Revision as of 02:00, 29 July 2016 editCornellier (talk | contribs)Extended confirmed users, Pending changes reviewers11,118 edits →Morphology: add brief description of noun declinationTag: Visual edit← Previous edit | Revision as of 12:32, 29 July 2016 edit undoCornellier (talk | contribs)Extended confirmed users, Pending changes reviewers11,118 edits Add descriptions of verb forms, rm WP:OVERLINK, copy editTags: nowiki added Visual editNext edit → | ||
Line 48: | Line 48: | ||
==Phonology== | ==Phonology== | ||
⚫ | ] has been reconstructed. The ] is reconstructed today as having had variable lexical stress, which could appear on any syllable and whose position often varied among different members of a paradigm (e.g. between singular and plural of a verbal paradigm). Stressed syllables received a higher pitch, therefore it is often said that PIE had ]. The location of the stress is associated with ] variations, especially between normal-grade vowels (*/e/ and */o/) and zero-grade (i.e. lack of a vowel), but not entirely predictable from it. | ||
] has been reconstructed to a large extent. Some uncertainties still remain, such as the exact nature of the three series of stops, and the exact number and distribution of the vowels. The ] | |||
⚫ | is |
||
To account for mismatches between the accent of Vedic Sanskrit and Ancient Greek, as well as a few other phenomena, a few historical linguists prefer to reconstruct PIE as a ] where each ] had an inherent tone; the sequence of tones in a word then evolved, according to that hypothesis, into the placement of lexical stress in different ways in different IE branches. | The accent is best preserved in ] and (in the case of nouns) ], and indirectly attested in a number of phenomena in other IE languages. To account for mismatches between the accent of Vedic Sanskrit and Ancient Greek, as well as a few other phenomena, a few historical linguists prefer to reconstruct PIE as a ] where each ] had an inherent tone; the sequence of tones in a word then evolved, according to that hypothesis, into the placement of lexical stress in different ways in different IE branches. | ||
==Morphology== | ==Morphology== | ||
Line 62: | Line 61: | ||
===Noun=== | ===Noun=== | ||
] |
] are declined for eight or nine cases:<ref>{{cite book|last1=Fortson|first1=Benjamin|title=Indo-European language and culture : an introduction|date=2004|publisher=Blackwell|location=Malden (USA)|isbn=1-4051-0316-7|page=102|accessdate=25 July 2016}}</ref> | ||
*]: marks the ] of a ] or the ] noun or predicate adjective, as opposed to its ] or other ]. Generally, the noun |
*]: marks the ] of a ] or the ] noun or predicate adjective, as opposed to its ] or other ]. Generally, the noun that is doing something is in the nominative, and the nominative is the dictionary form of the noun. | ||
*]: |
*]: for the ] of a ]. | ||
*]: marks a ] as modifying another noun. | *]: marks a ] as modifying another noun. | ||
*]: used to indicate the |
*]: used to indicate the noun to which something is given, as in "Maria gave Jacob a drink". Here, Jacob is an indirect dative. | ||
*]: used to indicate that a noun is the ''instrument'' or means by or with which the |
*]: used to indicate that a noun is the ''instrument'' or means by or with which the subject achieves or accomplishes an action. The noun may be either a physical object or an abstract concept. | ||
*]: used |
*]: used to express motion away from something. | ||
*]: corresponds vaguely to the English prepositions "in", "on", "at", and "by" |
*]: corresponds vaguely to the English prepositions "in", "on", "at", and "by". | ||
*]: used for a ] that identifies a person (animal, object, etc.) being addressed or, occasionally, the] of that noun. A vocative expression is an expression of direct address where the identity of the party spoken to is set forth expressly within a sentence. For example, in the sentence, "I don't know, John", ''John'' is a vocative expression that indicates the party being |
*]: used for a ] that identifies a person (animal, object, etc.) being addressed or, occasionally, the ] of that noun. A vocative expression is an expression of direct address where the identity of the party spoken to is set forth expressly within a sentence. For example, in the sentence, "I don't know, John", ''John'' is a vocative expression that indicates the party being addressed. | ||
*]: a type of the ] |
*]: a type of the ]. | ||
There were three |
There were three ]<nowiki/>s: | ||
*masculine | *masculine | ||
*feminine | *feminine | ||
Line 139: | Line 138: | ||
===Verb===<!-- This section is linked from ] --> | ===Verb===<!-- This section is linked from ] --> | ||
] were complex and, like the noun, exhibits a system of ablaut. The most basic categorization for the Indo-European verb was ]. Verbs were classed as: | ] were complex and, like the noun, exhibits a system of ablaut. The most basic categorization for the Indo-European verb was ]. Verbs were classed as: | ||
*]: verbs that depict a state of being |
*]: verbs that depict a state of being | ||
*]: verbs depicting ongoing, habitual or repeated action | *]: verbs depicting ongoing, habitual or repeated action | ||
*]: verbs depicting a completed action or actions viewed as an entire process). | *]: verbs depicting a completed action or actions viewed as an entire process). | ||
Verbs have at least four ]s: | Verbs have at least four ]s: | ||
*]: indicates that something is a statement of fact; in other words, to express what the speaker considers to be a known state of affairs, as in ]. | |||
*] | |||
*]: forms commands or requests, including the giving of prohibition or permission, or any other kind of ] or ]. | |||
*] | |||
*]: used to express various states of unreality such as wish, emotion, possibility, judgment, opinion, obligation, or action that has not yet occurred | |||
*] | |||
*]: indicates a wish or hope. It is similar to the ], and is closely related to the ]. | |||
*] | |||
Verbs had two ]s: | Verbs had two ]s: | ||
] | * ]: used in a clause whose subject expresses the main verb's ]. | ||
*] | |||
*]: for the ] and the ]. | |||
Verbs had three ]s: (first, second and third) | Verbs had three ]s: (first, second and third) | ||
Verbs had three ] | Verbs had three ]<nowiki/>s | ||
*singular | *singular | ||
*] | *]: referring to precisely two of the entities (objects or persons) identified by the noun or pronoun. | ||
*] |
*]: a number other than singular or dual. | ||
Verbs were also marked by a highly developed system of ]s, one for each combination of tense and voice, and an assorted array of ]s and adjectival formations. | Verbs were also marked by a highly developed system of ]s, one for each combination of tense and voice, and an assorted array of ]s and adjectival formations. | ||
Line 274: | Line 275: | ||
|{{PIE|*déḱmt}} | |{{PIE|*déḱmt}} | ||
|} | |} | ||
Rather than specifically 100, {{PIE|''*ḱm̥tóm''}} may originally have meant "a large number".<ref>{{Citation|last=Lehmann|first=Winfried P|title=Theoretical Bases of Indo-European Linguistics|year=1993|postscript=|pages=252–55|location=London|publisher=Routledge|isbn=0-415-08201-3}}</ref> | |||
===Particle=== | ===Particle=== | ||
Line 282: | Line 283: | ||
The ] of the older Indo-European languages has been studied in earnest since at least the late nineteenth century, by such scholars as ] and ]. In the second half of the twentieth century, interest in the topic increased and led to reconstructions of Proto-Indo-European syntax.<ref>{{cite encyclopedia |title=Preface |encyclopedia=Proto-Indo-European Syntax and its Development |editor1-first=Leonid |editor1-last=Kulikov |editor2-first=Nikolaos |editor2-last=Lavidas |publisher=John Benjamins |year=2015}}</ref> | The ] of the older Indo-European languages has been studied in earnest since at least the late nineteenth century, by such scholars as ] and ]. In the second half of the twentieth century, interest in the topic increased and led to reconstructions of Proto-Indo-European syntax.<ref>{{cite encyclopedia |title=Preface |encyclopedia=Proto-Indo-European Syntax and its Development |editor1-first=Leonid |editor1-last=Kulikov |editor2-first=Nikolaos |editor2-last=Lavidas |publisher=John Benjamins |year=2015}}</ref> | ||
Since all the early attested IE languages were inflectional, PIE is thought to have relied largely on morphological markers, rather than ], to signal ] relationships within sentences.{{r|eiec}} Still, a default (]) word order is thought to have existed in PIE. This was reconstructed by ] as being ] (SVO), based on evidence in |
Since all the early attested IE languages were inflectional, PIE is thought to have relied largely on morphological markers, rather than ], to signal ] relationships within sentences.{{r|eiec}} Still, a default (]) word order is thought to have existed in PIE. This was reconstructed by ] as being ] (SVO), based on evidence in Vedic Sanskrit, and the SVO hypothesis still has some adherents, but {{as of|2015|lc=y}} the "broad consensus" among PIE scholars is that PIE would have been a ] (SOV) language.<ref>{{cite encyclopedia |title=Proto-Indo-European verb-finality: Reconstruction, typology, validation |first=Hans Henrich |last=Hock |authorlink=Hans Henrich Hock |encyclopedia=Proto-Indo-European Syntax and its Development |editor1-first=Leonid |editor1-last=Kulikov |editor2-first=Nikolaos |editor2-last=Lavidas |publisher=John Benjamins |year=2015}}</ref> | ||
The SOV default word order with other orders used to express emphasis (e.g., ] to emphasize the verb) is attested in ], ], ] and ], while traces of it can be found in the ] personal pronouns of the ].<ref name="eiec">{{Cite encyclopedia |editor-first1=J. P. |editor-last1=Mallory |editor-first2=Douglas Q. |editor-last2=Adams |encyclopedia=] |title=Proto-Indo-European |publisher=Taylor & Francis |year=1997 |page=463}}</ref> A shift from OV to VO order is posited to have occurred in late PIE, since many of the descendant languages have this order: modern Greek, ] and ] prefer SVO, ] has VSO as the default order, and even the ] show some signs of this word order shift.{{r|lehmann}} The inconsistent order preference in Baltic, Slavic and Germanic can be attributed to contact with outside OV languages.<ref name="lehmann">{{Cite book |title=Proto-Indo-European Syntax |first=Winfred P. |last=Lehmann |authorlink=Winfred P. Lehmann |publisher=University of Texas Press |year=1974 |page=250 |url=http://www.utexas.edu/cola/centers/lrc/books/pies07.html#txu-oclc-922888.xml-div-d0e38555}}</ref> | The SOV default word order with other orders used to express emphasis (e.g., ] to emphasize the verb) is attested in ], ], ] and ], while traces of it can be found in the ] personal pronouns of the ].<ref name="eiec">{{Cite encyclopedia |editor-first1=J. P. |editor-last1=Mallory |editor-first2=Douglas Q. |editor-last2=Adams |encyclopedia=] |title=Proto-Indo-European |publisher=Taylor & Francis |year=1997 |page=463}}</ref> A shift from OV to VO order is posited to have occurred in late PIE, since many of the descendant languages have this order: modern Greek, ] and ] prefer SVO, ] has VSO as the default order, and even the ] show some signs of this word order shift.{{r|lehmann}} The inconsistent order preference in Baltic, Slavic and Germanic can be attributed to contact with outside OV languages.<ref name="lehmann">{{Cite book |title=Proto-Indo-European Syntax |first=Winfred P. |last=Lehmann |authorlink=Winfred P. Lehmann |publisher=University of Texas Press |year=1974 |page=250 |url=http://www.utexas.edu/cola/centers/lrc/books/pies07.html#txu-oclc-922888.xml-div-d0e38555}}</ref> | ||
Line 291: | Line 292: | ||
Published PIE sample texts: | Published PIE sample texts: | ||
* ] (''Avis akvasas ka'') by ] (1868), modernized by |
* ] (''Avis akvasas ka'') by ] (1868), modernized by Hermann Hirt (1939) and ] and ] (1979) | ||
* ] (''rēḱs deiwos-kʷe'') by S. K. Sen, E. P. Hamp et al. (1994) | * ] (''rēḱs deiwos-kʷe'') by S. K. Sen, E. P. Hamp et al. (1994) | ||
* "Before Babel: Ancient tales from Genesis retold in reconstructed Proto-Indo-European", by B. Barrois (2015), . | * "Before Babel: Ancient tales from Genesis retold in reconstructed Proto-Indo-European", by B. Barrois (2015), . |
Revision as of 12:32, 29 July 2016
"PIE" redirects here. For other uses, see PIE (disambiguation).Part of a series on |
Indo-European topics |
---|
Languages
|
Philology |
Origins
|
Archaeology
Pontic Steppe Caucasus East Asia Eastern Europe Northern Europe Pontic Steppe Northern/Eastern Steppe Europe
South Asia Steppe Europe Caucasus India |
Peoples and societies
Indo-Aryans Iranians East Asia Europe East Asia Europe Indo-Aryan Iranian |
Religion and mythology
Others
|
Indo-European studies
|
Template:PIE notice Proto-Indo-European (PIE) is the linguistic reconstruction of the common ancestor of the Indo-European languages. PIE was the first proposed proto-language to be widely accepted by linguists. Far more work has gone into reconstructing it than any other proto-language, and it is by far the best understood of all proto-languages of its age. During the 19th century, the vast majority of linguistic work was devoted to reconstruction of Proto-Indo-European or its daughter proto-languages such as Proto-Germanic, and most of the current techniques of linguistic reconstruction in historical linguistics (e.g. the comparative method and the method of internal reconstruction) were developed as a result. These methods supply all of the knowledge concerning PIE, since there is no written record of the language.
Scholars estimate that PIE may have been spoken as a single language (before divergence began) around 3500 BCE, though estimates by different authorities can vary by more than a millennium. A number of hypotheses have been proposed for the origin and spread of the language, the most popular among linguists being the Kurgan hypothesis, which postulates an origin in the Pontic–Caspian steppe of Eastern Europe. Features of the culture of the speakers of PIE, known as Proto-Indo-Europeans, have also been reconstructed based on the shared Indo-European vocabulary of the early Indo-European attested languages.
PIE is thought to have had a complex system of morphology that included inflectional suffixes as well as ablaut (vowel alterations, as preserved in English sing, sang, sung). Nouns and verbs had complex systems of declension and conjugation respectively.
In the descriptions below, an asterisk is used to mark reconstructed PIE words, such as *wódr̥ 'water', *ḱwṓ 'dog' (English hound), or *tréyes 'three (masculine)'.
Development of the theory
Method
There is no direct evidence of PIE, and no evidence suggesting it was ever written. Historical linguistics and Indo-European sound laws have been used to reconstruct all PIE sounds and words from later Indo-European languages using the comparative method and internal reconstruction. Many of the words in the modern Indo-European languages seem to have derived from such "protowords" via regular sound changes (e.g., c).
As the Proto-Indo-European language broke up, its sound system diverged, according to various sound changes in the daughter languages. Notable among these are Grimm's law and Verner's law in Proto-Germanic, loss of prevocalic *p- in Proto-Celtic, reduction to h of prevocalic *s- in Proto-Greek, Brugmann's law and Bartholomae's law in Proto-Indo-Iranian, Grassmann's law independently in both Proto-Greek and Proto-Indo-Iranian, and Winter's law and Hirt's law in Balto-Slavic.
History
The idea of a common root language was first postulated in the 1700s century when Sir William Jones, observed similarities between Sanskrit, Ancient Greek, and Latin. In The Sanscrit Language (1786) he suggested that all three languages had a common root, and that indeed they might further all be related, in turn, to Gothic and to the Celtic languages, as well as to Persian. This common source came to be known as Proto-Indo-European.
The classical phase of Indo-European comparative linguistics leads from Franz Bopp's Comparative Grammar (1833) to August Schleicher's 1861 Compendium and up to Karl Brugmann's Grundriß der vergleichenden Grammatik der indogermanischen Sprachen, which was published in the 1880s. Brugmann's Neogrammarian re-evaluation of the field and Ferdinand de Saussure's development of the laryngeal theory may be considered the beginning of "contemporary" Indo-European studies.
By the early 20th century, well-defined descriptions of PIE had been developed that are still accepted today, with some refinements. The largest developments of the 20th century were the discovery of the Anatolian and Tocharian languages and the acceptance of the laryngeal theory. This theory, in its early forms discussed since the 1880s, became mainstream after Jerzy Kuryłowicz's 1927 discovery of the survival of at least some of these hypothetical phonemes in Anatolian. The Anatolian languages have also spurred a major re-evaluation of theories concerning the development of various shared Indo-European language features and the extent to which these features were present in PIE itself. Relationships to other language families, including the Uralic languages, have been proposed but remain controversial.
Julius Pokorny's magisterial Indogermanisches etymologisches Wörterbuch ("Indo-European Etymological Dictionary", 1959) gave a detailed overview of the lexical knowledge accumulated up until that time, but neglected contemporary trends of morphology and phonology (including the laryngeal theory), and largely ignored Anatolian. The generation of Indo-Europeanists active in the last third of the 20th century (such as Calvert Watkins, Jochem Schindler and Helmut Rix) developed a better understanding of morphology and, in the wake of Kuryłowicz's 1956 Apophonie, understanding of the ablaut. From the 1960s, knowledge of Anatolian became certain enough to establish its relationship to PIE; see also Indo-Hittite.
Historical and geographical setting
There are several competing hypotheses about when, where, and by whom PIE was spoken. In the most popular model, the Kurgan hypothesis, Kurgans from the Pontic–Caspian steppe north of the Black Sea were the original speakers of PIE. This theory was first put forward by Marija Gimbutas and is accepted by most linguists. According to the theory PIE became widespread because its speakers, the Kurgans, were able to migrate into a large area of Europe and Asia because of certain cultural advances. These included the domestication of the horse, herding, and the use of wheeled vehicles.
The people of these cultures were nomadic pastoralists, who, according to the model, by the early 3rd millennium BC had expanded throughout the Pontic-Caspian steppe and into Eastern Europe.
Alternative theories include the Anatolian hypothesis, the Armenia hypothesis, and the indigenous Aryans theory.
Mainstream linguistic estimates of the time between PIE and the earliest attested texts (c. nineteenth century BC; see Kültepe texts) range around 1,500 to 2,500 years, with extreme proposals diverging up to another 100% on either side. Historically, some proposed models postulate the major dispersion of branches in:
- the 4th millennium BC (excluding the Anatolian branch) in Armenia, according to the Armenian hypothesis (proposed in the context of Glottalic theory);
- the 5th or 4th millennium in the Pontic–Caspian steppe, according to Marija Gimbutas's Kurgan hypothesis;
- the 6th millennium BC or later in Northern Europe according to Lothar Kilian's and, especially, Marek Zvelebil's models of a broader homeland;
- the 6th millennium BC in India, according to Koenraad Elst's Indigenous Aryans theory;
- the 7th millennium BC in Anatolia (the 5th, in the Balkans, excluding the Anatolian branch), according to Colin Renfrew's Anatolian hypothesis;
- the 7th millennium BC in Anatolia (the 6th excluding the Anatolian branch), according to glottochronological studies;
- Upper Paleolithic (before the 10th millennium BC in the Paleolithic Continuity Theory.
Renfrew's archaeological hypothesis assumes the Proto-Indo-Europeans brought agriculture to Europe long before the domestication of the horse, and is not accepted by most linguists. The Out-of-India and Northern-European hypotheses are fringe theories past their vogue.
Phonology
Proto-Indo-European phonology has been reconstructed. The Proto-Indo-European accent is reconstructed today as having had variable lexical stress, which could appear on any syllable and whose position often varied among different members of a paradigm (e.g. between singular and plural of a verbal paradigm). Stressed syllables received a higher pitch, therefore it is often said that PIE had pitch accent. The location of the stress is associated with ablaut variations, especially between normal-grade vowels (*/e/ and */o/) and zero-grade (i.e. lack of a vowel), but not entirely predictable from it.
The accent is best preserved in Vedic Sanskrit and (in the case of nouns) Ancient Greek, and indirectly attested in a number of phenomena in other IE languages. To account for mismatches between the accent of Vedic Sanskrit and Ancient Greek, as well as a few other phenomena, a few historical linguists prefer to reconstruct PIE as a tone language where each morpheme had an inherent tone; the sequence of tones in a word then evolved, according to that hypothesis, into the placement of lexical stress in different ways in different IE branches.
Morphology
Root
Proto-Indo-European roots are basic morphemes carrying a lexical meaning. PIE was a fusional language, in which the grammatical relationships between words were signaled through inflectional morphemes (usually endings). By addition of suffixes they form word stems, and by addition of desinences (usually endings), these form inflected nouns or verbs.
Ablaut
The Indo-European ablaut is the variation in vowels which occurred both within inflectional morphology (different grammatical forms of a noun or verb) and derivational morphology between, for example, a verb and an associated verbal noun. Originally, all categories were distinguished both by ablaut and different endings, but the loss of endings in some later Indo-European languages has led them to use ablaut alone to distinguish grammatical categories, as in the Modern English words sing, sang, sung.
Noun
Proto-Indo-European nouns are declined for eight or nine cases:
- nominative: marks the subject of a verb or the predicate noun or predicate adjective, as opposed to its object or other verb arguments. Generally, the noun that is doing something is in the nominative, and the nominative is the dictionary form of the noun.
- accusative: for the direct object of a transitive verb.
- genitive: marks a noun as modifying another noun.
- dative: used to indicate the noun to which something is given, as in "Maria gave Jacob a drink". Here, Jacob is an indirect dative.
- instrumental: used to indicate that a noun is the instrument or means by or with which the subject achieves or accomplishes an action. The noun may be either a physical object or an abstract concept.
- ablative: used to express motion away from something.
- locative: corresponds vaguely to the English prepositions "in", "on", "at", and "by".
- vocative: used for a noun that identifies a person (animal, object, etc.) being addressed or, occasionally, the determiners of that noun. A vocative expression is an expression of direct address where the identity of the party spoken to is set forth expressly within a sentence. For example, in the sentence, "I don't know, John", John is a vocative expression that indicates the party being addressed.
- allative: a type of the locative case.
There were three grammatical genders:
- masculine
- feminine
- neuter
Pronoun
Proto-Indo-European pronouns are difficult to reconstruct owing to their variety in later languages. PIE had personal pronouns in the first and second grammatical person, but not the third person, where demonstrative pronouns were used instead. The personal pronouns had their own unique forms and endings, and some had two distinct stems; this is most obvious in the first person singular, where the two stems are still preserved in English I and me. There were also two varieties for the accusative, genitive and dative cases, a stressed and an enclitic form.
Personal pronouns | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
First person | Second person | |||
Singular | Plural | Singular | Plural | |
Nominative | *h₁eǵ(oH/Hom) | *wei | *tuH | *yuH |
Accusative | *h₁mé, *h₁me | *nsmé, *nōs | *twé | *usmé, *wōs |
Genitive | *h₁méne, *h₁moi | *ns(er)o-, *nos | *tewe, *toi | *yus(er)o-, *wos |
Dative | *h₁méǵʰio, *h₁moi | *nsmei, *ns | *tébʰio, *toi | *usmei |
Instrumental | *h₁moí | *nsmoí | *toí | *usmoí |
Ablative | *h₁med | *nsmed | *tued | *usmed |
Locative | *h₁moí | *nsmi | *toí | *usmi |
Verb
Proto-Indo-European verbs were complex and, like the noun, exhibits a system of ablaut. The most basic categorization for the Indo-European verb was grammatical aspect. Verbs were classed as:
- stative: verbs that depict a state of being
- imperfective: verbs depicting ongoing, habitual or repeated action
- perfective: verbs depicting a completed action or actions viewed as an entire process).
Verbs have at least four grammatical moods:
- indicative: indicates that something is a statement of fact; in other words, to express what the speaker considers to be a known state of affairs, as in declarative sentences.
- imperative: forms commands or requests, including the giving of prohibition or permission, or any other kind of advice or exhortation.
- subjunctive: used to express various states of unreality such as wish, emotion, possibility, judgment, opinion, obligation, or action that has not yet occurred
- optative: indicates a wish or hope. It is similar to the cohortative mood, and is closely related to the subjunctive mood.
Verbs had two grammatical voices:
- mediopassive: for the middle voice and the passive voice.
Verbs had three grammatical persons: (first, second and third)
Verbs had three grammatical numbers
- singular
- dual: referring to precisely two of the entities (objects or persons) identified by the noun or pronoun.
- plural: a number other than singular or dual.
Verbs were also marked by a highly developed system of participles, one for each combination of tense and voice, and an assorted array of verbal nouns and adjectival formations.
The following table shows two possible reconstructions of the PIE verb endings. Sihler's reconstruction largely represents the current consensus among Indo-Europeanists, while Beekes' is a radical rethinking of thematic verbs; although not widely accepted, it is included to show an example of more far-reaching recent research.
Sihler (1995) | Beekes (1995) | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Athematic | Thematic | Athematic | Thematic | ||
Singular | 1st | *-mi | *-oh₂ | *-mi | *-oH |
2nd | *-si | *-esi | *-si | *-eh₁i | |
3rd | *-ti | *-eti/-ei | *-ti | *-e | |
Dual | 1st | *-wos | *-owos | *-ues | *-oues |
2nd | *-th₁es | *-eth₁es | *-tHes/-tHos | *-etHes/-etHos | |
3rd | *-tes | *-etes | *-tes | *-etes | |
Plural | 1st | *-mos | *-omos | *-mes | *-omom |
2nd | *-te | *-ete | *-th₁e | *-eth₁e | |
3rd | *-nti | *-onti | *-nti | *-o |
Numbers
Proto-Indo-European numerals are generally reconstructed as follows:
Sihler | Beekes | |
---|---|---|
one | *Hoi-no-/*Hoi-wo-/*Hoi-k(ʷ)o-; *sem- | *Hoi(H)nos |
two | *d(u)wo- | *duoh₁ |
three | *trei- (full grade) / *tri- (zero grade) | *treies |
four | *kʷetwor- (o-grade) / *kʷetur- (zero grade) (see also the kʷetwóres rule) |
*kʷetuōr |
five | *penkʷe | *penkʷe |
six | *s(w)eḱs; originally perhaps *weḱs | *(s)uéks |
seven | *septm̥ | *séptm |
eight | *oḱtō, *oḱtou or *h₃eḱtō, *h₃eḱtou | *h₃eḱteh₃ |
nine | *(h₁)newn̥ | *(h₁)néun |
ten | *deḱm̥(t) | *déḱmt |
Rather than specifically 100, *ḱm̥tóm may originally have meant "a large number".
