This is an old revision of this page, as edited by Quizkajer (talk | contribs) at 05:56, 6 June 2005 (→Background information). The present address (URL) is a permanent link to this revision, which may differ significantly from the current revision.
Revision as of 05:56, 6 June 2005 by Quizkajer (talk | contribs) (→Background information)(diff) ← Previous revision | Latest revision (diff) | Newer revision → (diff){{subst:#ifeq:a|b||{{subst:#ifexist:Misplaced Pages:Articles for deletion/{{subst:PAGENAME}}|{{subst:lessthan}}!-- The nomination page for this article already existed when this tag was added. If this was because the article had been nominated for deletion before, and you wish to renominate it, please replace "page={{subst:PAGENAME}}" with "page={{subst:PAGENAME}} (2nd nomination)" below before proceeding with the nomination.
-->}}}}This template must be substituted. Replace {{afd
with {{subst:afd
.
{{subst:lessthan}}!-- Once discussion is closed, please place on talk page:
This article was nominated for deletion on {{subst:#time:j F Y|{{subst:CURRENTTIMESTAMP}} }}. The result of the discussion was keep. |
-->
Race and intelligence refers to the controversy surrounding the findings of many studies that racial groups show differences in average intelligence quotient (IQ) scores. Members of these groups can be found at every IQ level, but the findings refer to where each groups' members tend to cluster along the IQ line.
These results have sparked public debates concerning not only the reliability of the studies and the motives of their authors, but also the validity and fairness of intelligence tests in general and extent to which measured intelligence is determined on the one hand by genetic and on the other hand by social and developmental factors including nutrition, richness of the learning environment during the individual's most formative years of life, etc.
Background information
Racial distinctions are most often based on skin color, facial features, ancestry, and national origin. Some scientists argue that common racial classifications are not meaningful, often on the basis of research indicating that more genetic variation exists within such races than between them. See the article Race for further discussion.
Intelligence is most commonly measured by performance on IQ tests. In turn, IQ tests are generally geared to measure the psychometric variable g. Some question the validity of all IQ testing or claim that there are aspects of intelligence not reflected in IQ tests. See the articles Intelligence, IQ, and g theory for further discussion.
Some researchers have argued that race and intelligence research is fundamentally flawed. Stephen Jay Gould expressed this viewin his 1981 book The Mismeasure of Man. Similarly, in a 2005 review paper Sternberg and colleagues question the basis of race and intelligence research :
- In this article, the authors argue that the overwhelming portion of the literature on intelligence, race, and genetics is based on folk taxonomies rather than scientific analysis. They suggest that because theorists of intelligence disagree as to what it is, any consideration of its relationships to other constructs must be tentative at best. They further argue that race is a social construction with no scientific definition. Thus, studies of the relationship between race and other constructs may serve social ends but cannot serve scientific ends.
The debates described in the following article assume that IQ tests measure some interesting aspect of intelligence and that some interesting information may be gained by studying racial group differences. For a critique of these assumptions, please see the previously mentioned articles.
The scholarly debate about race and intelligence involves both the relatively less controversial experimental results which find that average IQ test scores vary between racial groups, and the relatively more controversial interpretation of these IQ score differences. In general, interpretations of the "IQ gap" can be divided into two categories:
- "culture-only" interpretations that posit environmental causes (e.g., socioeconomic inequality or minority culture membership) that differentially affect racial groups; and
- "partly-genetic" interpretations that posit an IQ gap between racial groups caused by approximately the same matrix of genetic and environmental forces that cause IQ differences among individuals of the same race.
Moral criticism
A political motivation is frequently ascribed to researchers who work on questions of race and intelligence. Many have been described as racists. In turn, many researchers have questioned the political motivations of their critics.
A common criticism of race and intelligence research, which is more often stated implicitly rather than explicitly, is that society would be better off not knowing if races differ in IQ, regardless of whether the cause were genetic or not. For example, Glazer (1994, p. 16) asked of race and intelligence research in The Bell Curve, "what good will come of it?" He adds,
- Our society, our polity, our elites, according to Herrnstein and Murray, live with an untruth: that there is no good reason for this inequality, and therefore society is at fault and we must try harder. I ask myself whether the untruth is not better for American society than the truth.
More recently, Yale psychologist Robert Sternberg asked whether race and intelligence researchers Arthur Jensen and J. Philippe Rushton show "good taste" in their choice of research topics. Further, he questioned, "What good is research of the kind done by Rushton and Jensen supposed to achieve?" (Sternberg, 2005). Harvard University microbiologist Bernard Davis (1978) criticized this position as the "moralistic fallacy", implying it was the converse of the naturalistic fallacy. Some researchers in the field of race and intelligence argue that suppressing race and intelligence research is actually more harmful. For example, Gottfredson (2005) argues,
- Lying about race differences in achievement is harmful because it foments mutual recrimination. Because the untruth insists that differences cannot be natural, they must be artificial, manmade, manufactured. Someone must be at fault. Someone must be refusing to do the right thing. It therefore sustains unwarranted, divisive, and ever-escalating mutual accusations of moral culpability, such as Whites are racist and Blacks are lazy.
IQ gap among races
The modern controversy surrounding intelligence and race focuses on the results of IQ studies conducted during the second half of the 20th century mainly in the United States and some other industrialized nations. IQ studies outside these nations are few and small. It is uncertain what the average IQ or subgroup IQ tests scores would be with more complete studies in the developing world. IQ test scores in the developing world may be affected by factors less important in the developed world such as nutritional deficiencies. Most of the following article refers to studies attempting to explain race differences in IQ test scores in the US and do not refer to the world as a whole.
IQ gap in the US
In almost every testing situation where tests were administered and evaluated correctly, a difference of approximately one standard deviation was observed in the US between the mean IQ score of Blacks and Whites. In the United States, the mean IQ score among Blacks is approximately 85 and the mean IQ score among Whites is approximately 100; the mean IQ score of Hispanics is usually reported to be between the mean Black and White scores (Herrnstein and Murray report a mean "Latino" IQ of 89 in The Bell Curve). The mean score for people of East Asian and Jewish descent is usually higher than the mean score of Whites, but the extent of that difference is not precisely known. However, several studies place the median IQ of Ashkenazi Jews (who make up the overwhelming majority of American Jews) at approximately one standard deviation above the mean for other Whites, with the primary Jewish advantage in verbal reasoning and the East Asian advantage primarily in spatial reasoning. In The Bell Curve, Herrnstein and Murray report mean IQ scores for East Asians and Jewish Americans of 106 and 113, respectively.
Similar gaps are seen in other tests of cognitive ability or aptitude, including university admission exams such as the SAT and GRE as well as employment tests for corporate settings and the military (Roth et al. 2001).
Is the gap closing?
Richard Nisbett and others have argued that the Black-White gap on various ability tests has narrowed from the 1970s to the 1990s (Grissmer, 1994; Grissmer, Flanagan, & Williamson, 1998; Grissmer, Williamson, Kirby, & Berends, 1998; Hedges & Nowell, 1998; Nisbett, 1995, 1998, 2005). These tests include the Equality of Educational Opportunity (EEO) survey, the National Longitudinal Study, the High School and Beyond survey, the National Education Longitudinal Study, and the National Assessment of Educational Progress program (NAEP).
The Journal of Blacks in Higher Education found that although the Black-White gap on the SAT declined from 1976-1988, it has been increasing since 1988. On the other hand, some studies find that the gap has been decreasing for the most of the 20th century and that this continued during the nineties. .
Jensen (1998 pp. 375-376, 407-408, 494-495) has argued that the Black-White differences in g have not narrowed. In support of this claim, he presents evidence that, while there have been gains in measures of acquired competency such as scholastic achievement, these improvements do not indicate gains in g. Jensen also argues that Black-White differences in g seen in measures of reaction time have not narrowed.
A meta-analysis by Roth et al. (2001) found a mean Black-White score difference of 1.1 standard deviations (6,246,729 samples; ranging from 0.38 to 1.46 depending on the g loading of the test). As to whether the IQ gap is narrowing, they speculated that any reduction was "either small, potentially a function of sampling error ... or nonexistent for highly g loaded" tests (Roth et al. 2001).
Gottfredson (2005) agreed that the Black-White gap observed in the National Assessment of Educational Progress test has narrowed from 1.07 to 0.89 standard deviations. However, she then argues that reduction stopped by the mid-1980s and is compatible with stable group differences in g.
A large (21,260 children) and probably the most recent (1998) study found that Black-White gap for young children in reading and math scores was much smaller than in earlier studies, and that all of the remaining difference could be explained by a few environmental factors. One possible explanation is that the Flynn effect started earlier for Whites but has now stopped while continuing for Blacks. Reading and math scores are correlated with, but not substitutable for, IQ, so these findings alone may not indicate convergence in the IQ gap. Still, the correlation of IQ with grades is highest in elementary school (0.6 to 0.7; Jensen 1998), so convergence in scores may, in fact, indicate that the IQ gap is narrowing.
IQ gaps in other nations
Attempted compilations of average IQ by race generally place East Asians at the top, followed by Whites, other Asians, Arabs and Blacks. See IQ and the Wealth of Nations for an attempted compilation of average IQ for different nations and a discussion of associated measurement problems.
The IQ scores vary greatly among different nations for the same group. Blacks in Africa score much lower than Blacks in the US. The black-white gap is much smaller in the UK than in the US . Another example is Jews who score much lower in developing nations and Koreans who score much lower in Japan.
There are many examples of IQ score differences between close neighbours in the same nation, for example between French vs. Flemish speakers in Belgium, Slovaks vs. Gypsies in Slovakia, Irish and Scottish vs. English in Great Britain, and white speakers of Afrikaans vs. white speakers of English in South Africa. The difference between the white neighbours Protestants and Catholics in Northern Ireland is as large as the differences between whites and blacks in the US .
Reaction time
In 1991, Richard Lynn tested 1,468 9-year old children consisting of Blacks from South Africa, East Asians from Hong Kong and Japan, and Whites from Britain and Ireland. The content of the tests involved flipping a switch after one or more lights came on. Lynn found that the decision times (the time taken to make a decision about what to do) had a low correlation with IQ data on Raven Progressive Matrices tests also administered during the same study, and that movement times (the time taken to execute the decision) did not show any correlation. He found that the Asians had the fastest decision times, followed by the Whites, and then by the Blacks. He also determined that the Black children had movement times that were substantially faster than those of Whites and Asians on certain tests. Studies have shown similar patterns in response time on tests of arithmetic (Jensen, 1993; Jensen and Whang, 1994).
Brain size
See also: Craniometry, brain size and intelligence
Group differences in average IQ tend to mirror group differences in brain size. Numerous historical and modern studies, using skull and head measurements, weighing of brains at autopsy, and more recently, magnetic resonance imaging report racial differences. These studies are usually accompanied by a great deal of controversy.
In his 1839 Crania Americana, anthropologist Samuel George Morton reported that the mean cranial capacity of the skulls of Whites was 87 in³, while that of Blacks was 78 in³. Based on the measurement of 144 skulls of Native Americans, he reported an a figure of 82in³.
Morton's work has been criticized by Stephen Jay Gould, who alleged in his 1981 book The Mismeasure of Man that Morton was guilty of fudging data and "overpacking" the skulls with filler. Despite Gould's retabulation of Morton's data, however, the differences in brain size among different races still persist, with the data still showing a difference of about four cubic inches between modern Caucasians and Africans. Gould writes that the differences are "trivial", but J. Philippe Rushton (1996) responds that a difference of only a single cubic inch equates to millions of neurons.
In 1988, J. S. Michael remeasured a random sample of Morton's skulls and concluded that Morton had made very few errors. J. Philippe Rushton (1989) additionally reanalyzed Gould's retabulation, concluding that Morton had shown a pattern of decreasing brain size proceeding from East Asians, Europeans, and Africans.
In 1873, Paul Pierre Broca found the same pattern by weighing brains at autopsy. Other historical studies showing a Black-White difference in brain size include Bean (1906), Mall, (1909), Pearl, (1934) and Vint (1934).
In his controversial 1995 work Race, Evolution, and Behavior, J. Philippe Rushton reported an average endocranial volume of 1,415 cm³ for "Orientals ", 1,362 for Whites, and 1,268 for Blacks. When adjusted for average body size, the differences become more pronounced; i.e., the encephalization quotients (EQ) display greater differences than do absolute brain sizes (Jerisen, 1973, 2000; Rushton, 1991). Rushton (1991) found an EQ of 7.26 for East Asians as compared to 6.76 for Caucasians. Differences in brain size between Asians and Europeans sometimes do not appear until adjusted for body size (Rushton, 1997).
Other studies that have shown similar patterns in average brain size include Ho et al. (1980), who measured brains at autopsy, and Beals et al. (1984), who measured approximately 20,000 skulls, finding the same East Asian → European → African pattern. Other studies have shown the same pattern in average head size, including Rushton (1992), Rushton (1994), and Broman, Nichols, Shaugnessy, & Kennedy (1987) who measured the head circumferences and IQs of 50,000 children. Broman et al. found that the East Asian → European → African pattern in both cranial volume and IQ held as early as 4 months of age as well as at 1 year and 7 years of age.
Rushton and Ankney (2000) found the same pattern in head breadth and height relative to head width as well as the reverse pattern in prognathism, glabella size, postorbital constriction, and temporal fossae.
Modern studies using MRI imaging have revealed similar results (Harvey, Persaud, Ron, Baker, & Murray, 1994) and have shown that brain size correlates with IQ by a factor of roughly .35 to .40. In 1991, Willerman et al. used data from 40 White American university students and reported a correlation coefficient of .35. Other studies done on samples of Caucasians show similar results, with Andreasen et al (1993) determining a correlation of .38, while Raz et al (1993) obtained a figure of .43 and Wickett et al (1994) obtained a figure of .40. The correlation between brain size and IQ seems to hold for comparisons between and within families (Gignac et al. 2003; Jensen 1994; Jensen & Johnson 1994). However, one study found no within family correlation (Schoenemann et al. 2000). A study on twins (Thompson et al., 2001) showed that frontal gray matter volume was correlated with g and highly heritable. A related study has reported that the correlation between brain size (reported to have a heritability of 0.85) and g is 0.4, and that correlation is mediated entirely by genetic factors (Posthuma et al 2002).
The American Psychological Association's Task Force Report on Intelligence reports that with respect to "racial differences in the mean measured sizes of skulls and brains (with East Asians having the largest, followed by Whites and then Blacks)...there is indeed a small overall trend" (Neisser, 1997, p. 80).
Cranial vault size and shape have changed greatly during the last 150 years in the US. These changes must occur by early childhood because of the early development of the vault. The explanation for these changes may be related to the Flynn effect.
Interpretations
Test bias
It has been suggested that IQ tests may be biased against minorities, and that this accounts for part or all of the IQ gap. Some claim that there is no evidence for test bias. IQ tests are equally good predictors of IQ-related factors (such as school performance) for Blacks and Whites. The performance differences persist in tests and testing situations in which care has been taken to eliminate bias. It has also been suggested that IQ tests are formulated in such a way as to disadvantage minorities. Controlled studies have shown that test construction does not substantially contribute to the IQ gap.
The lack of test bias is widely accepted in the research community. From the American Psychological Association's summary of their 1996 task force report, "Intelligence: Knowns and Unknowns": "The differential between the mean intelligence test scores of Blacks and Whites does not result from any obvious biases in test construction and administration, nor does it simply reflect differences in socio-economic status." From The Wall Street Journal: Mainstream Science on Intelligence (PDF): "Intelligence tests are not culturally biased against American Blacks or other native-born, English-speaking people in the U.S. Rather, IQ scores predict equally accurately for all such Americans, regardless of race or social class."
Since the U.S. Supreme Court outlawed employee selection, including testing, which is "fair in form, but discriminatory in operation" (Griggs v. Duke Power Co., 1971; see this page on disparate impact), American companies have had a strong incentive to construct valid tests which do not produce an IQ gap between ethnic groups, called "selection bias" in employment. Despite this incentive, these efforts have generally failed. For example, in one case regarding a police selection test in Nassau County, New York, a scandal ensued when tests which showed no "selection bias" (Black-White score gap) were found to have been denuded of their ability to measure intelligence (Gottfredson, 2003, pp. 24-26 PDF).
Motivation
One environmental source of the IQ gap which has been suggested is poor motivation among low scorers. This hypothesis is seemingly discredited by findings promoted by the researcher Arthur Jensen (1998) using elementary cognitive tasks to measure intelligence. For example, one such test asks the subject to lift a finger from a depressed button to strike a light when it flashes. When more than one light is offered as a target the task involves a decision of which to hit (i.e. the one which is lit). These tests measure both reaction time (from when the bulb illuminates to when the subject lifts their finger) and movement time (from when the subject lifts their finger to when the subject reaches the bulb). While movement time measurements show no difference (or an advantage to Blacks), reaction time measurements negatively correlate with IQ scores and show the same performance gaps between those two races. Jensen argues that it is difficult to imagine that people could be motivated during one part of each segment of the test but not motivated during the other. The correlation between IQ and reaction time is low (from .20 to .40). A review by Deary (2000) that combined several studies with six measures produced a multiple correlation of reaction time to IQ of .67. This correlation is within the range of correlations between different kinds of IQ tests.
Socio-economic factors
Family income and parental education are positively correlated with SAT scores. However, the score gap between races persists at all levels of family income and parental education. These data are interpreted by some researchers to indicate that socio-economic factors cannot explain the score gap. For example, in 1995 Black students from the highest income group have lower average SAT scores than White students from the lowest income group. However, the stability of the gap across socio-economic groups has suggested alternative explanations:
- Some argue that Blacks are discriminated against such that they must have a higher or at least equal intelligence in order to achieve the same socioeconomic status (SES) as Whites. One should then expect that Black children should have a higher or equal IQ compared to children from Whites with the same SES. That they score lower on SAT and IQ tests can thus be interpreted as evidence for strong adverse influence from environmental factors different from SES or from SES factors other than income and parental education.
- It is possible that minorities achieve higher SES with lower intelligence; perhaps by having (on average) greater amounts of a compensating character, or thru affirmative action.
- Stereotype threat has been suggested to explain the poor performance of minorities on the SAT.
- Another alternative explanation is that by comparing the SES of parents to the intelligence of their children, the score gap shown here reflects regression towards different average racial scores from one generation to the next; which has been interpreted as evidence for a genetic contribution to the gap.]]
IQ is correlated with economic factors. Blacks and Hispanics suffer poorer economic conditions than Whites. It has been suggested that the effects of poverty are responsible for some or all of the IQ gap. However, some argue that economics cannot be the whole explanation. First, the gaps are slightly smaller but still persist for individuals from the same socioeconomic backgrounds. (For counter-argument see figure) Second, except for extreme environments, some argue that factors associated with poverty account for little of the variance in IQ scores. (Some studies claim to prove that the socioeconomic environment completely "overrides" the race factor, at least temporarily, in adopted Black children (e.g. Capron and Duyme, 1989). Third, some scientists believe that IQ determines income in developed nations, and not the other way around (Murray, 1998). (Even if true, this is not evidence that IQ is genetic since there are many other potential environmental factors beside income)
Other researchers have come across what they see as additional reasons for the IQ gap. The paper Poverty and Brain Development in Early Childhood holds that there is a large amount of neural damage in many American Black and Hispanic children due to inadequate nutrition, substance abuse of the children's parents, a high incidence of maternal depression, exposure to environmental toxins, psychological trauma, and the neural effects of physical abuse.
Researchers have found that many American Blacks and Hispanics are not given sufficient opportunity to learn language and thinking skills during the first three years of life, possibly due to economic status. The first three years are especially critical years for neural development of the brain, and previous studies have shown that when human children were deprived of most or all language skills at an early age, they never developed the ability to master language at a later age; if they only mastered a small amount of language and thinking skills at a young age, then they could only make small improvements in later years. A recent study has shown that many American Blacks and Hispanics are raised in homes where their parents speak relatively few sentences, and the sentences usually show only simple grammar. As a result, their children never hear millions of words during the time when their brains are developing linguistic skills. Without this linguistic input during their developing years, many are observed to quickly fall behind, and they can never catch up. Children in poorer welfare families, which includes a higher percentage of many minority populations, apparently hear up to 30 million fewer words by age three than children in higher income, usually White, families. (Source: The Early Catastrophe: The 30 Million Word Gap by Age 3)
The recent paper Socioeconomic status modifies heritability of IQ in young children finds that the role of the environment is more important in poorer families. "The models suggest that in impoverished families, 60% of the variance in IQ is accounted for by the shared environment, and the contribution of genes is close to zero; in affluent families, the result is almost exactly the reverse."
Cultural explanations
Many anthropologists have argued that intelligence is a cultural category; some cultures emphasize speed and competition more than others, for example. During WWI African-Americans from the north tested higher than those from the south. This could be because African-Americans in the north had received more formal education (see Race: Science and Politics, written by Ruth Benedict in 1940). Thousands of ethnographic studies indicate that innate capacities for cultural evolution are equal among all human populations. The American Anthropological Association has endorsed a statement deriding all studies of race and intelligence .
It has been suggested that Black culture disfavors academic achievement and fosters an environment that is damaging to IQ (Boykin, 1994). Likewise, it is argued that a persistence of racism reinforces this negative effect. John Ogbu (1978, 1994) has developed a hypothesis that the condition of being a "caste-like minority" affects motivation and achievement, depressing IQ. Even proponents of the view that the IQ gap is caused partly by genetic differences, such as Arthur Jensen, recognize that non-genetic factors are likely involved. Indeed, one author has compiled a list of over one hundred possible causes of the Black-White IQ gap .
Cultural explanations for the IQ deficit among Blacks and Hispanics compared to Whites and Asian minorities are complemented – and sometimes challenged – by the observation that Asian minorities score well on IQ tests and on average enjoy greater economic success than other minorities. Along these lines, East Asians are sometimes referred to as "model minorities". Likewise, Jewish populations have suffered past discrimination and persecution, but do not exhibit an IQ deficit. However, Jews and East Asians are today less discriminated than Blacks.
Black mothers are known to breastfeed infants less and for a shorter time than White mothers. Studies have shown IQ gains lasting into adulthood with increased duration of breastfeeding.
Language
Some argue that the higher IQ test scores in East Asian nations are in part attributed to some IQ tests' inherent bias towards testing spatial reasoning. They argue that logographic writing systems like those used by Chinese or Japanese develop spatial reasoning better than the alphabetic writing systems prevalent in Europe and America. The same reasoning has been used to explain why students from Asia-Pacific countries (e.g., Singapore, South Korea) tend to score better than average in tests of mathematics. Some argue that the East Asian advantage can also be explained by more rigorous education programs.
The Flynn effect
The Flynn effect consists of large documented worldwide increases in IQ scores for at least several decades. Attempted explanations have included improved nutrition, a trend towards smaller families, better education, greater environmental complexity, and heterosis.
Some think that there are no genetic differences and that the Flynn effect will eliminate differences in IQ test scores in the future. They argue that the Flynn effect started sooner and will end sooner for the more affluent parts of society and that Blacks sooner or later will close the gap.
