neglect all involve (or in some cases involve) aspects of impaired body schema. Hemispatial neglect is not uncommon because strokes sometimes cause it.

Tool use

Rhesus macaques are able to be trained to use rudimentary tools, but have never been proven to use tools spontaneously in the wild.

Not only is it necessary for the body schema to be able to integrate and form a three-dimensional representation of the body, but it also plays an important role in tool use. Studies recording neuronal activity in the intraparietal cortex in macaques have shown that, with training, the macaque body schema updates to include tools, such as those used for reaching, into the body schema. In humans, body schema plays an important role in both simple and complex tool use, far beyond that of macaques. Extensive training is also not necessary for this integration.

The mechanisms by which tools are integrated into the body schema are not fully understood. However, studies with long-term training have shown interesting phenomena. When wielding tools in both hands in a crossed posture, behavioral effects reverse in a similar way to when only hands are crossed. Thus, sensory stimuli are delivered the same way be it to the hands directly or indirectly via the tools. These studies suggest the mind incorporates the tools into the same or similar areas as it does the adjacent hands. Recent research into the short term plasticity of the body schema used individuals without any prior training with tools. These results, derived from the relation between afterimages and body schema, show that tools are incorporated into the body schema within seconds, regardless of length of training, though the results do not extend to other species besides humans.

Confusion with body image

Historically, body schema and body image were generally lumped together, used interchangeably, or ill-defined. In science and elsewhere, the two terms are still commonly misattributed or confused. Efforts have been made to distinguish the two and define them in clear and differentiable ways. A body image consists of perceptions, attitudes, and beliefs concerning one's body. In contrast, body schema consists of sensory-motor capacities that control movement and posture.

Body image may involve a person’s conscious perception of his or her own physical appearance. It is how individuals see themselves when picturing themselves in their mind, or when perceiving themselves in a mirror. Body image differs from body schema as perception differs from movement. Both may be involved in action, especially when learning new movements.

See also

References

  1. Head, H. (1920). Studies in Neurology. Vol 2. London: Oxford University Press.
  2. ^ Haggard, P.; D. Wolpert (2005). "Disorders of body schema". High-order motor disorders: from neuroanatomy and neurobiology to clinical neurology. Oxford University Press. pp. 261–271. ISBN 0-19-852576-1.
  3. Holmes, N.; C. Spence (2004). "The body schema and the multisensory representation(s) of peripersonal space". Cognitive Processing. 5 (2): 94–105. doi:10.1007/s10339-004-0013-3. PMC 1350799. PMID 16467906.
  4. ^ Macaluso, E. & A. Maravita (2010). "The representation of space near the body through touch and vision". Neuropsychologia. 48 (3): 782–795. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.10.010. PMID 19837101. S2CID 34334160.
  5. Maravita, A.; C. Spence; J. Driver (2003). "Multisensory integration and the body schema: close to hand and within reach". Current Biology. 13 (13): R531 – R539. doi:10.1016/S0960-9822(03)00449-4. PMID 12842033.
  6. ^ Berti, A.; F. Frassinetti (2000). "When far becomes near: Remapping of space by tool use". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 12 (3): 415–420. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.212.2227. doi:10.1162/089892900562237. PMID 10931768. S2CID 207576349.
  7. ^ Carlson, T.; et al. (2010). "Rapid Assimilation of External Objects Into the Body Schema". Psychological Science. 21 (7): 1000–5. doi:10.1177/0956797610371962. PMID 20483818. S2CID 27373885.
  8. ^ Johnson-Frey, S. (2004). "The neural bases of complex tool use in humans". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 8 (2): 71–78. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2003.12.002. PMID 15588811. S2CID 16447203.
  9. ^ Maravita, A.; A. Iriki (2004). "Tools for the body (schema)". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 8 (2): 79–86. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2003.12.008. PMID 15588812. S2CID 9679964.
  10. ^ Head, H.; G. Holmes (1911). "Sensory disturbances from cerebral lesions". Brain. 34 (2–3): 102. doi:10.1093/brain/34.2-3.102.
  11. ^ Chaminade, T.; A. Meltzoff & J. Decety (2005). "An fMRI study of imitation: action representation and body schema". Neuropsychologia. 43 (1): 115–127. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.04.026. PMC 3648209. PMID 15488911.
  12. Johnson, S. (2000). "Thinking ahead: the case for motor imagery in prospective judgements of prehension". Cognition. 74 (1): 33–70. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.294.9515. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00063-3. PMID 10594309. S2CID 554742.
  13. ^ Lewis, E.; D. M. Lloyd (2010). "Embodied experience: A first-person investigation of the rubber hand illusion". Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences. 9 (3): 317–339. doi:10.1007/s11097-010-9154-2. S2CID 145428473.
  14. Graziano, M.S.A.; Hu, X.T. & Gross, C.G (1997). "Coding the locations of objects in the dark". Science. 277 (5323): 239–241. doi:10.1126/science.277.5323.239. PMID 9211852.
  15. Graziano, M.S.A.; Yap, G.S. & Gross, C.G. (1994). "Coding of visual space by premotor neurons". Science. 266 (5187): 1054–1057. Bibcode:1994Sci...266.1054G. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.211.7236. doi:10.1126/science.7973661. PMID 7973661.
  16. Aglioti et al., 1996. Disownership of left hand and objects related to it in a patient with right brain damage. NeuroReport. 8:293-296.
  17. Reed, C. L., & Farah, M. J. (1995). The psychological reality of the body schema: A test with normal participants. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(2), 334-343.
  18. Yamamoto and Kitazawa. 2011. Reversal of subjective temporal order due to arm crossing. Nature Neuroscience 4: 759-765
  19. Guariglia and Antonucci. 1992. Personal and extrapersonal space: A case of neglect dissociation. Neuropsychologia, 30:1001-1009
  20. Gallagher, S.; J. Cole (1995). "Body schema and body image in a deafferented subject". Journal of Mind and Behavior. 16 (4): 369–390. Archived from the original on 26 May 2008. Retrieved 16 June 2012.
  21. Sirigu, A.; et al. (1991). "Multiple representations contribute to body knowledge processing: Evidence from a case of autotopagnosia". Brain. 114 (1): 629–42. doi:10.1093/brain/114.1.629. PMID 2004260.
  22. Ramachandran, V.S.; W. Hirstein (1998). "The perception of phantom limbs. The D. O. Hebb lecture". Brain. 121 (9): 1603–30. doi:10.1093/brain/121.9.1603. PMID 9762952.
  23. Giummarra, M.; et al. (2007). "Central mechanisms in phantom limb perception: the past, present and future". Brain Research Reviews. 54 (1): 219–232. doi:10.1016/j.brainresrev.2007.01.009. PMID 17500095. S2CID 42209339.
  24. Gallagher, S. (2006). How the body shapes the mind. Oxford University Press, USA. ISBN 0-19-920416-0.
Categories: