Revision as of 08:08, 23 February 2016 editAnoldsegacount (talk | contribs)Extended confirmed users913 editsm →Research on decapod crustaceans← Previous edit | Revision as of 21:41, 3 March 2016 edit undoDrChrissy (talk | contribs)Extended confirmed users21,946 edits →Argument by analogy: formatting refsNext edit → | ||
(146 intermediate revisions by 5 users not shown) | |||
Line 1: | Line 1: | ||
] | ] | ||
'''Pain in ]s''' is a contentious issue. Pain is a complex mental state, with a distinct perceptual quality but also associated with ], which is an emotional state. Because of this complexity, the presence of ], or another human for that matter, cannot be determined unambiguously using observational methods, but the conclusion that animals experience pain is often inferred on the basis of likely presence of ] which is deduced from comparative brain physiology as well as physical and behavioural reactions.<ref name="abbott1995">{{cite journal|author=Abbott, F.V., Franklin, K.B.J. and Westbrook, R.F.|year=1995|title=The formalin test: Scoring properties of the first and second phases of the pain response in rats|journal=Pain|volume=60|issue=1|pages=91–102|pmid=7715946|doi=10.1016/0304-3959(94)00095-V|url=http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/0304-3959(94)00095-V}}</ref><ref name="Key2014">{{cite journal|year=2015|author=Key, B.|title=Fish do not feel pain and its implications for understanding phenomenal consciousness|journal=Biology and Philosophy|volume=30|issue=2|pages=149–165|doi=10.1007/s10539-014-9469-4|url=http://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s10539-014-9469-4/fulltext.html}}</ref> | |||
'''Pain in ]''' is contentious as there is debate whether they are capable of the subjective experiences of ] and ], or only the non-subjective, ] of ]. | |||
Most definitions of pain involve the two key components of nociception and suffering. Nociception is an ability to detect ] which evokes a reflex response that moves the entire animal, or the affected part of |
Most definitions of pain involve the two key components of nociception and suffering. Nociception is an ability to detect ] which evokes a reflex response that moves the entire animal, or the affected part of the body, away from the source of the stimulus. The concept of nociception does not imply any adverse, subjective feeling - it is a simple reflex action, not processed by the brain. Suffering is the subjective experience of the pain; the internal, emotional interpretation of the nociceptive experience and is therefore a private, emotional experience. Whilst a nociceptive reflex quickly removes an animal from a potentially damaging stimulus, this will not prevent the animal from repeatedly exposing itself to this risk. Pain involves learning, which has the adaptive function of lowering the probability of the animal repeatedly exposing itself to potential injury. | ||
Suffering cannot be directly measured in other animals, including other humans. Responses to putatively painful stimuli can be measured, but not the experience itself. To address this problem when assessing the capacity of other species to experience pain, ] is sometimes used. Another approach is to construct a list of criteria and assess whether the animal fulfills these. | |||
In ]s, endogenous ]s are neurochemicals that moderate pain by interacting with opioid receptors. Opioid peptides and opioid receptors occur naturally in crustaceans, and although "...at present no certain conclusion can be drawn",<ref name="Sømme " /> some have interpreted their presence as an indication that crustaceans may experience pain.<ref name="afa" /><ref name="Sømme " /> Opioids may moderate pain in lobsters similar to the way they moderate pain in vertebrates.<ref name="afa" /> | |||
Crustaceans fulfill several criteria proposed as indicating that non-human animals may experience pain. These fulfilled criteria include a suitable nervous system and sensory receptors, opioid receptors and reduced responses to noxious stimuli when given analgesics and local anaesthetics, physiological changes to noxious stimuli, displaying protective motor reactions, exhibiting avoidance learning and making trade-offs between noxious stimulus avoidance and other motivational requirements. | |||
In ]s, endogenous ]s are neurochemicals that moderate pain by interacting with opioid receptors. Opioid peptides and opioid receptors occur naturally in crustaceans, and although it was concluded in 2005 "...at present no certain conclusion can be drawn",<ref name="Sømme " /> more recent considerations suggest their presence along with related physiological and behavioural responses as indicating that crustaceans may experience pain.<ref name="Dyuizen" /><ref name="Elwood2015" /> Opioids may moderate pain in crustaceans in a similar way to that in vertebrates. | |||
If crustaceans feel pain, there are ethical and animal welfare implications including the consequences of exposure to pollutants, and practices involving ] and ], ], food preparation and for crustaceans used in ]. | |||
==Background== | ==Background== | ||
{{See also|Pain in animals|Pain in invertebrates}} | {{See also|Pain in animals|Pain in invertebrates}} | ||
The possibility that crustaceans and other non-human animals may experience pain has a long history. Initially, this was based around theoretical and philosophical argument, but more recently has turned to scientific investigation. | |||
The belief that non-human animals might not feel ] in the same manner as humans dates back to at least the 17th century, when French philosopher ] argued that animals do not experience pain because they lack ].<ref name=Carbone149>{{cite book |author=Larry Carbone |year=2004 |url=http://books.google.com/books?id=Iheg3hkj99AC |title=What Animals Want: Expertise and Advocacy in Laboratory Animal Welfare Policy |publisher=] |page=149 |isbn=978-0-19-516196-0}}</ref><ref name=nuffield45> Nuffield Council on Bioethics, Accessed 27 February 2008 {{Wayback|url=http://www.nuffieldbioethics.org/fileLibrary/pdf/RIA_Report_FINAL-opt.pdf|date =20080227041442|bot=DASHBot}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |author=] |year=2007 |chapter=Animal research: a moral science |title=Animal research: a moral science. Talking Point on the use of animals in scientific research |journal=] |volume=8 |issue=6 |pages=521–525 |doi=10.1038/sj.embor.7400996 |pmid=17545990 |pmc=2002540}}</ref> ] of Colorado State University, the principal author of two U.S. federal laws regulating pain relief for animals, wrote that researchers remained unsure into the 1980s as to whether animals experience pain. Veterinarians trained in the U.S. before 1989 were simply taught to ignore animal pain.<ref name=Rollin117>{{cite book |author=] |title=The Unheeded Cry: Animal Consciousness, Animal Pain, and Science |location=New York |publisher=] |year=1989 |pages=xii, 117–118 |isbn=978-0-19-286104-7}} Cited in Carbone 2004, p. 150.</ref> In his interactions with scientists and other veterinarians, Rollin was regularly asked to prove that animals are conscious and to provide scientifically acceptable grounds for claiming that they feel pain.<ref name=Rollin117/> Veterinarian and author Larry Carbone has written that the view that animals feel pain differently is now a minority view. Academic reviews of the topic are more equivocal, noting that although the argument that animals have at least simple conscious thoughts and feelings has strong support,<ref>{{cite journal |author=Donald R. Griffin & Gayle B. Speck |year=2004 |title=New evidence of animal consciousness |journal=Animal Cognition |volume=7 |issue=1 |pages=5–18 |pmid=14658059 |doi=10.1007/s10071-003-0203-x}}</ref> some critics continue to question how reliably animal mental states can be determined.<ref name=nuffield45/><ref>{{cite journal |author=Colin Allen |year=1998 |url=http://jas.fass.org/cgi/reprint/76/1/42.pdf |format=] |title=Assessing animal cognition: ethological and philosophical perspectives |journal=] |volume=76 |issue=1 |pages=42–47 |pmid=9464883}}</ref> | |||
===Philosophy=== | |||
Pain is a private experience and therefore we cannot know with certainty whether other animals, or other humans, experience pain. However, the capacity for experiencing pain can be inferred using argument-by-analogy and physiological and behavioral reactions.<ref>{{Cite journal|author=Frances V. Abbott, Keith B. J. Franklin & R. Frederick Westbrook |title=The formalin test: scoring properties of the first and second phases of the pain response in rats |journal=] |volume=60 |issue=1 |pages=91–102 |date=January 1995 |pmid=7715946 |doi=10.1016/0304-3959(94)00095-V |url=http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/0304-3959(94)00095-V}}</ref><ref name="Sherwin, 2001">Sherwin, C.M., (2001). Can invertebrates suffer? Or, how robust is argument-by-analogy? Animal Welfare, 10 (supplement): S103-S118</ref><ref name="Elwood, 2011">Elwood, R.W., (2011). Pain and suffering in invertebrates? Institute of Laboratory Animal Resources Journal, 52(2): 175-84 </ref> Specialists currently believe that all vertebrates can feel pain, and that certain invertebrates, like the ], might too.<ref>{{cite web |url=http://www.parl.gc.ca/37/2/parlbus/commbus/senate/Com-e/lega-e/witn-e/shelly-e.htm |title=Do invertebrates feel pain? |author=The Senate Standing Committee on Legal and Constitutional Affairs |publisher=] |accessdate=June 11, 2008}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal|author=Jane A. Smith |title=A question of pain in invertebrates |journal=] |volume=33 |issue=1–2 |pages=25–31 |year=1991 |doi=10.1093/ilar.33.1-2.25}}</ref> ], the neurones required for the sensation of pain, have been found in ], ], ]<ref name ="St John Smith, et al., 2009">St John Smith, E. and Lewin, G.R., (2009). Nociceptors: a phylogenetic view. Journal of Comparative Physiology. A. Neuroethology Sensory Neural and Behavioral Physiology, 195: 1089-1106</ref> and in the arthropod, ],<ref>{{cite journal |author=W. Daniel Tracey, Jr., Rachel I. Wilson, Gilles Laurent & Seymour Benzer |year=2003 |title=''painless'', a ''Drosophila'' gene essential for nociception |journal=] |volume=113 |issue=2 |pages=261–273 |doi=10.1016/S0092-8674(03)00272-1 |pmid=12705873}}</ref> despite earlier claims that nociceptors were absent in insects.<ref>{{cite journal |author=C. H. Eisemann, W. K. Jorgensen, D. J. Merritt, M. J. Rice, B. W. Cribb, P. D. Webb & M. P. Zalucki |year=1984 |title=Do insects feel pain? – A biological view |journal=] |volume=40 |pages=1420–1423|url=http://www.ikhebeenvraag.be/mediastorage/FSDocument/41/Eisemann-164.pdf |format=]}}</ref> | |||
] | |||
The idea that non-human animals might not feel ] goes back to the 17th-century French philosopher, ], who argued that animals do not experience pain and suffering because they lack ].<ref name=Carbone149>{{cite book|author=Carbone, L.|year=2004|url=http://books.google.co.nz/books?id=Iheg3hkj99AC&printsec=frontcover&dq=%22What+Animal+Want:+Expertise+and+Advocacy+in+Laboratory+Animal+Welfare+Policy%22&ei=J8GoSrTRGJHSNYyw8JMK#v=onepage&q=&f=false|title=What Animals Want: Expertise and Advocacy in Laboratory Animal Welfare Policy|publisher=Oxford University Press|pages=149}}</ref><ref name="Radner">{{cite book|author=Radner, D. and Radner, M.|year=1989|title=Animal Consciousness|publisher=Prometheus Books: Buffalo}}</ref><ref name="Harrison">{{cite journal|author=Harrison, P.|year=1992|title=Descartes on animals|journal=The Philosophical Quarterly|pages=219–227|volume=42|issue=167|doi=10.2307/2220217|jstor=2220217}}</ref> In 1789, the British philosopher and social reformist, ], addressed in his book ''An Introduction to the Principles of Morals and Legislation'' the issue of our treatment of animals with the following often quoted words: "The question is not, Can they reason? nor, can they talk? but, Can they suffer?"<ref name="Bentham">{{cite book|author="Bentham, J.|year=1879|title=An Introduction to the Principles of Morals and Legislation|publisher=Clarendon Press}}</ref> | |||
], a bioethicist and author of '']'' published in 1975, suggested that consciousness is not necessarily the key issue: just because animals have smaller brains, or are ‘less conscious’ than humans, does not mean that they are not capable of feeling pain. He goes on further to argue that we do not assume newborn infants, people suffering from neurodegenerative brain diseases or people with learning disabilities experience less pain than we would.<ref name="WelcomeTrust">{{cite web|author=Sneddon, L.U.|title=Can animals feel pain?|publisher=The Welcome Trust|accessdate=September 24, 2015|url=http://www.wellcome.ac.uk/en/pain/microsite/culture2.html}}</ref> | |||
==Nociception== | |||
Nociception is the ability to perceive a noxious stimulus and react in a reflexive manner; it occurs across a wide range of taxa. The rockpool prawn ('']'') reacts to noxious stimuli with an immediate reflex tail flick response<ref name="Barr2008" /> | |||