Particle
Proto-Indo-European particles could be used both as adverbs and postpositions, like *upo "under, below". The postpositions became prepositions in most daughter languages. Other reconstructible particles include negators (*ne, *mē), conjunctions (*kʷe "and", *wē "or" and others) and an interjection (*wai!, an expression of woe or agony).
Syntax
The syntax of the older Indo-European languages has been studied in earnest since at least the late nineteenth century, by such scholars as Hermann Hirt and Berthold Delbrück. In the second half of the twentieth century, interest in the topic increased and led to reconstructions of Proto-Indo-European syntax.
Since all the early attested IE languages were inflectional, PIE is thought to have relied largely on morphological markers, rather than word order, to signal syntactic relationships within sentences. Still, a default (unmarked) word order is thought to have existed in PIE. This was reconstructed by Jacob Wackernagel as being subject–verb–object (SVO), based on evidence in Vedic Sanskrit, and the SVO hypothesis still has some adherents, but as of 2015 the "broad consensus" among PIE scholars is that PIE would have been a subject–object–verb (SOV) language.
The SOV default word order with other orders used to express emphasis (e.g., verb–subject–object to emphasize the verb) is attested in Old Indic, Old Iranian, Old Latin and Hittite, while traces of it can be found in the enclitic personal pronouns of the Tocharian languages. A shift from OV to VO order is posited to have occurred in late PIE, since many of the descendant languages have this order: modern Greek, Romance and Albanian prefer SVO, Insular Celtic has VSO as the default order, and even the Anatolian languages show some signs of this word order shift. The inconsistent order preference in Baltic, Slavic and Germanic can be attributed to contact with outside OV languages.
Sample texts
Since PIE was spoken by a prehistoric society, no genuine sample texts are available, but since the 19th century, modern scholars have made various attempts to compose example texts for purposes of illustration. These texts are educated guesses at best; Calvert Watkins observed in 1969 that in spite of its 150 years' history, comparative linguistics is not in the position to reconstruct a single well-formed sentence in PIE. Because of this and other similar objections based on Pratishakhyas, such texts are of limited use in getting an impression of what a coherent utterance in PIE might have sounded like.
Published PIE sample texts:
- Schleicher's fable (Avis akvasas ka) by August Schleicher (1868), modernized by Hermann Hirt (1939) and Winfred P. Lehmann and Ladislav Zgusta (1979)
- The king and the god (rēḱs deiwos-kʷe) by S. K. Sen, E. P. Hamp et al. (1994)
- "Before Babel: Ancient tales from Genesis retold in reconstructed Proto-Indo-European", by B. Barrois (2015), <hal-01231610>.
Relationships to other language families
Proposed genetic connections
Many higher-level relationships between Proto-Indo-European and other language families have been proposed, but these hypothesized connections are highly controversial. A proposal often considered to be the most plausible of these is that of an Indo-Uralic family, encompassing PIE and Uralic. The evidence usually cited in favor of this consists in a number of striking morphological and lexical resemblances. Opponents attribute the lexical resemblances to borrowing from Indo-European into Uralic. Frederik Kortlandt, while advocating a connection, concedes that "the gap between Uralic and Indo-European is huge", while Lyle Campbell denies that such relationship exists. .
Other proposals, further back in time (and proportionately less accepted), link Indo-European and Uralic with Altaic and the other language families of northern Eurasia, namely Yukaghir, Korean, Japanese, Chukotko-Kamchatkan, Nivkh, Kartvelian, Ainu, and Eskimo–Aleut, but excluding Yeniseian (the most comprehensive such proposal is Joseph Greenberg's Eurasiatic), or link Indo-European, Uralic, and Altaic to Afroasiatic and Dravidian (the traditional form of the Nostratic hypothesis), and ultimately to a single Proto-Human family.
A more rarely mentioned proposal associates Indo-European with the Northwest Caucasian languages in a group called the "Pontic languages".
Etruscan shows some similarities to Indo-European, such as a genitive in -s. There is no consensus on whether these are due to a genetic relationship, borrowing, chance and sound symbolism, or some combination of these.
Proposed areal connections
The existence of certain PIE typological features in Northwest Caucasian languages may hint at an early Sprachbund or substratum that reached geographically to the PIE homelands. This same type of languages, featuring complex verbs of which the current Northwest Caucasian languages might have been the sole survivors, was cited by Peter Schrijver to indicate a local lexical and typological reminiscence in western Europe pointing to a possible Neolithic substratum.