However, comparing the Flynn effect (IQ differences within races over time) to contemporary IQ differences between races is contested; for example, one report concludes "that the nature of the Flynn effect is qualitatively different from the nature of B-W differences in the United States" (Wicherts et al., 2004). Others note that a racial component is not mentioned in the abstract, that the above statement refers to "measurement invariance", and that it is not a statement about the role of genetics in the B-W gap.
Genetics
Part of the gap may well be genetic; there is no a priori reason to believe that every ethnic group or race has precisely the same distribution of genes that affect intelligence; a small amount of random variation early in human evolution may have later crystallized into differences seen today. Also there might have been smaller evolutionary pressure towards greater intelligence in some environments.
The genetic hypothesis is often ignored or disregarded in primary research on group differences. It has been well-studied by researchers doing meta-analyses that combine multiple sources of primary materials.
Arthur Jensen has concluded that the IQ gap is at least partly genetic. Jensen and colleagues reach this conclusion based on an evaluation of some of the following evidence. While accepting that some fraction of the supporting evidence may be false, they argue that the partly-genetic hypothesis is favored over the culture-only hypothesis by a preponderance of the evidence (Rushton and Jensen 2005; Gottfredson 2005). In this view, average intelligence differences among races are like average skin color differences: a product of different allelic frequencies within each population. Others are critical of Jensen's methods and evaluation (Sternberg 2005; Suzuki & Aronson 2005; Nisbett 2005).
The results of most (indirect) analyses used to test the genetic hypothesis do not logically contradict an environmental explanation of the lower IQ of Blacks. That is, a plausible (but some argue ad hoc) environmental explanation for the lower mean IQ in Blacks can be offered in most cases. However, many argue that the higher average IQ of East Asians than Whites is anomalous for an environment-only theory of IQ differences (Rushton & Jensen, 2005).
Within-group heritability
The heritability of intelligence within groups is high. It is widely recognized that within-group heritability does not in itself indicate that between-group differences are genetic in origin, although it is likely a necessary condition. Different kinds of evidence are needed to address the question of between-group heritability. As Herrnstein and Murray explain in The Bell Curve:
- As we discussed in Chapter 4, scholars accept that IQ is substantially heritable, somewhere between 40 and 80 percent, meaning that much of the observed variation in IQ is genetic. And yet this tells us nothing for sure about the origin of the differences between races in measured intelligence. This point is so basic, and so commonly misunderstood, that it deserves emphasis: That a trait is genetically transmitted in individuals does not mean that group differences in that trait are also genetic in origin. Anyone who doubts this assertion may take two handfuls of genetically identical seed corn and plant one handful in Iowa, the other in the Mojave Desert, and let nature (i.e., the environment) take its course. The seeds will grow in Iowa, not in the Mojave, and the result will have nothing to do with genetic differences. (Herrnstein & Murray, The Bell Curve, 1994, p. 298.)
In most studies, measured heritabilities for intelligence are the same for Blacks as for Whites. A 1975 review by Loehlin et al.. found some evidence suggesting lower heritability in Blacks than Whites (e.g., Scarr-Salapatek, 1971), but a larger body of evidence suggested equal heritabilities for both races. An analysis of the Georgia Twin Study by Osborne (1980) found equal heritabilities for both Blacks and Whites.
Two kinds of environmental effects can be distinguished: shared and nonshared effects (see nature versus nurture). Twin and adoption studies, used to measure heritability, can also be used to quantify the two types of environmental effects (Plomin, DeFries, & Loehlin, 1977).
Shared environmental effects are due to factors experienced in common by all children raised in the same family but that differ among families. Examples of shared environmental effects include socio-economic factors, family cultural practices, and parental influences on children.
Nonshared effects are unique for each child, and thus differ among families. Examples include chance events such as accidents, illness, and childhood friends. Anything that happens to one sibling and not to the other contributes to nonshared effects.
McGue et al. (1993) found that the nonshared environmental effects on IQ remain approximately constant throughout life, shared environmental effects remain approximately constant until 20 years of age but then drop to zero in adulthood, and genetic factors increase throughout development but especially after 20 years of age. Plomin et al. (2001) corroborates these results. Environmental factors usually proposed to explain the Black-White gap are shared effects (e.g. social class, religion, cultural practices, father absence, and parenting styles). Jensen (1997) argues that because these effects account for little variance within a race, they are unlikely to account for the differences among races in developed nations. Others studies do support that shared environmental factors in developed nations can affect IQ .
Spearman's hypothesis
Intelligence as measured by g as defined by the g theory, the in-contention "general factor of cognitive ability", and its various biological correlates (e.g., the volume of gray matter in the frontal cortex) are claimed to be partly genetically determined. g has the highest measured heritability of any cognitive ability factor. Jensen formulated a hypothesis now referred to as Spearman's hypothesis which states that the degree of difference between black and white cognitive test scores will be correlated with the degree of the test's g-loading. Spearman's hypothesis has a strong form, which says that all test-score differences can be traced to g, and a weak form, which claims that some but not all differences are due to g.
Jensen found that black-white cognitive test score differences and test g-loadings correlate with a correlation coefficient of 0.6 (Jensen, 1998), and concluded that the weak form of Spearman's hypothesis was thus confirmed. Jensen's study combined scores on 149 psychometric tests obtained from 15 independent samples totaling 43,892 Blacks and 243,009 Whites (Jensen, 1998). Dolan and Hamaker (2001) have reanalyzed the data from several previous studies (Jensen & Reynolds, 1982; Naglieri & Jensen, 1987) that used the statistical method invented by Jensen (the method of correlated vectors) with a more recent and improved method (multigroup confirmatory factor analysis). Their results statistically were consistent with the weak form of Spearman's hypothesis that black-white group differences were predominantly on the g factor. However, their analysis of the data set failed to "establish Spearman's hypothesis as an empirically established fact". They also speculate that "it is possible that the analysis of all available data sets ... will demonstrate that a model incorporating the weak version of Spearman's hypothesis provides the best description of the data." . This leaves the validity of Spearman's hypothesis, considered a central justification for the genetic explanation, an unresolved question.
Gene-environment interactions
Minority-specific effects on intelligence arising from cultural background differences between the races would be expected to affect the correlations between the measures of environmental background variables and outcome measures. Rowe et al. (1994) compared cross-sectional correlation matrices using both independent variables (e.g., home environment, peer characteristics) and developmental outcomes (e.g., achievement, delinquency). Rowe et al. (1995) compared correlations between academic achievement and family environment. They found that the covariance matrix of each group were equal. That is, they failed to find evidence for distortions in the correlations between the background variables and the outcome measures that would suggest a minority-specific developmental factor.
Similarly, Carretta (1995), Owen (1992), and Rushton et al. (2000, 2002, 2003) found nearly identical statistical structure on psychometric variables in each group. The factor structure of cognitive ability is nearly identical for Blacks and for Whites; there were no race-specific factors.
Using structural equation modeling Rowe and Cleveland (1996) estimated the genetic architecture for Black and White siblings. They found that the best-fitting model for the source of differences between and within races was the same: both genetic and environmental factors. Jensen (1998b, p. 465) reanalyzed a subset of this data. This analysis found that the Black-White IQ difference was best explained by a model of both genetic and environmental factors, and that the genetic-only and the environmental-only models were inadequate.
Nichols (1972) using differential heritabilities among Blacks and Whites and later Rushton (1989) using inbreeding depression calculated in Japan found that the Black-White gap is least on IQ subtests most affected by the environment, and greatest on subtests that are least affected by the environment. It is difficult to attribute the relationship between inbreeding depression from Japan with the Black-White IQ gap in the U.S. to an environmental (not-genetic) cause.
These results have not been thoroughly retested.
Comparison of interpretations
The Black-White IQ gap in the U.S. may be explained by a variety of interpretations. In order to differentiate between alternatives, additional evidence is needed. The "partly-genetic" and "culture-only" interpretations offer contrasting points of view on this evidence.