], the principal author of two U.S. federal laws regulating pain relief for animals, writes that researchers remained unsure into the 1980s as to whether animals experience pain, and veterinarians trained in the U.S. before 1989 were taught to simply ignore animal pain.<ref name=Rollin117>{{cite book|author=Rollin, B.|year=1989|title=The Unheeded Cry: Animal Consciousness, Animal Pain, and Science''|publisher=Oxford University Press, pp. xii, 117-118, cited in Carbone 2004, p. 150}}</ref> In his interactions with scientists and other veterinarians, Rollin was regularly asked to "prove" that animals are conscious, and to provide "scientifically acceptable" grounds for claiming that they feel pain.<ref name=Rollin117/> | |||
==Research on decapod crustaceans== | |||
], '']'']] | |||
===Prawns, crayfish and lobsters=== | |||
In 2005 a review of the literature by the Norwegian Scientific Committee for Food Safety tentatively concluded that "it is unlikely that can feel pain," though they note that "there is apparently a paucity of exact knowledge on sentience in crustaceans, and more research is needed." This conclusion is based on the ]'s simple nervous system. The report assumes that the violent reaction of lobsters to boiling water is a reflex response (i.e. does not involve conscious perception) to noxious stimuli.<ref name="Sømme ">{{Cite journal|author=L. Sømme |year=2005 |title=Sentience and pain in invertebrates: Report to Norwegian Scientific Committee for Food Safety |journal=], ]}}</ref> | |||
Continuing into the 1990s, discussions were further developed on the roles that philosophy and science had in understanding ] and mentality.<ref>{{cite journal|author=Allen, C.|year=1998 |url=https://dl.sciencesocieties.org/publications/jas/abstracts/76/1/42|title=Assessing animal cognition: Ethological and philosophical perspectives|journal=Journal of Animal Science |volume=76 |issue=1 |pages=42–47|pmid=9464883}}</ref> In subsequent years, it was argued there was strong support for the suggestion that some animals (most likely ]) have at least simple conscious thoughts and feelings<ref>{{cite journal|author=Griffin, D.R. and Speck, G.B. |year=2004 |title=New evidence of animal consciousness |journal=Animal Cognition |volume=7 |issue=1 |pages=5–18 |pmid=14658059 |doi=10.1007/s10071-003-0203-x}}</ref> and that the view animals feel pain differently to humans is now a minority view.<ref name=Carbone149 /> | |||
Latter studies at Queen's University, Belfast, provided evidence that similar crustaceans do feel pain.<ref name="Barr2008">{{Cite journal|author=Stuart Barr, Peter R. Laming, Jaimie T. A. Dick & Robert W. Elwood |year=2008 |title=Nociception or pain in a decapod crustacean? |journal=] |volume=75 |issue=3 |pages=745–751 |doi=10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.07.004}}</ref> When the antennae of rockpool prawns ] were rubbed with ] or ], the animals showed increased grooming and rubbing of the afflicted area against the side of the tank. Moreover, this reaction was inhibited by a local anesthetic, even though control prawns treated with only anesthetic did not show reduced activity. Elwood argues that sensing pain is crucial to prawn survival, because it encourages them to avoid damaging behaviors. Other scientists suggested the rubbing may reflect an attempt to clean the affected area<ref>{{cite news |author=Ian Sample |url=http://www.guardian.co.uk/science/2007/nov/08/animalrights.sciencenews |title=Blow for fans of boiled lobster: crustaceans feel pain, study says |publisher=] |date=November 8, 2007}}</ref> as application of anesthetic alone caused an increase in grooming. Several key effects were not observed in a separate study which found no behavioural or neural changes in three different species (red swamp crayfish ('']''), white shrimp ('']'') and '']'' sp.) in response to acids or bases.<ref name=puri2010>{{cite journal|author=Puri S, Faulkes Z|year=2010|title=Do decapod crustaceans have nociceptors for extreme pH?|journal=PLOS ONE|volume=5|issue=4|pages=e10244|doi=10.1371/journal.pone.0010244|url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0010244}}</ref> | |||
=== |
===Scientific investigation=== | ||
{{quote box | |||
In 2009, Elwood and Mirjam Appel showed that ]s make motivational tradeoffs between electric shocks and the quality of the shells they inhabit.<ref name="elwood2009">{{Cite journal|author=Robert W. Elwood & Mirjam Appel |year=2009 |title=Pain experience in hermit crabs? |journal=] |volume=77 |issue=5 |pages=1243–1246 |doi=10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.01.028 |url=http://www.mbl.edu/research/services/iacuc/pdf/pain_hermit_crabs.pdf |format=]}}</ref> In particular, as hermit crabs are shocked more intensely, they become increasingly willing to leave their current shells for new shells, and they spend less time deciding whether to enter those new shells. Moreover, because the researchers did not offer the new shells until after the electrical stimulation had ended, the change in motivational behavior was the result of memory of the noxious event, not an immediate reflex. | |||
|title = ''Cambridge Declaration on Consciousness'' (2012) | |||
|quote = The absence of a ] does not appear to preclude an organism from experiencing affective states. Convergent evidence indicates that non-human animals have the neuroanatomical, neurochemical, and neurophysiological substrates of conscious states along with the capacity to exhibit ]. Consequently, the weight of evidence indicates that humans are not unique in possessing the ]s that generate consciousness. Non-human animals, including all mammals and birds, and many other creatures, including octopuses, also possess these neurological substrates.<ref name="CDC">{{cite web|url=http://fcmconference.org/img/CambridgeDeclarationOnConsciousness.pdf|title=The Cambridge declaration on consciousness|date=July 7, 2012|author=Low, P.|editor=Jaak Panksepp, Diana Reiss, David Edelman, Bruno Van Swinderen, Philip Low and Christof Koch|publisher=University of Cambridge}}</ref> | |||
| quoted = true | |||
===Crabs=== | |||
| width = 360px | |||
] was quickly cut in half before cooking]] | |||
| align = right}} | |||
Shore crabs ('']'') also show motivational tradeoffs; they will discard a valuable resource (a preferred shelter) to avoid future encounters with painful stimuli, thereby indicating avoidance learning - a key criterion of the ability to experience pain.<ref name=elwood2013>{{cite journal|author=Robert W. Elwood and Barry Magee|year=2013|title=Shock avoidance by discrimination learning in the shore crab (Carcinus maenas) is consistent with a key criterion for pain|journal=]|doi=10.1242/jeb.072041|url=http://jeb.biologists.org/content/216/3/353.abstract}}</ref> | |||
In the 20th and 21st centuries, there were many scientific investigations of pain in non-human animals. | |||
====Mammals==== | |||
], an analgesic, and ], an opioid receptor antagonist, affect the estuarine crab ('']'') in a similar way to their effects on vertebrates: injections of morphine produced a dose-dependent reduction of their defensive response to an electric shock.<ref>{{Cite journal|author=M. Lozada, A. Romano & H. Maldonado |year=1988 |title=Effect of morphine and naloxone on a defensive response of the crab ''Chasmagnathus granulatus'' |journal=Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior |volume=30 |issue=3 |pages=635–640 |doi=10.1016/0091-3057(88)90076-7}}</ref> However, it has been suggested the attenuated defensive response could originate from either the analgesic or sedative properties of morphine, or both.<ref name="dyak">{{Cite journal|author=V. E. Dyakonova |year=2001 |title=Role of opioid peptides in behavior of invertebrates |journal=Journal of Evolutionary Biochemistry and Physiology |volume=37 |issue=4 |pages=335–347 |url=http://www.ingentaconnect.com/content/maik/joey/2001/00000037/00000004/00366536 |doi=10.1023/A:1012910525424}}</ref> These findings have been replicated for other invertebrate species,<ref name="dyak" /> but similar data are not yet available for lobsters. | |||
At the turn of the century, studies were published showing that arthritic rats self-select analgesic opiates.<ref name="Colpaert">{{cite journal |author=Colpaert, F.C., Tarayre, J.P., Alliaga, M., Slot. L.A.B., Attal, N. and Koek, W.| year = 2001 | title = Opiate self-administration as a measure of chronic nociceptive pain in arthritic rats | url = | journal = Pain | volume = 91 | issue = | pages=33–45 | doi=10.1016/s0304-3959(00)00413-9}}</ref> | |||
Formalin injection into the ] of shore crabs (''Hemigrapsus sanguineus'') evoked specific nociceptive behavior and neurochemical responses in the brain and thoracic ganglion.<ref>Dyuizen, I.V., Kotsyuba, E.P. and Lamash, N.E., (2012). Changes in the nitric oxide system in the shore crab ''Hemigrapsus sanguineus'' (Crustacea, decapoda) CNS induced by a nociceptive stimulus. The Journal of Experimental Biology, 215: 2668-2676</ref> | |||
In 2014, the veterinary ''Journal of Small Animal Practice'' published an article on the recognition of pain which started - "The ability to experience pain is universally shared by all mammals...".<ref name="Mathews">{{cite journal|author=Mathews, K., Kronen, P.W., Lascelles, D., Nolan, A., Robertson, S., Steagall, P.V., Wright, B. and Yamashita, K.|year=2014|title=Guidelines for recognition, assessment and treatment of pain.|journal=Journal of Small Animal Practice|volume=55|issue=6|pages=E10-E68|doi=10.1111/jsap.12200}}</ref> | |||
====Birds==== | |||
At the same time as the investigations using arthritic rats, studies were published showing that ]s with gait abnormalities self-select for a diet that contains ], a human ].<ref name="Danbury">{{cite journal|author=Danbury, T.C., Weeks, C.A., Chambers, J.P., Waterman-Pearson, A.E. and Kestin, S.C.|year=2000|title=Self-selection of the analgesic drug carprofen by lame broiler chickens|journal=The Veterinary Record|volume=146|issue=11|pages=307–311|doi=10.1136/vr.146.11.307}}</ref> In 2005, it was written "Avian pain is likely analogous to pain experienced by most mammals"<ref name="Machin">{{cite journal|author=Machin, K.L.|year=2005|title=Avian analgesia|journal=Seminars in Avian and Exotic Pet Medicine|volume=14|issue=4|pages=236–242|doi=10.1053/j.saep.2005.09.004}}</ref> and in 2014, "...it is accepted that birds perceive and respond to noxious stimuli and that birds feel pain"<ref name="Gaynor">{{cite book|editor=Gaynor, J.S. and Muir III, W. W.|year=2014|title=Handbook of Veterinary Pain Management|publisher=Elsevier Health Sciences|chapter=Chapter 26 - Bird-specific considerations: recognizing pain in pet birds.|author=Paul-Murphy, J. and Hawkins, M.G.}}</ref> | |||
====Reptiles and amphibians==== | |||
Veterinary articles have been published stating both reptiles<ref name="Mosley">{{cite journal|author=Mosley, C.A.|year=2005|title=Anesthesia and analgesia in reptiles|journal=Seminars in Avian and Exotic Pet Medicine|volume=14|issue=4|pages=243–262|doi=10.1053/j.saep.2005.09.005}}</ref><ref name="Mosley2001">{{cite journal|author=Mosley, C.|year=2011|title=Pain and nociception in reptiles|journal=Veterinary Clinics of North America: Exotic Animal Practice|volume=14|issue=1|pages=45–60|doi=10.1016/j.cvex.2010.09.009}}</ref><ref name="Sladky">{{cite journal|author=Sladky, K.K. and Mans, C.|year=2012|title=Clinical analgesia in reptiles|journal=Journal of Exotic Pet Medicine|volume=21|issue=2|pages=158–167|doi=10.1053/j.jepm.2012.02.012}}</ref> and amphibians<ref name="Machin1999">{{cite journal|author=Machin, K.L.|year=1999|title=Amphibian pain and analgesia|journal=Journal of Zoo and Wildlife Medicine|pages=2–10|jstor=20095815|volume=30}}</ref> | |||
<ref name="Machin2001">{{cite journal|author=Machin, K.L.|year=2001|title=Fish, amphibian, and reptile analgesia|journal=The Veterinary Clinics of North America. Exotic Animal Practice|volume=4|issue=1|pages=19–33}}</ref><ref name="Stevens2011">{{cite journal|author=Stevens, C.W.|year=2011|title=Analgesia in amphibians: preclinical studies and clinical applications|journal=Veterinary Clinics of North America: Exotic Animal Practice|volume=14|issue=1|pages=33–44|doi=10.1016/j.cvex.2010.09.007}}</ref> experience pain in a way analogous to humans, and that analgesics are effective in these two ] of vertebrates. | |||
====Argument by analogy==== | |||
] is sometimes used to assess the capacity of other animals to experience pain. This is based on the principle that if a non-human animal's responses to noxious stimuli are similar to those of humans, they are likely to have had an analogous experience. For example, if a pin is stuck in a chimpanzee's finger and she rapidly withdraws her hand, then argument by analogy indicates that like humans, she felt pain.<ref name="Gherardi">{{cite journal|author=Gherardi, F.|year=2009|title=Behavioural indicators of pain in crustacean decapods|journal=Annali dell'Istituto superiore di sanità|volume=45|issue=4|pages=432–438}}</ref><ref name="Sherwin">{{cite journal|author=Sherwin, C.M.|year=2001|title=Can invertebrates suffer? Or, how robust is argument-by-analogy?|journal=Animal Welfare (supplement)|volume=10|pages=S103-S118}}</ref><ref name="Elwood2011">{{cite journal|author=Elwood, R.W.|year=2011|title=Pain and suffering in invertebrates?|journal=Institute of Laboratory Animal Resources Journal|volume=52|issue=2|pages=175–84|url=http://dels-old.nas.edu/ilar_n/ilarjournal/52_2/PDFs/v5202Elwood.pdf}}</ref> | |||
In 2012 the American philosopher Gary Varner reviewed the research literature on pain in animals. His findings are summarised in the following table.<ref name=Varner2012>{{cite book|author=Varner, G.E.|year=2012|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=12U6Fr-083YC&printsec=frontcover&dq=%22Personhood,+Ethics,+and+Animal+Cognition%22&hl=en&sa=X&redir_esc=y#v=onepage&q=%22Personhood%2C%20Ethics%2C%20and%20Animal%20Cognition%22&f=false|chapter=Chapter 5 - Which animals are sentient?|title=Personhood, Ethics, and Animal Cognition: Situating Animals in Hare’s Two Level Utilitarianism|publisher=Oxford University Press|ISBN=9780199758784|doi=10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199758784.001.0001}} Table 5.2, page 113.</ref> Arguing by analogy, Varner claims that any animal which exhibits the properties listed in the table could be said to experience pain. On that basis, he concludes that all vertebrates, including fish, probably experience pain, but invertebrates (i.e. crustaceans) apart from ]s probably do not experience pain.<ref name=Varner2012 /><ref>{{cite book|author=Kristin, A.|year=2014|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=fiIhBQAAQBAJ&pg=PT101&dq=fish+pain+philosophy&hl=en&sa=X&ved=0CDAQ6AEwAmoVChMI2JOin7GoyAIV4dqmCh2-Tw9b#v=onepage&q=fish%20pain%20philosophy&f=false|title=The Animal Mind: An Introduction to the Philosophy of Animal Cognition (section 3.6.2)|publisher=Routledge|ISBN=9781317676751}}</ref> | |||
{| class="wikitable" | |||
|- | |||
! colspan=10 | Argument by analogy<ref name=Varner2012 /> | |||
|- | |||
! rowspan=2 | Property | |||
! colspan=4 | Invertebrates | |||
! colspan=5 | Vertebrates | |||
|- | |||
! Earthworms | |||
! Insects | |||
! Leeches/snails | |||
! Cephalopods | |||
! Fish | |||
! Amphibians | |||
! Reptiles | |||
! Birds | |||
! Mammals | |||
|- | |||
| Has ]s | |||
| align=center | ? | |||
| align=center | {{nay}} | |||
| align=center | {{aye}} | |||
| align=center | ? | |||
| align=center | {{aye}} | |||
| align=center | {{aye}} | |||
| align=center | {{aye}} | |||
| align=center | {{aye}} | |||
| align=center | {{aye}} | |||
|- | |||
| Has brain | |||
| align=center | {{nay}} | |||
| align=center | {{nay}} | |||
| align=center | {{nay}} | |||
| align=center | {{aye}} | |||
| align=center | {{aye}} | |||
| align=center | {{aye}} | |||
| align=center | {{aye}} | |||
| align=center | {{aye}} | |||
| align=center | {{aye}} | |||
|- | |||
| Nociceptors and brain linked | |||
| align=center | {{nay}} | |||
| align=center | {{nay}} | |||
| align=center | {{nay}} | |||
| align=center | {{aye}} | |||
| align=center | {{aye}} | |||
| align=center | ? / {{aye}} | |||
| align=center | ? / {{aye}} | |||
| align=center | ? / {{aye}} | |||
| align=center | {{aye}} | |||
|- | |||
| Has ] ]s | |||
| align=center | {{aye}} | |||
| align=center | {{aye}} | |||
| align=center | ? | |||
| align=center | {{nay}} | |||
| align=center | {{aye}} | |||
| align=center | {{aye}} | |||
| align=center | {{aye}} | |||
| align=center | {{aye}} | |||
| align=center | {{aye}} | |||
|- | |||
| ]s affect responses | |||
| align=center | ? | |||
| align=center | ? | |||
| align=center | ? | |||
| align=center | ? | |||
| align=center | {{aye}} | |||
| align=center | ? | |||
| align=center | ? | |||
| align=center | {{aye}} | |||
| align=center | {{aye}} | |||
|- | |||
| Response to damaging stimuli similar to humans | |||
| align=center | {{nay}} | |||
| align=center | {{nay}} | |||
| align=center | ? | |||
| align=center | {{aye}} | |||
| align=center | {{aye}} | |||
| align=center | {{aye}} | |||
| align=center | {{aye}} | |||
| align=center | {{aye}} | |||
| align=center | {{aye}} | |||
|} | |||
==The experience of pain== | |||
Although there are numerous definitions of ], almost all involve two key components. | |||
First, ] is required.<ref name="Sneddon, (2004)">{{cite journal | last1 = Sneddon | first1 = L.U. | year = 2004 | title = Evolution of nociception in vertebrates: comparative analysis of lower vertebrates | url = | journal = Brain Research Reviews | volume = 46 | issue = | pages = 123–130 | doi=10.1016/j.brainresrev.2004.07.007}}</ref> This is the ability to detect noxious stimuli which evoke a ] response that rapidly moves the entire animal, or the affected part of its body, away from the source of the stimulus. The concept of nociception does not imply any adverse, subjective "feeling" – it is a reflex action. An example in humans would be the rapid withdrawal of a finger that has touched something hot – the withdrawal occurs before any sensation of pain is actually experienced. | |||
The second component is the experience of "pain" itself, or ] – the internal, emotional interpretation of the nociceptive experience. Again in humans, this is when the withdrawn finger begins to hurt, moments after the withdrawal. Pain is therefore a private, emotional experience. Nociceptive reflexes act to immediately remove the animal or part of the body from a (potentially) damaging stimulus. However, without learning from this experience, the animal would likely expose itself to the damaging stimulus repeatedly. Pain has the adaptive advantage that it invokes a level of learning, thereby preventing the animal from repeatedly exposing itself to potential injury.<ref name="Elwood2016">{{cite journal|author=Elwood, R.W.|year=2016|title=A single strand of argument with unfounded conclusion|journal=Animal Sentience: An Interdisciplinary Journal on Animal Feeling|volume=1|issue=3|page=19}}</ref>Pain cannot be directly measured in other animals, including other humans; responses to putatively painful stimuli can be measured, but not the experience itself. To address this problem when assessing the capacity of other species to experience pain, argument-by-analogy is used. This is based on the principle that if an animal responds to a stimulus in a similar way to ourselves, it is likely to have had an analogous experience. | |||
===Nociception=== | |||
{{main|Nociception}} | |||
] | |||
In vertebrates, nociceptive responses involve the transmission of a signal along a chain of ]s from the site of a noxious stimulus at the periphery, to the spinal cord. This process evokes a ] response such as flinching or immediate withdrawal of a limb, generated at the spinal cord and not involving the brain. Nociception is found, in one form or another, across all major animal ].<ref name="Sneddon, (2004)"/> Nociception can be observed using modern imaging techniques and both ] and behavioural responses to nociception can be detected. Many crustacean species, including the rockpool prawn ],<ref name="Barr2008">{{cite journal|author=Barr, S., Laming, P.R., Dick, J.T.A. and Elwood, R.W.|year=2008 |title=Nociception or pain in a decapod crustacean? |journal=] |volume=75 |issue=3 |pages=745–751 |doi=10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.07.004}}</ref> exhibit the ] - an immediate, nociceptive, reflex tail-flick response to noxious stimuli (see here ). | |||
===Emotional pain=== | |||
{{main|Psychological pain}} | |||
Sometimes a distinction is made between "physical pain" and "emotional" or "]". Emotional pain is the pain experienced in the absence of physical trauma, e.g. the pain experienced by humans after the loss of a loved one, or the break-up of a relationship. It has been argued that only ]s and ]s can feel "emotional pain", because they are the only animals that have a ] - a part of the brain's cortex considered to be the "thinking area". However, research has provided evidence that monkeys, dogs, cats and birds can show signs of ] and display behaviours associated with ], i.e. lack of motivation, lethargy, anorexia, unresponsiveness to other animals.<ref name="WelcomeTrust" /> | |||
===Physical pain=== | |||
{{main|Pain}} | |||
The nerve impulses of the nociception response may be conducted to the brain thereby registering the location, intensity, quality and unpleasantness of the stimulus. This subjective component of pain involves conscious awareness of both the sensation and the unpleasantness (the aversive, negative ]). The brain processes underlying conscious awareness of the unpleasantness (suffering), are not well understood. | |||
There have been several published lists of criteria for establishing whether non-human animals experience pain, e.g.<ref name="Sneddon2014">{{cite journal|author=Sneddon, L.U., Elwood, R.W., Adamo, S.A. and Leach, M.C.|title=Defining and assessing animal pain|journal=Animal Behaviour|volume=97|year=2014|pages=201–212|url=http://animalstudiesrepository.org/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=1068&context=acwp_arte|doi=10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.09.007}}</ref><ref name="Elwood2009">{{cite journal|author=Elwood, R.W., Barr, S. and Patterson, L.|year=2009|title=Pain and stress in crustaceans?|journal=Applied Animal Behaviour Science|volume=118|issue=3|pages=128–136|doi=10.1016/j.applanim.2009.02.018}}</ref> Some criteria that may indicate the potential of another species, including crustaceans, to feel pain include:<ref name="Elwood2009" /> | |||
# Has a suitable ] and ]s | |||
# Has ]s and shows reduced responses to noxious stimuli when given ]s and ]s | |||
# Physiological changes to ] | |||
# Displays protective motor reactions that might include reduced use of an affected area such as limping, rubbing, holding or ] | |||
# Shows ] | |||
# Shows trade-offs between noxious stimulus avoidance and other motivational requirements | |||
# High ] and ] | |||
==Research findings== | |||
] | |||
The vast majority of research on pain in crustaceans has used (semi-) aquatic, ] species. Animals living in largely different environments are unlikely to have developed the same nociceptive or pain-detecting neural mechanisms. Different environments will result in diverse ]s on different animal groups, as well as exposing them to differing types of nociceptive stimuli. For example, crustaceans living in an aquatic world can maintain a certain level of ], so the risk of collision due to gravity is limited compared with a terrestrial vertebrate. Similarly, noxious chemicals might be diluted considerably in an aquatic environment compared to terrestrial. Therefore, nociceptive and pain systems in aquatic animals may be quite dissimilar to terrestrial animals.<ref name="Sneddon2015" /> | |||
===Peripheral nervous system=== | |||
] | |||
====Receptors==== | |||
Crayfish (''Procambarus clarkii'') respond quickly and strongly to high temperatures, however, they show no response to low temperature stimuli, or, when stimulated with ] or ] (both are irritants to mammals). Noxious high temperatures are considered to be a potentially ecologically relevant noxious stimulus for crayfish that can be detected by sensory neurons, which may be specialized nociceptors.<ref name="Puri" /> | |||
The common brown shrimp '']'' and the prawns '']'' and '']'' all exhibit a nociceptive sensitivity to both hot and cold temperatures. Both thermal sensitivity levels and nociceptive thresholds change with changes in ] temperature.<ref name="Davies">{{cite thesis|author=Davies, C.A.|year=1981|title=Thermal sensitivity and behaviour in the brown shrimp and some related crustacea (Doctoral dissertation)|publisher=University College of Swansea}}</ref> | |||
====Nerve fibres==== | |||
Crayfish have peripheral nerve fibres (see here ) which are responsive to noxious stimuli.<ref name="Puri">{{cite journal|author=Puri, S. and Faulkes, Z.|year=2015|title=Can crayfish take the heat? Procambarus clarkii show nociceptive behaviour to high temperature stimuli, but not low temperature or chemical stimuli|journal=Biology Open|pages=BIO20149654|url=http://bio.biologists.org/content/early/2015/02/20/bio.20149654.eLetters}}</ref> | |||
Neurons functionally specialized for nociception have been documented in other invertebrates including the leech '']'', the nematode '']'' and the molluscs '']'' and '']''. Changes in neuronal activity induced by noxious stimuli have been recorded in the nervous centres of '']'', '']'' and larval '']''.<ref name="Dyuizen" /> | |||
===Central nervous system=== | |||
The bodies of crustaceans are segmented; there is one ] (cluster of nerve cells) per segment. Each ganglion receives sensory and movement information via nerves coming from the muscles, body wall, and appendages such as walking legs, ]s and mouthparts. The ganglia show great functional autonomy; information received by the ganglion is processed by the same ganglion, enabling a faster response than if the message had to travel all the way up up the animal's body to the brain and then back. Information can be exchanged between ganglia enabling the animal to perform coordinated movements. | |||