Daughter language groupings
Generally accepted subfamilies (clades)
- Proto-Albanian language
- Proto-Anatolian language
- Proto-Armenian language
- Proto-Balto-Slavic language
- Proto-Celtic language
- Proto-Germanic language
- Proto-Greek language
- Proto-Indo-Iranian language
- Proto-Italic language
- Proto-Tocharian language
Marginally attested languages
These include languages that do not appear to be members of any of the above families but are so poorly attested that proper classification of them is not possible. Of them, Phrygian is by far the best attested.
- Phrygian language
- Dacian language
- Thracian language
- Illyrian language
- Venetic language
- Messapic language
- Ancient Macedonian language
- Lusitanian language
All of the above languages except for Lusitanian (which occurs in the area of modern Portugal) occur in or near the Balkan peninsula, and have been collectively termed the "Paleo-Balkan languages". This is a purely geographic grouping and makes no claims about the relatedness of the languages to each other as compared with other Indo-European languages.
Hypothetical clades
In popular culture
PIE is used in dialogue between humans and aliens in Ridley Scott's movie Prometheus. In one scene, an android studies Schleicher's fable.
Christopher Tin's song Water Prelude, from The Drop That Contained the Sea, is sung in PIE.
The words and much morphology and word order of the Atlantean language created by Dr. Marc Okrand for Disney's 2001 Atlantis: The Lost Empire is based on PIE.
Michael Z. Williamson's time-travel novel "A long time until now" has the American translator use PIE to create a dictionary to communicate with stone-age people.
On the videogame Far Cry Primal the Wenja, the Udam and the Izila tribes talk in their own dialects based on PIE.
See also
References
- "linguistics - The comparative method | science". Retrieved 2016-07-27.
- Mallory, J. P. (1991). In Search of the Indo-Europeans. Thames & Hudson. p. 185. ISBN 978-0500276167.
{{cite book}}
:|access-date=
requires|url=
(help) - Anthony, David W. (2007). The horse, the wheel, and language : how bronze-age riders from the Eurasian steppes shaped the modern world (8th reprint. ed.). Princeton, N.J.: Princeton University Press. ISBN 0-691-05887-3.
{{cite book}}
:|access-date=
requires|url=
(help) - Balter, Michael (13 February 2015). "Mysterious Indo-European homeland may have been in the steppes of Ukraine and Russia". © 2015 American Association for the Advancement of Science. Retrieved 2015-02-17.
- ^ Anthony, David W; Ringe, Done (2015). "The Indo-European Homeland from Linguistic and Archaeological Perspectives". The Annual Review of linguistics (1): 199–219.
- Gimbutas, Marija (1985). "Primary and Secondary Homeland of the Indo-Europeans: comments on Gamkrelidze-Ivanov articles". Journal of Indo-European Studies (Spring–summer).
- ^ Bouckaert, Remco; Lemey, P.; Dunn, M.; Greenhill, S. J.; Alekseyenko, A. V.; Drummond, A. J.; Gray, R. D.; Suchard, M. A.; et al. (24 August 2012), "Mapping the Origins and Expansion of the Indo-European Language Family", Science, 337 (6097): 957–960, doi:10.1126/science.1219669, PMC 4112997, PMID 22923579
- Gray, Russell D; Atkinson, Quentin D (27 November 2003), "Language-tree divergence times support the Anatolian theory of Indo-European origin" (PDF), Nature (426), NZ: Auckland: 435–39, doi:10.1038/nature02029, PMID 14647380
- Fortson, Benjamin (2004). Indo-European language and culture : an introduction. Malden (USA): Blackwell. p. 102. ISBN 1-4051-0316-7.
{{cite book}}
:|access-date=
requires|url=
(help) - ^ Beekes, Robert; Gabriner, Paul (1995). Comparative Indo-European linguistics : an introduction. Amsterdam: J. Benjamins Publishing Company. pp. 147, 212–217, 233, 243. ISBN 978-1556195044.
{{cite book}}
:|access-date=
requires|url=
(help) - ^ Sihler, Andrew L. (1995). New comparative grammar of Greek and Latin. New York u.a.: Oxford Univ. Press. ISBN 0-19-508345-8.
{{cite book}}
:|access-date=
requires|url=
(help) - Lehmann, Winfried P (1993), Theoretical Bases of Indo-European Linguistics, London: Routledge, pp. 252–55, ISBN 0-415-08201-3
- Kulikov, Leonid; Lavidas, Nikolaos, eds. (2015). "Preface". Proto-Indo-European Syntax and its Development. John Benjamins.
- ^ Mallory, J. P.; Adams, Douglas Q., eds. (1997). "Proto-Indo-European". Encyclopedia of Indo-European Culture. Taylor & Francis. p. 463.
- Hock, Hans Henrich (2015). "Proto-Indo-European verb-finality: Reconstruction, typology, validation". In Kulikov, Leonid; Lavidas, Nikolaos (eds.). Proto-Indo-European Syntax and its Development. John Benjamins.
- ^ Lehmann, Winfred P. (1974). Proto-Indo-European Syntax. University of Texas Press. p. 250.