Partly genetic | Culture only |
---|---|
Black-White-Asian differences in IQ, reaction time, brain size and other physiological variables in the United States and a few other developed countries (e.g. UK, Japan). Larger brain size and higher IQ of East Asians than Whites is a challenge for the culture-only theory. | The gaps may be explained by many other factors except genetics (see above). Differences among some White groups are as large as the difference between Whites and Blacks in the US. Many racial groups show great variation when tested at different times and in different places, indicating large environmental influences. The IQ scores and larger brain size of East Asians have numerous possible explanations, e.g. many East Asian nations have a very high consumption of fish. One study has shown larger head size at birth and higher IQ scores at 4 years of age when the mothers took fish oil supplement during pregnancy and lactation . |
Black-White-Asian differences in culture-fair and reaction-time IQ test scores exist world-wide despite international differences in social, cultural, and economic conditions. Higher IQ scores among East Asians (living in East and South Asia) than Whites (living in North American and Europe) is a challenge for culture-only theories because standards of living in Asia are lower than or equal to those in North America or Europe. | The only nationwide IQ tests have been done in a few developed countries, and the few studies in other nations have been severely criticized, see IQ and the Wealth of Nations. One argument of many against the reliability of the IQ scores in developing nations is that some such countries a majority of the population would be classified by the IQ scores as mentally retarded. A large proportion of the population should also be classified as moderately (<16%) and severely (<2%) mentally retarded. In the U.S., the moderately mentally retarded require moderate supervision and the severely mentally retarded often have other physical disabilities and may thus require constant supervision, be unable to provide for themselves, be unable to speak long sentences, and, in many cases, be unable to do things like getting dressed without help. |
The Black-White IQ gap in the US has remained constant at approximately one standard deviation since it was first measured in the early 1900s despite social and economic change during that time, including the civil rights movement and Brown v. Board of Education. Recent changes in skill test score gaps do not indicate changes in g. | Other studies show that the gap in the US is narrowing. For example, one large recent study found much smaller differences than earlier studies in math and reading skills in young children and found that all of the remaining differences could be explained by a few environmental factors. |
The IQ gaps remains approximately constant as individuals age. Some environmenal theories would predict a growing or shrinking IQ gap during development. | Jensen (1998) reports some evidence of decreasing IQ among African American boys at adolescence. |
Correlations between an IQ subtest's heritability or inbreeding depression and the magnitude of the Black-White-Asian score gap for that subtest. Environmental theories would predict the opposite. | What subtest are most heritable and how to measure this is debated. |
Correlations between an IQ subtest's g-loading, and the magnitude of the Black-White-Asian score gap for that subtest. | g-loading and the method of correlated vectors, the statistical method used in many older studies, has been criticized heavily in recent research as discussed previously. |
Rising heritability of IQ with age, and decreasing shared-family effects (e.g., socioeconomic factors) on IQ after adolescence. An environmental cause of the IQ gap would necessarily be a shared family effect. Lack of shared family effects on adult IQ is a challenge to culture only theories. | High within-group heritability does not logically exclude the all environmental interpretation. |
Studies suggesting that IQ heritability and gene-environment interactions within races are the same for Blacks and Whites. No race-specific factors have been identified. The IQ gap exists even among middle- and upper-class Black and White families where within-race heritabilities are high and shared family effects are near zero. | Many older studies have only studied middle class families but SES has recently been shown to be relatively more important in poorer families. |
The finding that when Black and White children are matched for IQ, their siblings tend to have IQs that regress towards different means (85 for Blacks and 100 for Whites). For example, among Black and White children matched with an IQ of 120, the siblings of the Black children have an average IQ of 100 whereas the siblings of the White children have an average IQ of 110. This is a stronger test of the party-genetic hypothesis than regression from parents to offspring because siblings share a similar environment (Jensen, 1973). This is a novel prediction of the partly genetic hypothesis. | Regression towards the mean only shows that mean IQ scores are different which is not a new finding. That is not evidence that the cause of this difference is genetic. |
American Blacks have a lower average IQ than Hispanic and Native American groups, which are more socio-economically deprived. For example, the Inuit, who live in the Arctic, have higher average IQs than North American Blacks (Berry 1966; MacArthur 1968) despite being extremely poor (Vernon 1965, 1979). | That Blacks are less socioeconomically deprived than Hispanics or Native Americans in the US is controversial. The Inuit cannot be directly compared to the US population, for example they have substantially different nutrition from eating large amounts of fish. |
The three-way differences in the IQ and SAT scores of children persists even after controlling for parental income or education, which seems to counter arguments that the gap is due to socioeconomic conditions. In addition, some researchers have argued from studies in siblings that IQ affects socioeconomic status, rather than the other way around. | Adjustments for socioeconomic conditions almost completely eliminate differences in IQ scores between black and white children. The remaining difference is statistically insignificant. . SAT scores are not the same as IQ scores. |
Ashkenazi Jews have often been persecuted and discriminated against, but they still display the highest average IQ of any ethnic group, as well as SAT scores higher than those of non-Jewish Caucasians. This seems to counter arguments that depressed IQ scores of African Americans are due to discrimination or prejudice. | Persecution of and discrimination against Jews was strongest in the past, while Blacks are still being discriminated against in various ways today. |
The three-way difference in average IQ can be measured in very young children. For example, a one standard deviation gap is observed in Black and White 3-year olds matched for gender, birth order, and maternal education (Peoples, Fagan, & Drotar, 1995). Lynn (1996) found that by age 6 the average IQ of East Asian children is 107, 103 for White children and 89 for Black children. Broman et al. (1987) found that the same trichotomy in brain size and IQ held at 4 months, 1 year, and 7 years of age. | Environmental factors can affect very young children, for example nutrition by the mother during pregnancy and breastfeeding. More breastfeeding gives IQ gains and the duration of this is known to differ between White and Black mothers. |
Three-way differences in reaction times have been demonstrated, and it is difficult to explain differences in reaction time through lack of motivation or cultural differences on the part of the subjects. | Differences in reaction time or brain volume may be caused by environmental factors. As noted, there have been large changes in cranial vault size and shape during the last century in the US for both Black and Whites, far beyond what can be explained genetically. |
Average Black-White-Asian differences in IQ (both positive and negative) remain following transracial adoption. Three studies of Asian children adopted by White familes found that even if children were malnurished by school age they had had higher average IQ scores than Whites (Clark & Hanisee, 1982; Frydman & Lynn, 1989; Winick, et al., 1975.). The higher average IQ of adopted Asian children is a challenge to the culture-only theory. | Several adoption studies finds no IQ difference. |
No studies of Black-White genetic admixture have been performed with the multi-locus DNA sequencing required to make reliable conclusions. | IQ have very low positive to low negative correlation with Whiteness of skin, degree of European blood groups, or self-reported degree of European ancestry among Blacks. |
No studies that make use of proper behavior genetic techniques have been able to identify environmental factors to explain the IQ gap. | A study which showed near-disappearance of the black-white gap among children of black and white servicemen raised by German mothers after World War II. Some, like the American Psychological Association, consider this study be strong evidence against the genetic explanation. |
One statistical analysis suggests that the Flynn effect is qualitatively different than the Black-White IQ gap (i.e. the B-W gap reflects differences in g but the Flynn effect does not). | One estimate is that the average IQ in the U.S. was below 75 before the Flynn effect started and it seems likely that the effect started earlier and may end sooner for Whites. |
A similar dichotomy in spatial/nonspatial intelligence test scores is present in both East Asians and several Native American and Inuit populations (Connelly, 1983; Diessner & Walker, 1986; Tempest, 1987; Zarske & Moore, 1982; McShane & Plas, 1982, 1984) . | Dichotomy in intelligence is entirely compatible with an all environmental explanation. East Asians may have higher spatial ability for example due to their knowledge of iconographic languages. |
Party-genetic theory is predicated on a model that the IQ gap has (the genetic) part of its origin in human evolution. Thus, it predicts that the Black-White-Asian differences in average IQ, reaction-time, and brain size should be accompanied by a similar pattern of differences in other inherited traits. Proponents cite three-way average differences in personality, maturation, and reproductive traits as support of this prediction. | Differences may have environmental causes and may be unrelated to one another. |
Other interpretations
The two most widely-known works concerning race and intelligence are The Mismeasure of Man by Stephen Jay Gould, originally published in 1981, and The Bell Curve by Richard Herrnstein and Charles Murray, published in 1994. Media controversy surrounding The Bell Curve motivated Gould to revise and expand The Mismeasure of Man to respond to arguments from The Bell Curve, publishing the book's second edition in 1996. Many current researchers think that both books are outdated due to new research.