When shore crabs ('']'') have formalin injected into the ] (claw), this evokes specific nociceptive behavior and neurochemical responses in the thoracic ganglia and the brain.<ref name="Dyuizen">{{cite journal|author=Dyuizen, I.V., Kotsyuba, E.P. and Lamash, N.E.|year=2012|title=Changes in the nitric oxide system in the shore crab Hemigrapsus sanguineus (Crustacea, decapoda) CNS induced by a nociceptive stimulus|journal=Journal of Experimental Biology|volume=215|issue=15|pages=2668–2676}}</ref> | |||
====Brain==== | |||
] animals characteristically have a collection of nervous tissue toward the ] region of their body. Depending on the size, this may be termed the "]" or the "]". In decapods, the brain is divided into three main regions, the ], which consists of two optic lobes, and the median protocerebrum.<ref name="Sandeman">{{cite book|author=Sandeman, D.|year=1990|chapter=Structural and functional levels in the organization of decapod crustacean brains|title=Frontiers in Crustacean Neurobiology|pages=223–239|publisher=Birkhäuser Basel}}</ref> | |||
In 2002, James Rose (University of Wyoming) and more recently Brian Key (University of Queensland) published reviews arguing that fish (and presumably crustaceans) cannot feel pain because they lack a ] in the brain and therefore do not have consciousness.<ref name="Rose2002">{{cite journal|author=Rose, J.D.|year=2002|url=http://www.nal.usda.gov/awic/pubs/Fishwelfare/Rose.pdf|title=The neurobehavioral nature of fishes and the question of awareness and pain|journal=Reviews in Fisheries Science|volume=10|issue=1|pages=1–38|doi=10.1080/20026491051668}}</ref><ref name="RoseWEB">{{cite web|author=Rose, J.D.|url=http://www.coloradotu.org/do-fish-feel-pain/|title=Do fish feel pain?|year=2002|accessdate=September 27, 2007}}</ref><ref name="Key2014">{{cite journal|year=2015|author=Key, B.|title=Fish do not feel pain and its implications for understanding phenomenal consciousness|journal=Biology and Philosophy|volume=30|issue=2|pages=149–165|doi=10.1007/s10539-014-9469-4|url=http://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s10539-014-9469-4/fulltext.html}}</ref> This has been robustly contested. Animal behaviouralist, ], (Colorado State University) argues that animals could still have consciousness without a neocortex because "different species can use different brain structures and systems to handle the same functions."<ref name="grandin183">{{cite book |title= Animals in Translation|author=Grandin, T. and Johnson, C.|year= 2005|publisher= Scribner|location= New York|isbn=0-7432-4769-8|pages= 183–184}}</ref> Lynne Sneddon (university of Liverpool) proposes that to suggest a function suddenly arises without a primitive form defies the laws of evolution.<ref name="SneddonEvolution">{{cite journal|author=Sneddon, L.U.|year=2012|title=Pain perception in fish: Evidence and implications for the use of fish|journal=Journal of Consciousness Studies|volume=18|pages=209–229}}</ref> Other researchers also believe that animal consciousness does not require a neocortex, but can arise from ] ] brain networks.<ref name="CDC">{{cite web|url=http://fcmconference.org/img/CambridgeDeclarationOnConsciousness.pdf|title=The Cambridge declaration on consciousness|date=July 7, 2012|author=Low, P.|editor=Jaak Panksepp, Diana Reiss, David Edelman, Bruno Van Swinderen, Philip Low and Christof Koch|publisher=University of Cambridge}}</ref> | |||
===Opioid system and effects of analgesics=== | |||
Opiates modulate nociception in vertebrates. In vertebrates, ] is an analgesic and therefore ameliorates the sensation of pain. ] is an opioid-receptor antagonist and therefore blocks the effects of morphine. Opioid modulation of nociception has been demonstrated in several invertebrate species.<ref name="Nieto">{{cite journal|author=Nieto-Fernandez, F., Andrieux, S., Idrees, S., Bagnall, C., Pryor, S.C. and Sood, R.|year=2009|title=The effect of opioids and their antagonists on the nocifensive response of Caenorhabditis elegans to noxious thermal stimuli|journal=Invertebrate Neuroscience|volume=9|pages=195–200}}</ref> | |||
] | |||
The first report of opiate effects in invertebrates is based on the behavioural responses of the crustacean mantis shrimp '']''. These shrimp respond to an electric shock with an immediate, violent, convulsive-live flexion of the body. If they are injected with morphine-HCL, this produces a dose-dependent analgesia by increasing the intensity threshold to the shock. This effect is fully blocked by naloxone.<ref name="Moldanado1982">{{cite journal|author=Maldonado, H. and Miralto, A.|year=1982|title=Effect of morphine and naloxone on a defensive response of the mantis shrimp (Squilla mantis)|journal=Journal of Comparative Physiology|volume=147|issue=4|pages=455–459}}</ref> | |||
Crustaceans have a functional opioid system which includes the presence of opioid receptors similar to those of mammals. ] and ]-opioid receptors have been described in crustaceans.<ref name="Hanke">{{cite journal|author=Hanke, J., Willig, A., Yinon, U. and Jaros, P.P.|year=1997|title=Delta and kappa opioid receptors in eyestalk ganglia of a crustacean|journal=Brain Research|volume=744|issue=2|pages=279–284}}</ref> ] research on the ] (''Homarus americanus'') has revealed the presence of a ] transcript in neural and immune tissues, which exhibits a 100% sequence identity with its human counterpart.<ref name="Casares">{{cite journal|author=Casares, F.M., McElroy, A., Mantione, K.J., Baggermann, G., Zhu, W. and Stefano, G.B.|year=2005|title=The American lobster, Homarus americanus, contains morphine that is coupled to nitric oxide release in its nervous and immune tissues: Evidence for neurotransmitter and hormonal signaling|journal=Neuro Endocrinol. Lett.|volume=26|pages=89–97}}</ref> | |||
In the American lobster, endogenous morphine is found in the ] and ventral nerve cord. In lobsters which have had a ] (walking leg) cut off or been injected with the irritant ], the endogenous morphine levels initially increased by 24% for haemolymph and 48% for the nerve cord.<ref name="Casares" /> | |||
In vertebrates, opioid peptides (i.e., ]s) have been shown to be involved in nociception. ] and ] are present in the thoracic ganglia of the shore crab, ''Carcinus maenas''.<ref name="Lüschen">{{cite journal|author=Lüschen, W., Buck, F., Willig, A. and Jaros, P.P.|year=1991|title= Isolation, sequence analysis, and physiological properties of enkephalins in the nervous tissue of the shore crab Carcinus maenas L.|journal=Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences|volume=88|issue=19|pages=8671–8675}}</ref> | |||
Both morphine and naloxone affect the estuarine crab ('']'') in a similar way to their effects on vertebrates: injections of morphine produce a dose-dependent reduction of their defensive response to an electric shock.<ref name="Lozada">{{Cite journal|author=Lozada, M., Romano, A. and Maldonado, H.|year=1988 |title=Effect of morphine and naloxone on a defensive response of the crab ''Chasmagnathus granulatus'' |journal=Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior |volume=30 |issue=3 |pages=635–640 |doi=10.1016/0091-3057(88)90076-7}}</ref> However, it has been suggested the attenuated defensive response could originate from either the analgesic or sedative properties of morphine, or both.<ref name="dyak">{{Cite journal|author=Dyakonova, V.E.|year=2001 |title=Role of opioid peptides in behavior of invertebrates |journal=Journal of Evolutionary Biochemistry and Physiology |volume=37 |issue=4 |pages=335–347 |url=http://www.ingentaconnect.com/content/maik/joey/2001/00000037/00000004/00366536 |doi=10.1023/A:1012910525424}}</ref> One study on the effects of a danger stimulus on the crab ''Chasmagnathus granulatus'' reported this induces opioid analgesia, which is influenced by naloxone.<ref name="Valeggia">{{cite journal|author=Valeggia, C., Fernandez-Duque, E. and Maldonado, H.|year=1989|title=Danger stimulus-induced analgesia in the crab Chasmagnathus granulatus|title=Brain Research|volume=481|issue=2|pages=304–308}}</ref> In American lobsters, the response of endogenous morphine in both haemocytes and neural cells to noxious stimuli are mediated by naloxone.<ref name="Casares" /> | |||
When the antennae of rockpool prawns ''Palaemon elegans'' are rubbed with ] or ] (both are irritants in mammals), they increase grooming and rubbing of the afflicted area against the side of the tank; this reaction is inhibited by ] (a local anaesthetic in mammals), even though control prawns treated with only anaesthetic do not show reduced activity.<ref name="Barr2008" /> | |||
] (juvenile shown) has become the preferred species for shrimp farming]] | |||
] is the removal of one or both ]s from a crustacean. It is routinely practiced on female prawns in almost every marine shrimp maturation or reproduction facility in the world, both research and commercial. The aim of ablation is to stimulate the female shrimp to develop mature ovaries and spawn.<ref>http://www.plosone.org/article/info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fjournal.pone.0024427</ref> In '']'', prawns treated with ] (a local anaesthetic in mammals), showed less rubbing, flicking and sheltering than those without the anaesthetic.<ref name="Diarte">{{cite journal|author=Diarte-Plata, G., Sainz-Hernández, J.C., Aguiñaga-Cruz, J.A., Fierro-Coronado, J.A., Polanco-Torres, A. and Puente-Palazuelos, C.|year=2012|title=Eyestalk ablation procedures to minimize pain in the freshwater prawn Macrobrachium americanum|journal=Applied Animal Behaviour Science|volume=140|issue=3|pages=172–178}}</ref> | |||
One study on reducing the stress of prawns resulting from transportation concluded that Aqui-STM and ] (a natural anaesthetic) may be suitable anaesthetic treatments for prawns.<ref name="Coyle">{{cite journal|author=Coyle, S.D., Dasgupta, S., Tidwell, J.H., Beavers, T., Bright, L.A. and Yasharian, D.K.|year=2005|title=Comparative efficacy of anesthetics for the freshwater prawn Macrobrachiurn rosenbergii|journal=Journal of the World Aquaculture Society|volume=36|issue=3|pages=282–290}}</ref> | |||
===Physiological responses=== | |||
Higher levels of stress, as measured by lactate, occur in shore crabs exposed to brief electric shock compared to non-shocked controls. However, shocked crabs showed more vigorous behaviour than controls, possibly indicating it is increased behaviour causing the increased lactate. But, when crabs with the same level of behaviour are matched, shocked crabs still have a stronger stress response compared with controls. The authors suggested that their findings, coupled with previous findings of long-term motivational change and avoidance learning, "...fulfils the criteria expected of a pain experience".<ref name="Elwood2015">{{cite journal|author=Elwood, R.W. and Adams, L.|year=2015|title=Electric shock causes physiological stress responses in shore crabs, consistent with prediction of pain|journal=Biology Letters|volume=11|issue=11|pages=20150800}}</ref> | |||
Injection of formalin into the ] of shore crabs (''Hemigrapsus sanguineus'') evokes specific nociceptive behavior and neurochemical responses in the brain and thoracic ganglion.<ref>Dyuizen, I.V., Kotsyuba, E.P. and Lamash, N.E., (2012). Changes in the nitric oxide system in the shore crab ''Hemigrapsus sanguineus'' (Crustacea, decapoda) CNS induced by a nociceptive stimulus. The Journal of Experimental Biology, 215: 2668-2676 </ref> | |||
===Protective responses=== | |||
]Most species of hermit crabs have long, spirally curved abdomens, which are soft, unlike the hard, calcified abdomens seen in related crustaceans. They protect themselves from predators by entering a salvaged empty seashell, into which they can retract their whole body. As they grow, they must leave their shell and find another larger, more suitable shell (see here ). Their shells are therefore highly valuable to them. When hermit crabs ('']'') are given an electric shock, they leave their shells and subsequently perform prolonged abdominal grooming at the site of where they received the shock.<ref name="Appel2009">{{cite journal|author=Appel, M. and Elwood, R.W.|year=2009|title=Gender differences, responsiveness and memory of a potentially painful event in hermit crabs|journal=Animal Behaviour|volume=78|pages=1373–1379|doi=10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.09.008}}</ref> | |||