- Kortlandt, Frederik (1993), General linguistics and Indo-European reconstruction (PDF), NL
{{citation}}
: CS1 maint: location missing publisher (link) - Kortlandt, Frederik (1989), The spread of the Indo-Europeans (PDF), NL
{{citation}}
: CS1 maint: location missing publisher (link) - Peter Schrijver (March 2007), Keltisch en de buren: 9000 jaar taalcontact (PDF) (in Dutch), NL: University of Utrecht
- Language Log » Proto-Indo-European in Prometheus?, Languagelog.ldc.upenn.edu, 2012-06-08, retrieved 2013-03-12
Further reading
Introductory works
- Mallory, JP; Adams, DQ (2006), The Oxford Introduction to Proto-Indo-European and the Proto-Indo-European World, Oxford: Oxford University Press, ISBN 9780199296682
- Meier-Brügger, Michael (2003), Indo-European Linguistics, New York: de Gruyter, ISBN 3-11-017433-2
- Szemerényi, Oswald (1996), Introduction to Indo-European Linguistics, Oxford
Major technical handbooks on Proto-Indo-European
- Pokorny, Julius (2005) , Indogermanisches etymologisches Wörterbuch (5 ed.), Francke, ISBN 3-7720-0947-6
- Rix, Helmut (2001), Lexikon der indogermanischen Verben (2 ed.), Dr. Ludwig Reichert Verlag, ISBN 3-89500-219-4
- Wodtko, Dagmar; et al. (2008), Nomina im Indogermanischen Lexikon, Heidelberg: Universitätsverlag Winter
- Dunkel, George E. (2014), Lexikon der indogermanischen Partikeln und Pronominalstämme, Heidelberg: Universitätsverlag Winter
Other major technical works on daughter languages
- Buck, Carl Darling (1933), Comparative Grammar of Greek and Latin, Chicago: University of Chicago Press, ISBN 0-226-07931-7
- Hoffner, Harry A., Jr.; Melchert, H. Craig (2008), A Grammar of the Hittite Language, 1: Reference Grammar, Eisenbrauns, ISBN 1-57506-119-8
{{citation}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - Thurneysen, Rudolf (1990) , A Grammar of Old Irish (Rev Enl Re ed.), Colton Book Imports, ISBN 1-85500-161-6
- Whitney, William Dwight (1889), Sanskrit Grammar, Harvard University Press, ISBN 0-486-43136-3 (India ISBN 81-208-0621-2)
Miscellaneous
- Ivanov, Vyacheslav V.; Gamkrelidze, Tamaz V. (March 1990), "The Early History of Indo-European Languages", Scientific American, 262 (N3): 110–116, doi:10.1038/scientificamerican0390-110
- Grigoriev, Stanislav (2002). "Ancient Indo-Europeans". Chelyabinsk. Rifei.
- Lehmann, Winfred P.; Zgusta, L. (1979), "Schleicher's tale after a century", in Brogyanyi, B. (ed.), Festschrift for Oswald Szemerényi on the Occasion of his 65th Birthday, Amsterdam, pp. 455–466
{{citation}}
: CS1 maint: location missing publisher (link) - Remys, Edmund (2007), "General distinguishing features of various Indo-European languages and their relationship to Lithuanian", Indogermanische Forschungen, vol. 112, Berlin, New York: Walter de Gruyter
- Renfrew, Colin (1987), Archaeology & Language. The Puzzle of the Indo-European Origins, London: Jonathan Cape, ISBN 0-224-02495-7
{{citation}}
: Invalid|ref=harv
(help)
External links
This article's use of external links may not follow Misplaced Pages's policies or guidelines. Please improve this article by removing excessive or inappropriate external links, and converting useful links where appropriate into footnote references. (July 2016) (Learn how and when to remove this message) |
- PIE theoretical grammar, U Texas.
- Proto-Indo-European Swadesh list of basic vocabulary words (from Wiktionary's Swadesh list appendix).
- Blažek, Václav, "On the internal classification of Indo-European languages" (PDF), Linguistica (survey) (online ed.), Brno, CZ, ISSN 1801-5336
{{citation}}
: External link in
(help).|journal=
- Ford, CF, Indo-European family tree, showing Indo-European languages and sub branches (JPEG) (image), IA State.
- An Overview of the Proto-Indo-European Verb System 2009‐10‐27, Piotr Gąsiorowski.
- Köbler, Gerhard, Indo-European/Modern German Dictionary (in German), DE; contains an Indo-European Grammar in Vorwort section.
- Kortlandt, Frederik, The Indo-Uralic Verb (PDF).
- Nikolaev, SL; Starostin, SA, PIE root etymology database, RU: RI net.
- Pokorny, Julius, "A list of PIE etyma and their meanings", Indo-European Etymological Dictionary, University of Texas.
- ———, Dictionary (database query), NL: Leiden University; includes comments and searchable cognates.
- Sampson, Geoffrey, Say something in Proto-Indo-European.
- Indo-European migrations from the Armenian Highlands (JPEG) (image), U Penn.
- Powell, Eric, Telling Tales in Proto-Indo-European (includes audio files), Archaeology
{{citation}}
: Italic or bold markup not allowed in:|publisher=
(help).
Proto-Indo-European language | |
---|---|
Phonology | |
Morphology | |
Parts of speech | |
Main sources | |
Artificial compositions | |
Theories | |
Society | |
See also |