A recent paper in the Psychological Review, "Heritability Estimates Versus Large Environmental Effects: The IQ Paradox Resolved" by William T. Dickens of The Brookings Institution and James R. Flynn presents a mechanism by which environmental effects on IQ may be magnified by feedback effects. This work may provide a resolution of the contradiction between the viewpoint of The Bell Curve's authors and the 'nurture' effects observed by others. A latter paper responded to objections .
Some cite research that they believe indicates that discriminated or lower-status minorities do tend to have lower IQ, some without apparent genetic differences. Like Blacks and Hispanics in the U.S., minorities in some societies show achievement gaps (such as the Maori in New Zealand, aboriginals in Australia, scheduled castes ("untouchables") in India, non-European Jews in Israel, and the Burakumin in Japan). The most prominent finding cited is that Northern Irish Catholics used to score about 15 points lower than Protestants. Similarly, Irish, Italian and Polish immigrants in the U.S. are reported to have all scored about 80 in the beginning of the 19th century, but now tend to reach 100. The same is true of persons from rural versus urban areas in general (see e.g. this article by conservative columnist and economist Thomas Sowell and this page on European and Greek IQ. More arguments of the kind are to be found here).
In 1974, an English biologist named John Randal Baker presented a lengthy argument in favor of innate racial differences in intelligence in a volume entitled "Race" (Oxford University Press, 1974), in which he claimed there had been a systematic, politically motivated suppression of classical biological anthropology outside Germany since the 1930s, and went on to attempt to demonstrate a relation among five historical civilizations (Sumerian, Egyptian, Indus valley, Helladic-Minoan and Sinic) and the supposed biological dispositions of their creators.
In his 1995 work Race, Evolution, and Behavior, J. Philippe Rushton argued that racial differences in IQ, as well as a number of other racial differences, could be explained by different degrees of evolution to either r-selection or K-selection. All humans are extremely K-selected, he claims; although the human races differ in the extent to they are so. He posits that the comparatively cold and harsh environment of Europe caused the evolution of those who migrated there slightly more to a K-selected pattern than those who remained in Africa, and the even harsher environment of Northeastern Asia forced the evolution of East Asians to an even higher level of K-selective behavior. This theory has been severely criticized .
Richard Lynn has developed similar theories and argues that the ice age that took place in East Asia from about 28,000 to 12,000 years ago acted as a selection force on East Asians to increase intelligence by requiring the building of shelter, making clothes, and making fires, and selected especially strongly for spatial skills such as those needed to hunt large prey and build the tools necessary to do so. (Lynn 1991). Rushton (1996) has cited the fact that the order in Blacks, Whites, and East Asians appeared is the same as the order of their respective brain sizes as additional evidence. This theory, however, has difficulty explaining why Native Americans, who appeared even later and emigrated from the northernmost parts of Asia, do not currently have high scores on IQ tests. On the other hand, Rushton (1995) argues that lower scores of Native Americans can be attributed to the evolutionary relaxation of cognitive demands due to the more temperate environment and comparative ease with which North American fauna could be hunted. But it can be argued that life along the fertile river plains in China was not particularly harsh. It is also questionable that conditions in deserts are no less harsh but people living there do not currently score high on IQ tests.
Opinions of scholars
A survey performed in the 1980s of a broad sample of 1,020 scholars in specialties that would give them reason to be knowledgeable about IQ asked, "Which of the following best characterizes your opinion of the heritability of the Black-White difference in I.Q.?" (emphasis original). The responses were divided into five categories:
- The difference is entirely due to environmental variation: 15%.
- The difference is entirely due to genetic variation: 1% (8 respondents).
- The difference is a product of both genetic and environmental variation: 45%.
- The data are insufficient to support any reasonable opinion: 24%.
- No response: 14%.
No single response was endorsed by more than half of those surveyed. In general, this could be considered as evidence that no consensus opinion exists on the cause of the Black-White IQ gap. However, the survey is old and it is unclear what specialties are represented.
IQ, race, and public policy
There is substantial overlap in the distribution of IQ scores among individuals of each race. Jensen (1998, p. 357) estimates that in a random sample of equal numbers of US Blacks and Whites, most of variance in IQ would be unrelated to race or social class. However, the appearance of a large practical importance for intelligence makes some scholars claim that the source and meaning of the IQ gap is a pressing social concern. Two statistical effects interact to exasperate IQ differences. First, there seem to be minimum statistical thresholds of IQ for many socially valued outcomes (e.g., high school graduation and college admission). Second, because of the shape of the normal distribution, only about 16% of the population is at least one standard deviation above the mean. Thus, although the IQ distributions for Blacks and Whites are largely overlapping, different IQ thresholds can have a significant impact on the proportion of Blacks and Whites above and below a particular cut-off. (See IQ for a discussion of its practical importance.)
IQ range | Whites | Blacks | Black:White ratio | Training prospects | High school dropout | Lives in poverty | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
<75 | 3.6% | 18.0% | ~5:1 | simple, supervised work; eligible for government assistance | 55% | 30% | |
<90 | 21.9% | 59.4% | ~2:1 | very explicit hands on training; IQ >80 for military training; no government assistance | 35% | 16% | |
>100 | 53.8% | 15.7% | ~1:3 | written material plus experience | 6% | 6% | |
>110 | 27.9% | 3.8% | ~1:7 | college format | 0.4% | 3% | |
>125 | 5.4% | 0.2% | ~1:32 | independent, self-teaching | 0% | 2% | |
Based on Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale IQs for Whites (mean = 101.4, SD = 14.7) and for Blacks (mean = 86.9, SD = 13.0) from (Reynolds, Chastain, Kaufman, & McLean, 1987, p. 330). Significance data is from Herrnstein & Murray (1994). Note that correlation is not causation. For example poverty can be both a cause and consequence of low IQ. |
Real-world outcomes
The IQ gap is reflected by gaps in the academic, economic, and social factors correlated with IQ (Gordon 1997; Gottfredson 1997). For example, Murray (1998) argues from IQ variation among siblings (i.e. within families, thus controlling for the impact of socio-economic variation) that IQ has a very large effect on life outcomes .
However, IQ is only one of several factors that are important for economic outcomes. Personality, for example, and especially conscientiousness, may play a large role in economic outcomes. Some scholars dispute the importance of IQ test score results for real-world achievement. Some argue that the role of IQ gradually becomes weaker with age and after school. They cite evidence that IQ is correlated with early school grades but other factors explain most of the variance. Regarding economic inequality, one study found that if we could magically give everyone identical IQs, we would still see 90 to 95 percent of the inequality we see today. . Another study found that wealth, race and schooling are important to the inheritance of economic status, but IQ is not a major contributor and the genetic transmission of IQ is even less important . Some argue that IQ scores are used as an excuse for not trying to reduce poverty or otherwise improve living standards for all. Claimed low intelligence has historically been used to justify the feudal system and unequal treatment of women.
Differences in intelligence have been used to explain differences in economic growth between nations. On example is IQ and the Wealth of Nations. The book, which has not been peer-reviewed, is sharply criticized in the peer-reviewed paper The Impact of National IQ on Income and Growth . It has been proposed that proportion of population who have high IQ score on verbal subtests are particularly important, this to explain the underachivement compared to IQ scores of East Asian nations .