Male ''Chasmagnathus granulatus'' crabs exhibit a "defensive response" to electric shocks.<ref name="Lozada" /> During a study on the ability of shore crabs ('']'') learning to avoid an electrical shock, it was observed that many crabs emerged from the dark shelter to avoid the shock thus entering a brightly light area which would normally be avoided.<ref name=elwood2013 /> Immediately after the injection of ] (an irritant in mammals) or saline into one ] (the leg which ends with the claw), shore crabs move quickly into the corner of the aquarium and "freeze" after 2 to 3 seconds. After 1 to 3 minutes, these injected animals are fidgety and exhibit a wide range of movements such as flexion, extension, shaking or rubbing the affected claw. Formalin-treated animals show 20-times more rubbing behaviour during the first minute after injection than saline-treated crabs. Intense rubbing of the claw results in ] (shedding) in 20% of animals of the formalin-treated group whereas saline-injected crabs do not autotomise the injected cheliped. During the 10 minutes after injection, crabs in the formalin-treated group tried to use the intact cheliped, guarding the damaged cheliped. The scientists conducting this study commented "...the present results obtained in crabs may be indicative of pain experience rather than relating to a simple nociceptive reflex".<ref name="Dyuizen" /> Other species of crab have been shown to autotomise legs when placed on a hot plate or exposed to a small electric shock.<ref name="Gherardi" /> | |||
When the antennae of rockpool prawns ] are rubbed with ] or ] (both are irritants), the animals show increased grooming and rubbing of the afflicted area against the side of the tank. Furthermore, this reaction is inhibited by a local anaesthetic, even though control prawns treated with only anaesthetic did not show reduced activity. Other scientists suggested the rubbing may reflect an attempt to clean the affected area<ref>{{cite news |author=Ian Sample |url=http://www.guardian.co.uk/science/2007/nov/08/animalrights.sciencenews |title=Blow for fans of boiled lobster: crustaceans feel pain, study says |publisher=] |date=November 8, 2007}}</ref> as application of anaesthetic alone caused an increase in grooming. | |||
In one study, no behavioural or neural changes in three different crustacean species (red swamp crayfish ('']''), white shrimp ('']'') and '']'' sp.) were observed in response to noxious ]s or ].<ref name=puri2010>{{cite journal|author=Puri, S. and Faulkes, Z.|year=2010|title=Do decapod crustaceans have nociceptors for extreme pH?|journal=PLOS ONE|volume=5|issue=4|pages=e10244|doi=10.1371/journal.pone.0010244|url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0010244}}</ref> | |||
===Avoidance learning=== | |||
Shore crabs quickly (within 1 or 2 trials) learn to avoid one of two dark shelters if choosing that shelter consistently results in them receiving an electric shock.<ref name="Magee">{{cite journal|author=Magee, B. and Elwood, R.W.|year=2013|title=Shock avoidance by discrimination learning in the shore crab (Carcinus maenas) is consistent with a key criterion for pain|journal=Journal of Experimental Biology|volume=216|issue=3|pages=353–358}}</ref> The crayfish ''Procambarus clarkii'' and the crab ''Chasmagnathus granulatus'' learn to associate an electric shock with a light turning on, or with the occupancy of the light compartment of the aquarium, respectively. They quickly learn to respond to these associations by walking to a safe area in which the shock is not delivered (crayfish) or by refraining from entering the light compartment (crab).<ref name="Gherardi" /> | |||
===Trade-offs in motivation=== | |||
] | |||
Nociceptive responses are reflexes that do not change regardless of motivational priorities. In contrast, a painful experience may change the motivation for normal behavioural responses, thereby indicating a plastic response to an aversive stimulus, rather than a simple reflex response.<ref name="Elwood2009" /> | |||
In 2009, Elwood and Mirjam Appel showed that ]s make motivational trade-offs between electric shocks and the quality of the shells they inhabit.<ref name="elwood2009">{{Cite journal|author=Robert W. Elwood & Mirjam Appel |year=2009 |title=Pain experience in hermit crabs? |journal=] |volume=77 |issue=5 |pages=1243–1246 |doi=10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.01.028 |url=http://www.mbl.edu/research/services/iacuc/pdf/pain_hermit_crabs.pdf |format=]}}</ref> In particular, as hermit crabs are shocked more intensely, they become increasingly willing to leave their current shells for new shells, and they spend less time deciding whether to enter those new shells. Moreover, because the researchers did not offer the new shells until after the electrical stimulation had ended, the change in motivational behavior was the result of memory of the noxious event, not an immediate reflex. It was also shown that hermit crabs experiencing increasing electric shocks left their shell at a reduced intensity intensity when the shell was from a less preferred species than did those in shells of a more desirable species. This demonstrates that hermit crabs are willing to risk predator attack by evacuating their shells to avoid a noxious stimulus and that this is dependent upon how valuable the shell is.<ref name="Sneddon2015" /> | |||
Shore crabs (''Carcinus maenas'') also show motivational trade-offs; they will discard a valuable resource (a preferred shelter) to avoid future encounters with painful stimuli, thereby indicating avoidance learning - a key criterion of the ability to experience pain.<ref name=elwood2013>{{cite journal|author=Robert W. Elwood and Barry Magee|year=2013|title=Shock avoidance by discrimination learning in the shore crab (Carcinus maenas) is consistent with a key criterion for pain|journal=]|doi=10.1242/jeb.072041|url=http://jeb.biologists.org/content/216/3/353.abstract}}</ref> | |||
A 2014 study<ref name="Fossat">{{cite journal|author=Fossat, P., Bacqué-Cazenave, J., De Deurwaerdère, P., Delbecque, J.-P. and Cattaert, D.|year=2014|title=Anxiety-like behavior in crayfish is controlled by serotonin|journal=Science|volume=344|pages=1293–1297|doi=10.1126/science.1248811}}</ref> on crayfish (''Procambarus clarkii'') tested their responses in a fear paradigm, the ] in which animals choose to walk on an elevated cross which offers both aversive and preferable conditions (in this case, two arms were lit and two were dark). Crayfish which experienced an electric shock displayed enhanced fearfulness or anxiety as demonstrated by their preference for the dark arms more than the light. Furthermore, shocked crayfish had relatively higher brain serotonin concentrations coupled with elevated blood glucose, which suggests a stress response.<ref name="Sneddon2015">{{cite journal|author=Sneddon, L.U.|year=2015|title=Pain in aquatic animals|journal=Journal of Experimental Biology|volume=218|issue=7|pages=967–976|url=http://jeb.biologists.org/content/218/7/967.full}}</ref> A follow-up study using the same species showed the intensity of the anxiety-like behaviour, presumably resulting from the pain, was dependent on the intensity of the electric shock until reaching a plateau. Such a quantitative relationship between stress and anxiety is also a very common feature of human and vertebrate anxiety.<ref name="Fossat2015">{{cite journal|author=Fossat, P., Bacqué-Cazenave, J., De Deurwaerdère, P., Cattaert, D. and Delbecque, J.P.|year=2015|title=Serotonin, but not dopamine, controls the stress response and anxiety-like behavior in the crayfish Procambarus clarkii|journal=Journal of Experimental Biology|volume=218|issue=17|pages=2745–2752}}</ref> | |||
==Opinion== | ==Opinion== | ||
Advocates for Animals, a Scottish ] group, stated in 2005 that "scientific evidence ... strongly suggests that there is a potential for ] crustaceans and cephalopods to experience pain and suffering". This is primarily due to "The likelihood that decapod crustaceans can feel pain is supported by the fact that they have been shown to have opioid receptors and to respond to opioids (analgesics such as morphine) in a similar way to vertebrates." Similarities between decapod and vertebrate stress systems and behavioral responses to noxious stimuli were given as additional evidence for the capacity of decapods to experience pain.<ref name="afa" /> | ]. Note the tail flipper second from left which has been notched by researchers to indicate she is an active breeding female.]]Advocates for Animals, a Scottish ] group, stated in 2005 that "scientific evidence ... strongly suggests that there is a potential for ] crustaceans and cephalopods to experience pain and suffering". This is primarily due to "The likelihood that decapod crustaceans can feel pain is supported by the fact that they have been shown to have opioid receptors and to respond to opioids (analgesics such as morphine) in a similar way to vertebrates." Similarities between decapod and vertebrate stress systems and behavioral responses to noxious stimuli were given as additional evidence for the capacity of decapods to experience pain.<ref name="afa">{{Cite book|title=Cephalopods and decapod crustaceans: their capacity to experience pain and suffering |publisher=Advocates for Animals |year=2005 |url=http://www.onekind.org/uploads/publications/cephalopods-decapod-crustaceans.pdf}}</ref> | ||
In 2005 a review of the literature by the Norwegian Scientific Committee for Food Safety tentatively concluded that "it is unlikely that can feel pain," though they note that "there is apparently a paucity of exact knowledge on sentience in crustaceans, and more research is needed." This conclusion is based on the ]'s simple nervous system. The report assumes that the violent reaction of lobsters to boiling water is a reflex response (i.e. does not involve conscious perception) to noxious stimuli.<ref name="Sømme">{{Cite journal|author=Sømme, L.|year=2005 |title=Sentience and pain in invertebrates: Report to Norwegian Scientific Committee for Food Safety |journal=], ]}}</ref> | |||
A ] (EFSA) 2005 publication<ref name="EFSA2005">{{cite journal|journal=The EFSA Journal|year=2005|volume=292|pages=1–46|title=Opinion on the aspects of the biology and welfare of animals used for experimental and other scientific purposes|publisher=European Food Safety Authority}}</ref> stated that the largest of decapod crustaceans have complex behaviour, some degree of awareness, a pain system and considerable learning abilities. Based on this evidence, they placed all decapod crustaceans into the same category of research-animal protection as vertebrates. | |||
==Pain during killing== | |||
] was cut in half before cooking]] | |||
The EFSA summarized that the killing methods most likely to cause pain and distress are:<ref name="Yue">{{cite web|author=Yue, S.|year=2008|title=The welfare of crustaceans at slaughter|publisher=Humane Society of the United States|url=http://animalstudiesrepository.org/hsus_reps_impacts_on_animals/4}}</ref> | |||
* Any procedures whereby the abdomen is separated from the thorax | |||
* The removal of tissue, flesh, or limbs while the crustacean is alive and fully conscious | |||
* Placing crustaceans in slowly heated water to the boiling point | |||
* Placing crustaceans directly into boiling water | |||
* Placing marine crustaceans in fresh water | |||
* Unfocused microwaving of the body as opposed to focal application to the head | |||
A device called the ] has been invented to electrocute ] such as lobsters, ], and ] before cooking. The device works by applying a 110-], 2 to 5 ] electrical charge to the animal. The CrustaStun renders the shellfish unconscious in 0.3 seconds and kills the animal in 5 to 10 seconds, compared to 3 minutes to kill a lobster by boiling.<ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.independent.co.uk/life-style/food-and-drink/news/ill-have-my-lobster-electrocuted-please-1824925.html|title=I'll have my lobster electrocuted, please|author=McSmith, A.|publisher=The Independent (Newspaper)|year=2009|accessdate=June 14, 2013}}</ref><ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.dailymail.co.uk/sciencetech/article-1324142/CrustaStun-The-humane-gadget-kills-lobsters-single-jolt-electricity.html|title=CrustaStun: The 'humane' gadget that kills lobsters with a single jolt of electricity|author=Anon.|publisher=MailOnline (Newspaper)|year=2010|accessdate=June 14, 2013}}</ref> | |||
==See also== | ==See also== | ||
{{Portal|Crustaceans}} | {{Portal|Crustaceans}} | ||
* ] | * ] | ||
* ] (device which administers a lethal electric shock to a crustacean) | |||
* ] | * ] | ||
* ] | * ] | ||
* ] | * ] | ||
* ] | |||
* ] | |||
* ] | |||
* ] | * ] | ||
* ] | * ] |
Revision as of 21:41, 3 March 2016
Pain in crustaceans is a contentious issue. Pain is a complex mental state, with a distinct perceptual quality but also associated with suffering, which is an emotional state. Because of this complexity, the presence of pain in an animal, or another human for that matter, cannot be determined unambiguously using observational methods, but the conclusion that animals experience pain is often inferred on the basis of likely presence of phenomenal consciousness which is deduced from comparative brain physiology as well as physical and behavioural reactions.
Most definitions of pain involve the two key components of nociception and suffering. Nociception is an ability to detect noxious stimuli which evokes a reflex response that moves the entire animal, or the affected part of the body, away from the source of the stimulus. The concept of nociception does not imply any adverse, subjective feeling - it is a simple reflex action, not processed by the brain. Suffering is the subjective experience of the pain; the internal, emotional interpretation of the nociceptive experience and is therefore a private, emotional experience. Whilst a nociceptive reflex quickly removes an animal from a potentially damaging stimulus, this will not prevent the animal from repeatedly exposing itself to this risk. Pain involves learning, which has the adaptive function of lowering the probability of the animal repeatedly exposing itself to potential injury.
Suffering cannot be directly measured in other animals, including other humans. Responses to putatively painful stimuli can be measured, but not the experience itself. To address this problem when assessing the capacity of other species to experience pain, argument by analogy is sometimes used. Another approach is to construct a list of criteria and assess whether the animal fulfills these.
Crustaceans fulfill several criteria proposed as indicating that non-human animals may experience pain. These fulfilled criteria include a suitable nervous system and sensory receptors, opioid receptors and reduced responses to noxious stimuli when given analgesics and local anaesthetics, physiological changes to noxious stimuli, displaying protective motor reactions, exhibiting avoidance learning and making trade-offs between noxious stimulus avoidance and other motivational requirements. In vertebrates, endogenous opioids are neurochemicals that moderate pain by interacting with opioid receptors. Opioid peptides and opioid receptors occur naturally in crustaceans, and although it was concluded in 2005 "...at present no certain conclusion can be drawn", more recent considerations suggest their presence along with related physiological and behavioural responses as indicating that crustaceans may experience pain. Opioids may moderate pain in crustaceans in a similar way to that in vertebrates.
If crustaceans feel pain, there are ethical and animal welfare implications including the consequences of exposure to pollutants, and practices involving commercial and recreational fishing, aquaculture, food preparation and for crustaceans used in scientific research.
Background
See also: Pain in animals and Pain in invertebratesThe possibility that crustaceans and other non-human animals may experience pain has a long history. Initially, this was based around theoretical and philosophical argument, but more recently has turned to scientific investigation.
Philosophy
The idea that non-human animals might not feel pain goes back to the 17th-century French philosopher, René Descartes, who argued that animals do not experience pain and suffering because they lack consciousness. In 1789, the British philosopher and social reformist, Jeremy Bentham, addressed in his book An Introduction to the Principles of Morals and Legislation the issue of our treatment of animals with the following often quoted words: "The question is not, Can they reason? nor, can they talk? but, Can they suffer?"
Peter Singer, a bioethicist and author of Animal Liberation published in 1975, suggested that consciousness is not necessarily the key issue: just because animals have smaller brains, or are ‘less conscious’ than humans, does not mean that they are not capable of feeling pain. He goes on further to argue that we do not assume newborn infants, people suffering from neurodegenerative brain diseases or people with learning disabilities experience less pain than we would.
Bernard Rollin, the principal author of two U.S. federal laws regulating pain relief for animals, writes that researchers remained unsure into the 1980s as to whether animals experience pain, and veterinarians trained in the U.S. before 1989 were taught to simply ignore animal pain. In his interactions with scientists and other veterinarians, Rollin was regularly asked to "prove" that animals are conscious, and to provide "scientifically acceptable" grounds for claiming that they feel pain.
Continuing into the 1990s, discussions were further developed on the roles that philosophy and science had in understanding animal cognition and mentality. In subsequent years, it was argued there was strong support for the suggestion that some animals (most likely amniotes) have at least simple conscious thoughts and feelings and that the view animals feel pain differently to humans is now a minority view.
Scientific investigation
Cambridge Declaration on Consciousness (2012)The absence of a neocortex does not appear to preclude an organism from experiencing affective states. Convergent evidence indicates that non-human animals have the neuroanatomical, neurochemical, and neurophysiological substrates of conscious states along with the capacity to exhibit intentional behaviors. Consequently, the weight of evidence indicates that humans are not unique in possessing the neurological substrates that generate consciousness. Non-human animals, including all mammals and birds, and many other creatures, including octopuses, also possess these neurological substrates.
In the 20th and 21st centuries, there were many scientific investigations of pain in non-human animals.
Mammals
At the turn of the century, studies were published showing that arthritic rats self-select analgesic opiates. In 2014, the veterinary Journal of Small Animal Practice published an article on the recognition of pain which started - "The ability to experience pain is universally shared by all mammals...".
Birds
At the same time as the investigations using arthritic rats, studies were published showing that birds with gait abnormalities self-select for a diet that contains carprofen, a human analgesic. In 2005, it was written "Avian pain is likely analogous to pain experienced by most mammals" and in 2014, "...it is accepted that birds perceive and respond to noxious stimuli and that birds feel pain"
Reptiles and amphibians
Veterinary articles have been published stating both reptiles and amphibians experience pain in a way analogous to humans, and that analgesics are effective in these two classes of vertebrates.
Argument by analogy
Argument by analogy is sometimes used to assess the capacity of other animals to experience pain. This is based on the principle that if a non-human animal's responses to noxious stimuli are similar to those of humans, they are likely to have had an analogous experience. For example, if a pin is stuck in a chimpanzee's finger and she rapidly withdraws her hand, then argument by analogy indicates that like humans, she felt pain.
In 2012 the American philosopher Gary Varner reviewed the research literature on pain in animals. His findings are summarised in the following table. Arguing by analogy, Varner claims that any animal which exhibits the properties listed in the table could be said to experience pain. On that basis, he concludes that all vertebrates, including fish, probably experience pain, but invertebrates (i.e. crustaceans) apart from cephalopods probably do not experience pain.
Argument by analogy | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Property | Invertebrates | Vertebrates | |||||||
Earthworms | Insects | Leeches/snails | Cephalopods | Fish | Amphibians | Reptiles | Birds | Mammals | |
Has nociceptors | ? | N | Y | ? | Y | Y | Y | Y | Y |
Has brain | N | N | N | Y | Y | Y | Y | Y | Y |
Nociceptors and brain linked | N | N | N | Y | Y | ? / Y | ? / Y | ? / Y | Y |
Has endogenous opiods | Y | Y | ? | N | Y | Y | Y | Y | Y |
Analgesics affect responses | ? | ? | ? | ? | Y | ? | ? | Y | Y |
Response to damaging stimuli similar to humans | N | N | ? | Y | Y | Y | Y | Y | Y |
The experience of pain
Although there are numerous definitions of pain, almost all involve two key components.
First, nociception is required. This is the ability to detect noxious stimuli which evoke a reflex response that rapidly moves the entire animal, or the affected part of its body, away from the source of the stimulus. The concept of nociception does not imply any adverse, subjective "feeling" – it is a reflex action. An example in humans would be the rapid withdrawal of a finger that has touched something hot – the withdrawal occurs before any sensation of pain is actually experienced.
The second component is the experience of "pain" itself, or suffering – the internal, emotional interpretation of the nociceptive experience. Again in humans, this is when the withdrawn finger begins to hurt, moments after the withdrawal. Pain is therefore a private, emotional experience. Nociceptive reflexes act to immediately remove the animal or part of the body from a (potentially) damaging stimulus. However, without learning from this experience, the animal would likely expose itself to the damaging stimulus repeatedly. Pain has the adaptive advantage that it invokes a level of learning, thereby preventing the animal from repeatedly exposing itself to potential injury.Pain cannot be directly measured in other animals, including other humans; responses to putatively painful stimuli can be measured, but not the experience itself. To address this problem when assessing the capacity of other species to experience pain, argument-by-analogy is used. This is based on the principle that if an animal responds to a stimulus in a similar way to ourselves, it is likely to have had an analogous experience.
Nociception
Main article: NociceptionIn vertebrates, nociceptive responses involve the transmission of a signal along a chain of nerve fibres from the site of a noxious stimulus at the periphery, to the spinal cord. This process evokes a reflex arc response such as flinching or immediate withdrawal of a limb, generated at the spinal cord and not involving the brain. Nociception is found, in one form or another, across all major animal taxa. Nociception can be observed using modern imaging techniques and both physiological and behavioural responses to nociception can be detected. Many crustacean species, including the rockpool prawn (Palaemon elegans), exhibit the caridoid escape reaction - an immediate, nociceptive, reflex tail-flick response to noxious stimuli (see here ).
Emotional pain
Main article: Psychological painSometimes a distinction is made between "physical pain" and "emotional" or "psychological pain". Emotional pain is the pain experienced in the absence of physical trauma, e.g. the pain experienced by humans after the loss of a loved one, or the break-up of a relationship. It has been argued that only primates and humans can feel "emotional pain", because they are the only animals that have a neocortex - a part of the brain's cortex considered to be the "thinking area". However, research has provided evidence that monkeys, dogs, cats and birds can show signs of emotional pain and display behaviours associated with depression during painful experience, i.e. lack of motivation, lethargy, anorexia, unresponsiveness to other animals.
Physical pain
Main article: PainThe nerve impulses of the nociception response may be conducted to the brain thereby registering the location, intensity, quality and unpleasantness of the stimulus. This subjective component of pain involves conscious awareness of both the sensation and the unpleasantness (the aversive, negative affect). The brain processes underlying conscious awareness of the unpleasantness (suffering), are not well understood.
There have been several published lists of criteria for establishing whether non-human animals experience pain, e.g. Some criteria that may indicate the potential of another species, including crustaceans, to feel pain include:
- Has a suitable nervous system and sensory receptors
- Has opioid receptors and shows reduced responses to noxious stimuli when given analgesics and local anaesthetics
- Physiological changes to noxious stimuli
- Displays protective motor reactions that might include reduced use of an affected area such as limping, rubbing, holding or autotomy
- Shows avoidance learning
- Shows trade-offs between noxious stimulus avoidance and other motivational requirements
- High cognitive ability and sentience
Research findings
The vast majority of research on pain in crustaceans has used (semi-) aquatic, decapoda species. Animals living in largely different environments are unlikely to have developed the same nociceptive or pain-detecting neural mechanisms. Different environments will result in diverse selection pressures on different animal groups, as well as exposing them to differing types of nociceptive stimuli. For example, crustaceans living in an aquatic world can maintain a certain level of buoyancy, so the risk of collision due to gravity is limited compared with a terrestrial vertebrate. Similarly, noxious chemicals might be diluted considerably in an aquatic environment compared to terrestrial. Therefore, nociceptive and pain systems in aquatic animals may be quite dissimilar to terrestrial animals.
Peripheral nervous system
Receptors
Crayfish (Procambarus clarkii) respond quickly and strongly to high temperatures, however, they show no response to low temperature stimuli, or, when stimulated with capsaicin or isothiocyanate (both are irritants to mammals). Noxious high temperatures are considered to be a potentially ecologically relevant noxious stimulus for crayfish that can be detected by sensory neurons, which may be specialized nociceptors.
The common brown shrimp Crangon crangon and the prawns Palaemon serratue and Palaemon elegana all exhibit a nociceptive sensitivity to both hot and cold temperatures. Both thermal sensitivity levels and nociceptive thresholds change with changes in acclimation temperature.
Nerve fibres
Crayfish have peripheral nerve fibres (see here ) which are responsive to noxious stimuli.
Neurons functionally specialized for nociception have been documented in other invertebrates including the leech Hirudo medicinalis, the nematode Caenorhabditis elegans and the molluscs Aplysia californica and Cepaea nemoralis. Changes in neuronal activity induced by noxious stimuli have been recorded in the nervous centres of Caenorhabditis elegans, Drosophila melanogaster and larval Manduca sexta.
Central nervous system
The bodies of crustaceans are segmented; there is one ganglion (cluster of nerve cells) per segment. Each ganglion receives sensory and movement information via nerves coming from the muscles, body wall, and appendages such as walking legs, swimmerets and mouthparts. The ganglia show great functional autonomy; information received by the ganglion is processed by the same ganglion, enabling a faster response than if the message had to travel all the way up up the animal's body to the brain and then back. Information can be exchanged between ganglia enabling the animal to perform coordinated movements.
When shore crabs (Hemigrapsus sanguineus) have formalin injected into the cheliped (claw), this evokes specific nociceptive behavior and neurochemical responses in the thoracic ganglia and the brain.
Brain
Bilaterally symmetrical animals characteristically have a collection of nervous tissue toward the anterior region of their body. Depending on the size, this may be termed the "cerebral ganglion" or the "brain". In decapods, the brain is divided into three main regions, the protocerebrum, which consists of two optic lobes, and the median protocerebrum.