Achievement in science may be more closely associated with IQ than for example income, which may be greatly influence by inherited wealth, other personality characteristics, or even physical characteristics such as athletic ability or beauty. Only 0.25% of the world population are Jews, but 20-30% of all Nobel prize winners in physics, chemistry, and medicine are Jewish . A large scale decline in the number of Nobel prized awarded to Europe and similar increase in the number of prized awarded to the U.S. occurred at the same time as Nazi persecutions of Jews during the 1930's and the Holocaust during the 1940's .
Policy implications
The public policy implications of IQ and race research are possibly the greatest source of controversy surrounding this issue. For example, the conservative policy recommendations of Herrnstein and Murray in The Bell Curve were denounced by many. Indeed, even proponents of a partly-genetic interpretation of the IQ gap such as Rushton and Jensen (2005) and Gottfredson (2005) argue that their interpretation does not in itself demand any particular policy response. They argue instead that the primary determinant of policy conclusions from race and intelligence research will be political philosophy itself.
For example, while a conservative/libertarian commentator like Charles Murray may call for reductions in affirmative action, a liberal commentator may use the same evidence to argue from a Rawlsian point of view (that genetic advantages are undeserved and unjust) for substantial and permanent affirmative action (Gottfredson, 2005). This point of view was summarized in the "Mainstream Science on Intelligence" statement published in the Wall Street Journal in 1994:
- The research findings neither dictate nor preclude any particular social policy, because they can never determine our goals. They can, however, help us estimate the likely success and side-effects of pursuing those goals via different means.
Summary
The source of and meaning of the IQ gap is not known. Many theories have been proposed, but none are generally accepted. Most of the theories are supported by only indirect evidence. The cause may be environmental. Many attribute the difference primarily to cultural factors that disadvantage caste-like minorities. Many researchers in the field of intelligence suggest that the difference is partially genetic and partially environmental. Other observers insist that the differences may be entirely environmental. The cause of the IQ gap may be identical to the cause of IQ differences between all individuals, or it may represent a race-specific effect. This is an active area of research. In general, simple correlations cannot decide the role of genetics. Advanced statistical methods are instead used with hotly debated results.
Biological differences in brain volume and reaction time, which show low to moderate correlations with IQ, are not by themselves evidence for genetic differences. Even if these differences or the average IQ test score gap indicate a gap in actual intelligence, this may be due to environmental differences in factors such as nutrition during pregnancy or early childhood which may produce such differences without any genetic cause.
Because the cause of the IQ gap is ultimately an empirical question, it should be possible to resolve this question in the future. Irrefutable direct evidence is currently lacking and may continue to be so until intelligence is mapped to specific genes.
Most research has been done in the US and a few other developed nations. That research cannot directly be generalized to the world as a whole. Blacks in the US do not constitute a random sample from Africa, and environmental conditions differ among nations. IQ tests done in developing countries are likely to have been affected by conditions associated with poverty that are common in the developing world, such as nutritional deficiencies (for instance, iodine deficiency is known to affect intelligence) and the impact of diseases (e.g., HIV, anemia or chronic parasites that may affect IQ test scores).
Finally, genetic engineering may soon be able to directly change the genetic determinants of intelligence. This change may make genetic intelligence and other genetic characteristics a matter of voluntary parental (or enforced governmental) decision. This could theoretically, in a single generation, dramatically increase human intelligence and make the current concept and discussion of race and possible associated characteristics obsolete.
See also
- Race
- Flynn effect
- Stanley Porteus
- Model minority
- Sex and intelligence
- Brain size and intelligence
- William Shockley
- Cyril Burt
References
- Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- Broca, P. (1873). Sur les crânes de la caverne de l’Homme Mort (Loere). Revue d’Anthropologie, 2, 1–53.
- Broman, S. H., Nichols, P. L., Shaughnessy, P. & Kennedy, W. (1987). Retardation in young children. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
- Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- Connelly, J. B. (1983). Recategorized WISC-R score patterns of older and younger referred Tlingit Indian children. Psychology in the Schools, 20, 271-275.
- Template:Journal reference
- . ISBN 019852417X.
{{cite book}}
: Missing or empty|title=
(help); Unknown parameter|Author=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help); Unknown parameter|Publisher=
ignored (|publisher=
suggested) (help); Unknown parameter|Title=
ignored (|title=
suggested) (help); Unknown parameter|Year=
ignored (|year=
suggested) (help) - Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- Diessner, R. & Walker, Jacqueline L. (1986). A cognitive pattern of the Yakima Indian students. Journal of American Indian Education 25 (2).
- Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- Dolan, C.V. and Hamaker, E.L. (2001). "Investigating black-white differences in psychometric IQ: Multi-group confirmatory factor analyses of the WISC-R and K-ABC, and a critique of the method of correlated vectors". In: Frank Columbus, Editor, Advances in Psychological Research vol. VI, pp. 31-60. Huntington, NY: Nova Science Publishers, Inc.
- . ISBN 0710006519.
{{cite book}}
: Missing or empty|title=
(help); Unknown parameter|Author=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help); Unknown parameter|Publisher=
ignored (|publisher=
suggested) (help); Unknown parameter|Title=
ignored (|title=
suggested) (help); Unknown parameter|Year=
ignored (|year=
suggested) (help) - Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- Gignac, G. E., Vernon, P. A., & Wickett, J. C. (2003). "Factors influencing the relationship between brain size and intelligence". In H. Nyborg (Ed.), The scientific study of general intelligence: Tribute to Arthur R. Jensen (pp. 93–106). London: Elsevier.
- Glazer, N. (1994, October 31). "The lying game." The New Republic, 15–16.
- Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference PDF
- Gottfredson, L. S. (2005). "Suppressing intelligence research: Hurting those we intend to help". In R. H. Wright & N. A. Cummings (Eds.), Destructive trends in mental health: The well-intentioned path to harm (pp. 155-186). New York: Taylor and Francis. Pre-print PDF PDF
- Template:Journal reference
- Gottfredson, L. S. (in press). "Social consequences of group differences in cognitive ability (Consequencias sociais das diferencas de grupo em habilidade cognitiva)". In C. E. Flores-Mendoza & R. Colom (Eds.), Introducau a psicologia das diferncas individuais. Porto Allegre, Brazil: ArtMed Publishers. PDF
- . ISBN 0833016164.
{{cite book}}
: Missing or empty|title=
(help); Unknown parameter|Author=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help); Unknown parameter|Publisher=
ignored (|publisher=
suggested) (help); Unknown parameter|Title=
ignored (|title=
suggested) (help); Unknown parameter|Year=
ignored (|year=
suggested) (help) - Grissmer, D., Flanagan, A., & Williamson, S. (1998). "Why did the Black-White score gap narrow in the 1970s and 1980s?" In M. Phillips (Ed.), The Black-White test score gap (pp. 182–226). Washington, DC: Brookings Institution.
- Grissmer, D., Williamson, S., Kirby, S. N., & Berends, M. (1998). "Exploring the rapid rise in Black achievement scores in the United States" (1970–1990). In U. Neisser (Ed.), The rising curve: Long-term changes in IQ and related measures (pp. 251–286). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
- Template:Journal reference
- Harvey, I., Persaud, R., Ron, M. A., Baker, G., & Murray, R. M. (1994). Volumetric MRI measurements in bipolars compared with schizophrenics and healthy controls. Psychological Medicine, 24, 689–699.
- Hedges, L., & Nowell, A. (1998). "Black-White test score convergence since 1965". In A. Phillips (Ed.), The Black-White test score gap (pp. 149–181). Washington, DC: Brookings Institution.
- . ISBN 0029146739.
{{cite book}}
: Missing or empty|title=
(help); Unknown parameter|Author=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help); Unknown parameter|Publisher=
ignored (|publisher=
suggested) (help); Unknown parameter|Title=
ignored (|title=
suggested) (help); Unknown parameter|Year=
ignored (|year=
suggested) (help) - Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- . ISBN 0060121947.