In 2002, James Rose (University of Wyoming) and more recently Brian Key (University of Queensland) published reviews arguing that fish (and presumably crustaceans) cannot feel pain because they lack a neocortex in the brain and therefore do not have consciousness. This has been robustly contested. Animal behaviouralist, Temple Grandin, (Colorado State University) argues that animals could still have consciousness without a neocortex because "different species can use different brain structures and systems to handle the same functions." Lynne Sneddon (university of Liverpool) proposes that to suggest a function suddenly arises without a primitive form defies the laws of evolution. Other researchers also believe that animal consciousness does not require a neocortex, but can arise from homologous subcortical brain networks.
Opioid system and effects of analgesics
Opiates modulate nociception in vertebrates. In vertebrates, morphine is an analgesic and therefore ameliorates the sensation of pain. Naloxone is an opioid-receptor antagonist and therefore blocks the effects of morphine. Opioid modulation of nociception has been demonstrated in several invertebrate species.
The first report of opiate effects in invertebrates is based on the behavioural responses of the crustacean mantis shrimp Squilla mantis. These shrimp respond to an electric shock with an immediate, violent, convulsive-live flexion of the body. If they are injected with morphine-HCL, this produces a dose-dependent analgesia by increasing the intensity threshold to the shock. This effect is fully blocked by naloxone.
Crustaceans have a functional opioid system which includes the presence of opioid receptors similar to those of mammals. Delta- and Kappa-opioid receptors have been described in crustaceans. RT-PCR research on the American lobster (Homarus americanus) has revealed the presence of a Mu-opioid receptor transcript in neural and immune tissues, which exhibits a 100% sequence identity with its human counterpart.
In the American lobster, endogenous morphine is found in the haemolymph and ventral nerve cord. In lobsters which have had a pereiopod (walking leg) cut off or been injected with the irritant lipopolysaccharide, the endogenous morphine levels initially increased by 24% for haemolymph and 48% for the nerve cord.
In vertebrates, opioid peptides (i.e., enkephalins) have been shown to be involved in nociception. Leu-enkephalin and Met-enkephalin are present in the thoracic ganglia of the shore crab, Carcinus maenas.
Both morphine and naloxone affect the estuarine crab (Neohelice granulata) in a similar way to their effects on vertebrates: injections of morphine produce a dose-dependent reduction of their defensive response to an electric shock. However, it has been suggested the attenuated defensive response could originate from either the analgesic or sedative properties of morphine, or both. One study on the effects of a danger stimulus on the crab Chasmagnathus granulatus reported this induces opioid analgesia, which is influenced by naloxone. In American lobsters, the response of endogenous morphine in both haemocytes and neural cells to noxious stimuli are mediated by naloxone.
When the antennae of rockpool prawns Palaemon elegans are rubbed with sodium hydroxide or acetic acid (both are irritants in mammals), they increase grooming and rubbing of the afflicted area against the side of the tank; this reaction is inhibited by benzocaine (a local anaesthetic in mammals), even though control prawns treated with only anaesthetic do not show reduced activity.
Eyestalk ablation is the removal of one or both eyestalks from a crustacean. It is routinely practiced on female prawns in almost every marine shrimp maturation or reproduction facility in the world, both research and commercial. The aim of ablation is to stimulate the female shrimp to develop mature ovaries and spawn. In Macrobrachium americanum, prawns treated with lignocaine (a local anaesthetic in mammals), showed less rubbing, flicking and sheltering than those without the anaesthetic.
One study on reducing the stress of prawns resulting from transportation concluded that Aqui-STM and clove oil (a natural anaesthetic) may be suitable anaesthetic treatments for prawns.
Physiological responses
Higher levels of stress, as measured by lactate, occur in shore crabs exposed to brief electric shock compared to non-shocked controls. However, shocked crabs showed more vigorous behaviour than controls, possibly indicating it is increased behaviour causing the increased lactate. But, when crabs with the same level of behaviour are matched, shocked crabs still have a stronger stress response compared with controls. The authors suggested that their findings, coupled with previous findings of long-term motivational change and avoidance learning, "...fulfils the criteria expected of a pain experience".
Injection of formalin into the cheliped of shore crabs (Hemigrapsus sanguineus) evokes specific nociceptive behavior and neurochemical responses in the brain and thoracic ganglion.
Protective responses
Most species of hermit crabs have long, spirally curved abdomens, which are soft, unlike the hard, calcified abdomens seen in related crustaceans. They protect themselves from predators by entering a salvaged empty seashell, into which they can retract their whole body. As they grow, they must leave their shell and find another larger, more suitable shell (see here ). Their shells are therefore highly valuable to them. When hermit crabs (Pagurus bernhardus) are given an electric shock, they leave their shells and subsequently perform prolonged abdominal grooming at the site of where they received the shock.
Male Chasmagnathus granulatus crabs exhibit a "defensive response" to electric shocks. During a study on the ability of shore crabs (Carcinus maenas) learning to avoid an electrical shock, it was observed that many crabs emerged from the dark shelter to avoid the shock thus entering a brightly light area which would normally be avoided. Immediately after the injection of formalin (an irritant in mammals) or saline into one cheliped (the leg which ends with the claw), shore crabs move quickly into the corner of the aquarium and "freeze" after 2 to 3 seconds. After 1 to 3 minutes, these injected animals are fidgety and exhibit a wide range of movements such as flexion, extension, shaking or rubbing the affected claw. Formalin-treated animals show 20-times more rubbing behaviour during the first minute after injection than saline-treated crabs. Intense rubbing of the claw results in autotomy (shedding) in 20% of animals of the formalin-treated group whereas saline-injected crabs do not autotomise the injected cheliped. During the 10 minutes after injection, crabs in the formalin-treated group tried to use the intact cheliped, guarding the damaged cheliped. The scientists conducting this study commented "...the present results obtained in crabs may be indicative of pain experience rather than relating to a simple nociceptive reflex". Other species of crab have been shown to autotomise legs when placed on a hot plate or exposed to a small electric shock.
When the antennae of rockpool prawns (Palaemon elegans) are rubbed with sodium hydroxide or acetic acid (both are irritants), the animals show increased grooming and rubbing of the afflicted area against the side of the tank. Furthermore, this reaction is inhibited by a local anaesthetic, even though control prawns treated with only anaesthetic did not show reduced activity. Other scientists suggested the rubbing may reflect an attempt to clean the affected area as application of anaesthetic alone caused an increase in grooming.
In one study, no behavioural or neural changes in three different crustacean species (red swamp crayfish (Procambarus clarkii), white shrimp (Litopenaeus setiferus) and Palaemonetes sp.) were observed in response to noxious acids or bases.
Avoidance learning
Shore crabs quickly (within 1 or 2 trials) learn to avoid one of two dark shelters if choosing that shelter consistently results in them receiving an electric shock. The crayfish Procambarus clarkii and the crab Chasmagnathus granulatus learn to associate an electric shock with a light turning on, or with the occupancy of the light compartment of the aquarium, respectively. They quickly learn to respond to these associations by walking to a safe area in which the shock is not delivered (crayfish) or by refraining from entering the light compartment (crab).
Trade-offs in motivation
Nociceptive responses are reflexes that do not change regardless of motivational priorities. In contrast, a painful experience may change the motivation for normal behavioural responses, thereby indicating a plastic response to an aversive stimulus, rather than a simple reflex response.
In 2009, Elwood and Mirjam Appel showed that hermit crabs make motivational trade-offs between electric shocks and the quality of the shells they inhabit. In particular, as hermit crabs are shocked more intensely, they become increasingly willing to leave their current shells for new shells, and they spend less time deciding whether to enter those new shells. Moreover, because the researchers did not offer the new shells until after the electrical stimulation had ended, the change in motivational behavior was the result of memory of the noxious event, not an immediate reflex. It was also shown that hermit crabs experiencing increasing electric shocks left their shell at a reduced intensity intensity when the shell was from a less preferred species than did those in shells of a more desirable species. This demonstrates that hermit crabs are willing to risk predator attack by evacuating their shells to avoid a noxious stimulus and that this is dependent upon how valuable the shell is.
Shore crabs (Carcinus maenas) also show motivational trade-offs; they will discard a valuable resource (a preferred shelter) to avoid future encounters with painful stimuli, thereby indicating avoidance learning - a key criterion of the ability to experience pain.
A 2014 study on crayfish (Procambarus clarkii) tested their responses in a fear paradigm, the elevated plus maze in which animals choose to walk on an elevated cross which offers both aversive and preferable conditions (in this case, two arms were lit and two were dark). Crayfish which experienced an electric shock displayed enhanced fearfulness or anxiety as demonstrated by their preference for the dark arms more than the light. Furthermore, shocked crayfish had relatively higher brain serotonin concentrations coupled with elevated blood glucose, which suggests a stress response. A follow-up study using the same species showed the intensity of the anxiety-like behaviour, presumably resulting from the pain, was dependent on the intensity of the electric shock until reaching a plateau. Such a quantitative relationship between stress and anxiety is also a very common feature of human and vertebrate anxiety.
Opinion
Advocates for Animals, a Scottish animal welfare group, stated in 2005 that "scientific evidence ... strongly suggests that there is a potential for decapod crustaceans and cephalopods to experience pain and suffering". This is primarily due to "The likelihood that decapod crustaceans can feel pain is supported by the fact that they have been shown to have opioid receptors and to respond to opioids (analgesics such as morphine) in a similar way to vertebrates." Similarities between decapod and vertebrate stress systems and behavioral responses to noxious stimuli were given as additional evidence for the capacity of decapods to experience pain.
In 2005 a review of the literature by the Norwegian Scientific Committee for Food Safety tentatively concluded that "it is unlikely that can feel pain," though they note that "there is apparently a paucity of exact knowledge on sentience in crustaceans, and more research is needed." This conclusion is based on the lobster's simple nervous system. The report assumes that the violent reaction of lobsters to boiling water is a reflex response (i.e. does not involve conscious perception) to noxious stimuli.
A European Food Safety Authority (EFSA) 2005 publication stated that the largest of decapod crustaceans have complex behaviour, some degree of awareness, a pain system and considerable learning abilities. Based on this evidence, they placed all decapod crustaceans into the same category of research-animal protection as vertebrates.
Pain during killing
The EFSA summarized that the killing methods most likely to cause pain and distress are:
- Any procedures whereby the abdomen is separated from the thorax
- The removal of tissue, flesh, or limbs while the crustacean is alive and fully conscious
- Placing crustaceans in slowly heated water to the boiling point
- Placing crustaceans directly into boiling water
- Placing marine crustaceans in fresh water
- Unfocused microwaving of the body as opposed to focal application to the head
A device called the CrustaStun has been invented to electrocute shellfish such as lobsters, crabs, and crayfish before cooking. The device works by applying a 110-volt, 2 to 5 amp electrical charge to the animal. The CrustaStun renders the shellfish unconscious in 0.3 seconds and kills the animal in 5 to 10 seconds, compared to 3 minutes to kill a lobster by boiling.
See also
- Animal cognition
- Declawing of crabs
- Emotion in animals
- Moral status of animals in the ancient world
- Pain and suffering in laboratory animals
- Sentience
References
- Abbott, F.V., Franklin, K.B.J. and Westbrook, R.F. (1995). "The formalin test: Scoring properties of the first and second phases of the pain response in rats". Pain. 60 (1): 91–102. doi:10.1016/0304-3959(94)00095-V. PMID 7715946.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Key, B. (2015). "Fish do not feel pain and its implications for understanding phenomenal consciousness". Biology and Philosophy. 30 (2): 149–165. doi:10.1007/s10539-014-9469-4.
- ^ Sømme, L. (2005). "Sentience and pain in invertebrates: Report to Norwegian Scientific Committee for Food Safety". Norwegian University of Life Sciences, Oslo.
- ^ Dyuizen, I.V., Kotsyuba, E.P. and Lamash, N.E. (2012). "Changes in the nitric oxide system in the shore crab Hemigrapsus sanguineus (Crustacea, decapoda) CNS induced by a nociceptive stimulus". Journal of Experimental Biology. 215 (15): 2668–2676.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Elwood, R.W. and Adams, L. (2015). "Electric shock causes physiological stress responses in shore crabs, consistent with prediction of pain". Biology Letters. 11 (11): 20150800.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Carbone, L. (2004). What Animals Want: Expertise and Advocacy in Laboratory Animal Welfare Policy. Oxford University Press. p. 149.
- Radner, D. and Radner, M. (1989). Animal Consciousness. Prometheus Books: Buffalo.
{{cite book}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - Harrison, P. (1992). "Descartes on animals". The Philosophical Quarterly. 42 (167): 219–227. doi:10.2307/2220217. JSTOR 2220217.