{{cite book}}
: Missing or empty|title=
(help); Unknown parameter|Author=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help); Unknown parameter|Publisher=
ignored (|publisher=
suggested) (help); Unknown parameter|Title=
ignored (|title=
suggested) (help); Unknown parameter|Year=
ignored (|year=
suggested) (help) - . ISBN 0029164303.
{{cite book}}
: Missing or empty|title=
(help); Unknown parameter|Author=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help); Unknown parameter|Publisher=
ignored (|publisher=
suggested) (help); Unknown parameter|Title=
ignored (|title=
suggested) (help); Unknown parameter|Year=
ignored (|year=
suggested) (help) - Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- Jensen, A. R. (1997). "The puzzle of nongenetic variance". In R. J. Sternberg & E. L. Grigorenko (Eds.), Intelligence, heredity, and environment (pp. 42–88). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
- Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- . ISBN 0275961036.
{{cite book}}
: Missing or empty|title=
(help); Unknown parameter|Author=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help); Unknown parameter|Publisher=
ignored (|publisher=
suggested) (help); Unknown parameter|Title=
ignored (|title=
suggested) (help); Unknown parameter|Year=
ignored (|year=
suggested) (help) - Jerison, H. J. (1973). Evolution of the brain and intelligence. New York: Academic Press.
- Jerison, H. J. (2000). The evolution of intelligence. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of intelligence (pp. 216–244). Cambridge: Cambridge University.
- Template:Journal reference
- Lynn, R. (1978). "Ethnic and racial differences in intelligence: International comparisons". In R. T. Osborne, C. E. Noble, & N. Weyl (Eds.), Human variation: The biopsychology of age, race, and sex (pp. 261–286). New York: Academic Press.
- Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- . ISBN 027597510X.
{{cite book}}
: Missing or empty|title=
(help); Unknown parameter|Author=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help); Unknown parameter|Publisher=
ignored (|publisher=
suggested) (help); Unknown parameter|Title=
ignored (|title=
suggested) (help); Unknown parameter|Year=
ignored (|year=
suggested) (help) - Template:Journal reference
- McShane, D. A. & Plas, J. M. (1982). Wechsler scale performance patterns of American Indian children. Psychology in the Schools, 19, 8-17.
- McShane, D. A., & Plas, J. M. (1984). The cognitive functioning of American Indian children: Moving from the WISC to the WISC-R. The School Psychology Review, 13, 61-73.
- Template:Journal reference
- McGue, M., Bouchard, T. J., Jr., Iacono, W. G., & Lykken, D. T. (1993). "Behavioral genetics of cognitive ability: A life-span perspective". In R. Plomin & G. E. McClearn (Eds.), Nature, nurture, and psychology (pp. 59–76). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
- Murray, Charles (1998). Income Inequality and IQ, AEI Press PDF
- Template:Journal reference
- Neisser, U. (1997). Never a dull moment. American Psychologist, 52, 79-81.
- Nisbett, R. E. (1995). "Race, IQ and scientism". In S. Fraser (Ed.), The bell curve wars (pp. 36–57). New York: HarperCollins.
- Nisbett, R. E. (1998). "Race, genetics, and IQ". In A. Phillips (Ed.), The Black-White test score gap (pp. 86–102). Washington, DC: Brookings Institution.
- Template:Journal reference
- Ogbu, J. U. (2002). "Cultural amplifiers of intelligence: IQ and minority status in crosscultural perspective". In J. M. Fish (Ed.), Race and intelligence: Separating science from myth (pp. 241–278). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
- . ISBN 0936396008.
{{cite book}}
: Missing or empty|title=
(help); Unknown parameter|Author=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help); Unknown parameter|Publisher=
ignored (|publisher=
suggested) (help); Unknown parameter|Title=
ignored (|title=
suggested) (help); Unknown parameter|Year=
ignored (|year=
suggested) (help) - Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- . ISBN 0716751593.
{{cite book}}
: Missing or empty|title=
(help); Unknown parameter|Author=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help); Unknown parameter|Publisher=
ignored (|publisher=
suggested) (help); Unknown parameter|Title=
ignored (|title=
suggested) (help); Unknown parameter|Year=
ignored (|year=
suggested) (help) - Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- . ISBN 0965683613.
{{cite book}}
: Missing or empty|title=
(help); Unknown parameter|Author=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help); Unknown parameter|Publisher=
ignored (|publisher=
suggested) (help); Unknown parameter|Title=
ignored (|title=
suggested) (help); Unknown parameter|Year=
ignored (|year=
suggested) (help) - Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- Rushton, J. P., & Ankney, C. D. (2000). Size matters: a review and new analyses of racial differences in cranial capacity and intelligence that refute Kamin and Omari. Personality and Individual Differences, 29, 591–620.
- Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- . ISBN 0887388396.
{{cite book}}
: Missing or empty|title=
(help); Unknown parameter|Author=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help); Unknown parameter|Publisher=
ignored (|publisher=
suggested) (help); Unknown parameter|Title=
ignored (|title=
suggested) (help); Unknown parameter|Year=
ignored (|year=
suggested) (help) - Template:Journal reference
- The Persistent Racial Scoring Gap on the SAT (in Special Report: The SAT as a Major Roadblock to Black Students' Aspirations to Higher Education) The Journal of Blacks in Higher Education, 29. (Autumn, 2000), 85–88.
- Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- . ISBN 0716707373.
{{cite book}}
: Missing or empty|title=
(help); Unknown parameter|Author=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help); Unknown parameter|Publisher=
ignored (|publisher=
suggested) (help); Unknown parameter|Title=
ignored (|title=
suggested) (help); Unknown parameter|Year=
ignored (|year=
suggested) (help) - Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- Template:Journal reference
- Zarske, J. A., & Moore, C. L. (1982). Recategorized WISC-R scores of learning disabled Navajo Indian children. Psychology in the Schools, 19: 156-159.
External links
- The Wall Street Journal: Mainstream Science on IntelligencePDF
- American Psychological Association Task Force Report, "Intelligence: Knowns and Unknowns"
- Legal scholar and microbiologist Pilar Ossorio answers questions from viewers about science, medicine, racial classification, and more.
- Prof. Evelynn Hammonds on the history of race in science and medicine in the United States
- "Heritability Estimates Versus Large Environmental Effects: The IQ Paradox Resolved"; Psychological Review, 2001, Vol. 108, No. 2, 346-369
- "We're All Related To Kevin Bacon" – Steve Olson on popular misconceptions about genetics in the Washington Post
- The Skeptic's Dictionary entry on IQ and race
- Poverty and Brain Development in Early Childhood 1999 report
- The Early Catastrophe: The 30 Million Word Gap by Age, American Educator, Spring 2003
- Race and Intelligence
- The Black-White Test Score Gap (1998) online (page-image) version of ISBN 0815746091
- Race, Genetics and IQ – Professor Richard E. Nisbett examines the evidence and different arguments. (PDF)
- Miscellaneous articles by Richard Lynn et al.
- Thirty Years of Research on Race Differences in Cognitive Ability by J. Philippe Rushton and Arthur R. Jensen in Psychology, Public Policy, and Law 2005, Vol. 11, No. 2, 235–294
- Crippled by Their Culture - Race doesn't hold back America's black rednecks. Nor does racism by Thomas Sowell
- Cultural Bias in Intelligence Testing
- Cultural Bias in IQ Testing
- Are IQ Tests Biased?
- Psychological Testing (Powerpoint)
- Culturally Inane
- Scholars Provide an Overview of Explanations for Black-White Test Score Gap
- What if the Heriditarian Hypothesis is true? by Linda Gottredson
- Natural History of Ashkenazi Intelligence
- Researchers Say Intelligence and Diseases May Be Linked in Ashkenazic Genes by Nicholas Wade, New York Times, June 3, 2005.