- "Bentham, J. (1879). An Introduction to the Principles of Morals and Legislation. Clarendon Press.
- ^ Sneddon, L.U. "Can animals feel pain?". The Welcome Trust. Retrieved September 24, 2015.
- ^ Rollin, B. (1989). The Unheeded Cry: Animal Consciousness, Animal Pain, and Science. Oxford University Press, pp. xii, 117-118, cited in Carbone 2004, p. 150.
- Allen, C. (1998). "Assessing animal cognition: Ethological and philosophical perspectives". Journal of Animal Science. 76 (1): 42–47. PMID 9464883.
- Griffin, D.R. and Speck, G.B. (2004). "New evidence of animal consciousness". Animal Cognition. 7 (1): 5–18. doi:10.1007/s10071-003-0203-x. PMID 14658059.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Low, P. (July 7, 2012). Jaak Panksepp, Diana Reiss, David Edelman, Bruno Van Swinderen, Philip Low and Christof Koch (ed.). "The Cambridge declaration on consciousness" (PDF). University of Cambridge.
{{cite web}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: editors list (link) - Colpaert, F.C., Tarayre, J.P., Alliaga, M., Slot. L.A.B., Attal, N. and Koek, W. (2001). "Opiate self-administration as a measure of chronic nociceptive pain in arthritic rats". Pain. 91: 33–45. doi:10.1016/s0304-3959(00)00413-9.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - Mathews, K., Kronen, P.W., Lascelles, D., Nolan, A., Robertson, S., Steagall, P.V., Wright, B. and Yamashita, K. (2014). "Guidelines for recognition, assessment and treatment of pain". Journal of Small Animal Practice. 55 (6): E10 – E68. doi:10.1111/jsap.12200.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - Danbury, T.C., Weeks, C.A., Chambers, J.P., Waterman-Pearson, A.E. and Kestin, S.C. (2000). "Self-selection of the analgesic drug carprofen by lame broiler chickens". The Veterinary Record. 146 (11): 307–311. doi:10.1136/vr.146.11.307.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - Machin, K.L. (2005). "Avian analgesia". Seminars in Avian and Exotic Pet Medicine. 14 (4): 236–242. doi:10.1053/j.saep.2005.09.004.
- Paul-Murphy, J. and Hawkins, M.G. (2014). "Chapter 26 - Bird-specific considerations: recognizing pain in pet birds.". In Gaynor, J.S. and Muir III, W. W. (ed.). Handbook of Veterinary Pain Management. Elsevier Health Sciences.
{{cite book}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - Mosley, C.A. (2005). "Anesthesia and analgesia in reptiles". Seminars in Avian and Exotic Pet Medicine. 14 (4): 243–262. doi:10.1053/j.saep.2005.09.005.
- Mosley, C. (2011). "Pain and nociception in reptiles". Veterinary Clinics of North America: Exotic Animal Practice. 14 (1): 45–60. doi:10.1016/j.cvex.2010.09.009.
- Sladky, K.K. and Mans, C. (2012). "Clinical analgesia in reptiles". Journal of Exotic Pet Medicine. 21 (2): 158–167. doi:10.1053/j.jepm.2012.02.012.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - Machin, K.L. (1999). "Amphibian pain and analgesia". Journal of Zoo and Wildlife Medicine. 30: 2–10. JSTOR 20095815.
- Machin, K.L. (2001). "Fish, amphibian, and reptile analgesia". The Veterinary Clinics of North America. Exotic Animal Practice. 4 (1): 19–33.
- Stevens, C.W. (2011). "Analgesia in amphibians: preclinical studies and clinical applications". Veterinary Clinics of North America: Exotic Animal Practice. 14 (1): 33–44. doi:10.1016/j.cvex.2010.09.007.
- ^ Gherardi, F. (2009). "Behavioural indicators of pain in crustacean decapods". Annali dell'Istituto superiore di sanità. 45 (4): 432–438.
- Sherwin, C.M. (2001). "Can invertebrates suffer? Or, how robust is argument-by-analogy?". Animal Welfare (supplement). 10: S103 – S118.
- Elwood, R.W. (2011). "Pain and suffering in invertebrates?" (PDF). Institute of Laboratory Animal Resources Journal. 52 (2): 175–84.
- ^ Varner, G.E. (2012). "Chapter 5 - Which animals are sentient?". Personhood, Ethics, and Animal Cognition: Situating Animals in Hare’s Two Level Utilitarianism. Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199758784.001.0001. ISBN 9780199758784. Table 5.2, page 113.
- Kristin, A. (2014). The Animal Mind: An Introduction to the Philosophy of Animal Cognition (section 3.6.2). Routledge. ISBN 9781317676751.
- ^ Sneddon, L.U. (2004). "Evolution of nociception in vertebrates: comparative analysis of lower vertebrates". Brain Research Reviews. 46: 123–130. doi:10.1016/j.brainresrev.2004.07.007.
- Elwood, R.W. (2016). "A single strand of argument with unfounded conclusion". Animal Sentience: An Interdisciplinary Journal on Animal Feeling. 1 (3): 19.
- ^ Barr, S., Laming, P.R., Dick, J.T.A. and Elwood, R.W. (2008). "Nociception or pain in a decapod crustacean?". Animal Behaviour. 75 (3): 745–751. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.07.004.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - Sneddon, L.U., Elwood, R.W., Adamo, S.A. and Leach, M.C. (2014). "Defining and assessing animal pain". Animal Behaviour. 97: 201–212. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.09.007.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Elwood, R.W., Barr, S. and Patterson, L. (2009). "Pain and stress in crustaceans?". Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 118 (3): 128–136. doi:10.1016/j.applanim.2009.02.018.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Sneddon, L.U. (2015). "Pain in aquatic animals". Journal of Experimental Biology. 218 (7): 967–976.
- ^ Puri, S. and Faulkes, Z. (2015). "Can crayfish take the heat? Procambarus clarkii show nociceptive behaviour to high temperature stimuli, but not low temperature or chemical stimuli". Biology Open: BIO20149654.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - Davies, C.A. (1981). Thermal sensitivity and behaviour in the brown shrimp and some related crustacea (Doctoral dissertation) (Thesis). University College of Swansea.
- Sandeman, D. (1990). "Structural and functional levels in the organization of decapod crustacean brains". Frontiers in Crustacean Neurobiology. Birkhäuser Basel. pp. 223–239.
- Rose, J.D. (2002). "The neurobehavioral nature of fishes and the question of awareness and pain" (PDF). Reviews in Fisheries Science. 10 (1): 1–38. doi:10.1080/20026491051668.
- Rose, J.D. (2002). "Do fish feel pain?". Retrieved September 27, 2007.
- Grandin, T. and Johnson, C. (2005). Animals in Translation. New York: Scribner. pp. 183–184. ISBN 0-7432-4769-8.
{{cite book}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - Sneddon, L.U. (2012). "Pain perception in fish: Evidence and implications for the use of fish". Journal of Consciousness Studies. 18: 209–229.
- Nieto-Fernandez, F., Andrieux, S., Idrees, S., Bagnall, C., Pryor, S.C. and Sood, R. (2009). "The effect of opioids and their antagonists on the nocifensive response of Caenorhabditis elegans to noxious thermal stimuli". Invertebrate Neuroscience. 9: 195–200.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - Maldonado, H. and Miralto, A. (1982). "Effect of morphine and naloxone on a defensive response of the mantis shrimp (Squilla mantis)". Journal of Comparative Physiology. 147 (4): 455–459.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - Hanke, J., Willig, A., Yinon, U. and Jaros, P.P. (1997). "Delta and kappa opioid receptors in eyestalk ganglia of a crustacean". Brain Research. 744 (2): 279–284.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Casares, F.M., McElroy, A., Mantione, K.J., Baggermann, G., Zhu, W. and Stefano, G.B. (2005). "The American lobster, Homarus americanus, contains morphine that is coupled to nitric oxide release in its nervous and immune tissues: Evidence for neurotransmitter and hormonal signaling". Neuro Endocrinol. Lett. 26: 89–97.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - Lüschen, W., Buck, F., Willig, A. and Jaros, P.P. (1991). "Isolation, sequence analysis, and physiological properties of enkephalins in the nervous tissue of the shore crab Carcinus maenas L.". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 88 (19): 8671–8675.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Lozada, M., Romano, A. and Maldonado, H. (1988). "Effect of morphine and naloxone on a defensive response of the crab Chasmagnathus granulatus". Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior. 30 (3): 635–640. doi:10.1016/0091-3057(88)90076-7.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - Dyakonova, V.E. (2001). "Role of opioid peptides in behavior of invertebrates". Journal of Evolutionary Biochemistry and Physiology. 37 (4): 335–347. doi:10.1023/A:1012910525424.
- Valeggia, C., Fernandez-Duque, E. and Maldonado, H. (1989). "Brain Research". 481 (2): 304–308.
{{cite journal}}
: Cite journal requires|journal=
(help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - http://www.plosone.org/article/info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fjournal.pone.0024427
- Diarte-Plata, G., Sainz-Hernández, J.C., Aguiñaga-Cruz, J.A., Fierro-Coronado, J.A., Polanco-Torres, A. and Puente-Palazuelos, C. (2012). "Eyestalk ablation procedures to minimize pain in the freshwater prawn Macrobrachium americanum". Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 140 (3): 172–178.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - Coyle, S.D., Dasgupta, S., Tidwell, J.H., Beavers, T., Bright, L.A. and Yasharian, D.K. (2005). "Comparative efficacy of anesthetics for the freshwater prawn Macrobrachiurn rosenbergii". Journal of the World Aquaculture Society. 36 (3): 282–290.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - Dyuizen, I.V., Kotsyuba, E.P. and Lamash, N.E., (2012). Changes in the nitric oxide system in the shore crab Hemigrapsus sanguineus (Crustacea, decapoda) CNS induced by a nociceptive stimulus. The Journal of Experimental Biology, 215: 2668-2676
- Appel, M. and Elwood, R.W. (2009). "Gender differences, responsiveness and memory of a potentially painful event in hermit crabs". Animal Behaviour. 78: 1373–1379. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.09.008.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Robert W. Elwood and Barry Magee (2013). "Shock avoidance by discrimination learning in the shore crab (Carcinus maenas) is consistent with a key criterion for pain". Journal of Experimental Biology. doi:10.1242/jeb.072041.
- Ian Sample (November 8, 2007). "Blow for fans of boiled lobster: crustaceans feel pain, study says". The Guardian.
- Puri, S. and Faulkes, Z. (2010). "Do decapod crustaceans have nociceptors for extreme pH?". PLOS ONE. 5 (4): e10244. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0010244.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link) - Magee, B. and Elwood, R.W. (2013). "Shock avoidance by discrimination learning in the shore crab (Carcinus maenas) is consistent with a key criterion for pain". Journal of Experimental Biology. 216 (3): 353–358.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - Robert W. Elwood & Mirjam Appel (2009). "Pain experience in hermit crabs?" (PDF). Animal Behaviour. 77 (5): 1243–1246. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.01.028.
- Fossat, P., Bacqué-Cazenave, J., De Deurwaerdère, P., Delbecque, J.-P. and Cattaert, D. (2014). "Anxiety-like behavior in crayfish is controlled by serotonin". Science. 344: 1293–1297. doi:10.1126/science.1248811.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - Fossat, P., Bacqué-Cazenave, J., De Deurwaerdère, P., Cattaert, D. and Delbecque, J.P. (2015). "Serotonin, but not dopamine, controls the stress response and anxiety-like behavior in the crayfish Procambarus clarkii". Journal of Experimental Biology. 218 (17): 2745–2752.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - Cephalopods and decapod crustaceans: their capacity to experience pain and suffering (PDF). Advocates for Animals. 2005.
- "Opinion on the aspects of the biology and welfare of animals used for experimental and other scientific purposes". The EFSA Journal. 292. European Food Safety Authority: 1–46. 2005.
- Yue, S. (2008). "The welfare of crustaceans at slaughter". Humane Society of the United States.
- McSmith, A. (2009). "I'll have my lobster electrocuted, please". The Independent (Newspaper). Retrieved June 14, 2013.
- Anon. (2010). "CrustaStun: The 'humane' gadget that kills lobsters with a single jolt of electricity". MailOnline (Newspaper). Retrieved June 14, 2013.
Further reading
- Grandin, Temple and Deesing, Mark (2003 update) "Distress in Animals: Is it Fear, Pain or Physical Stress?" American Board of Veterinary Practitioners – Symposium 2002.
- "Consider the Lobster" Essay by David Foster Wallace, published in Gourmet, August 2004.
Animal cognition | |
---|---|
Cognition | |
Intelligence | |
Pain | |
Relation to brain | |